Journal of Comparative Physiology A

, Volume 158, Issue 1, pp 103–109 | Cite as

Magnetic compass orientation in the Eastern red-spotted newt (Notophthalmus viridescens)

  • John B. Phillips
Article

Summary

Laboratory tests were carried out to examine the orientation behavior of adult Eastern red-spotted newts (Notophthalmus viridescens) to earth-strength magnetic fields. Groups of 30 to 40 newts were housed in water-filled, all-glass aquaria with an artificial shoreline at one end. The aquaria were located in a greenhouse or outdoors adjacent to the laboratory building, and aligned on either the magnetic north-south or east-west axis. Tests were carried out in an enclosed indoor arena. Newts were tested in four horizontal alignments of the magnetic field: the ambient magnetic field (magnetic north at North) and three altered fields (magnetic north rotated to East, South or West). Data were analyzed after pooling the magnetic bearings from all four conditions in such a way as to retain the component of the newts' orientation that was a consistent response to the magnetic field. Elevation of training tank water temperature was used to increase the newts' motivation to orient in the direction of shore. Newts exposed to a training tank water temperature of 33–34 °C just prior to testing exhibited consistent unimodal magnetic compass orientation. The direction of orientation was altered predictably by changing training tank alignment and location relative to the laboratory building. The results provide the first evidence of a strong, replicable magnetic compass response in a terrestrial vertebrate under controlled laboratory conditions. Further, the present study demonstrates that the Eastern newt is able to learn a directional response relative to the earth's magnetic field.

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. Able KP (1980) Mechanisms of orientation, navigation and homing. In: Gauthreaux SA (ed) Animal migration, orientation and navigation, pp 284–373Google Scholar
  2. Adler K (1976) Extraocular photoreception in amphibians. Photochem Photobiol 23:275–298Google Scholar
  3. Baker RR (1981) Human navigation and the sixth sense. Hodder & Stoughton, LondonGoogle Scholar
  4. Batschelet E (1981) Circular statistics in biology. Academic Press, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  5. Beason RC, Nichols JE (1984) Magnetic orientation and magnetically sensitive material in a transequatorial migratory bird. Nature 309:151–153Google Scholar
  6. Bingman VP (1981) Ontogeny of a multiple stimulus orientation system in the savannah sparrow (Passerculus sandwichensis). Ph. D. Thesis, State Univ. New York, p 116Google Scholar
  7. Bingman VP (1983) Magnetic field orientation of migratory savannah sparrows with different first summer experience. Behaviour 87:43–53Google Scholar
  8. Conant R (1975) A field guide to reptiles and amphibians of eastern and central North America. Houghton Mifflin, BostonGoogle Scholar
  9. Emlen ST, Wiltschko W, Demong NJ, Wiltschko R, Bergman S (1976) Magnetic direction finding: Evidence for its use in migratory Indigo Buntings. Science 193:505–508Google Scholar
  10. Ferguson DE (1971) The sensory basis of orientation in amphibians. Ann NY Acad Sci 188:30–36Google Scholar
  11. Gill DE (1978) Effective population size and interdemic migration rates in a metapopulation of the red-spotted newt,Notophthalmus viridescens (Rafinesque). Evolution 32:839–849Google Scholar
  12. Gill DE (1979) Density dependence and homing behavior in adult red-spotted newtsNotophthalmus viridescens (Rafinesque). Ecology 60:800–813Google Scholar
  13. Gould JL, Able KA (1981) Human homing: an elusive phenomenon. Science 212:1061–1065Google Scholar
  14. Griffin DR (1982) Ecology of migration: Is magnetic orientation a reality. Q Rev Biol 57:293–295Google Scholar
  15. Healey WR (1973) Life history variation and the growth of juvenileNotophthalmus viridescens from Massachusetts. Copeia 1973:641–647Google Scholar
  16. Healey WR (1974) Population consequences of alternative life histories inNotophthalmus viridescens. Copeia 1974:221–229Google Scholar
  17. Kreithen ML, Keeton WT (1974) Attempts to condition homing pigeons to magnetic stimuli. J Comp Physiol 91:355–362Google Scholar
  18. Lemkau PJ (1976) An attempt to condition the green turtle,Chelonia mydas, to magnetic fields. M.S. Thesis, Univ. Rhode Island, p 51Google Scholar
  19. Mather JG, Baker RR (1981) Magnetic sense of direction in woodmice for route-based navigation. Nature 291:152–155Google Scholar
  20. Ossenkopp K, Barbeito R (1978) Bird orientation and the geomagnetic field: A review. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2:255–270Google Scholar
  21. Phillips JB (1977) Use of the earth's magnetic field by orienting cave salamanders (Eurycea lucifuga). J Comp Physiol 121:273–288Google Scholar
  22. Phillips JB (1986) Homing orientation by the Eastern red-spotted newt (Notophthalmus viridescens). Behav Ecol Sociobiol (submitted for publication)Google Scholar
  23. Phillips JB, Waldvogel JA (1982) Reflected light cues generate the short-term deflector-loft effect. In: Papi F, Wallraff H (eds) Avian navigation, Springer, Berlin Heidelberg New York, pp 179–189Google Scholar
  24. Quinn TP (1980) Evidence for celestial and magnetic compass orientation in lake migrating sockeye salmon fry. J Comp Physiol 137:243–248Google Scholar
  25. Quinn TP, Groot C (1984) The effect of water flow rate on bimodal orientation of juvenile chum salmon,Oncorhychus keta. Anim Behav 32:628–629Google Scholar
  26. Quinn TP, Merrill RT, Brannon EL (1981) Magnetic field detection in sockeye salmon. J Exp Zool 217:137–142Google Scholar
  27. Rodda GH (1984) Orientation and navigation of juvenile alligators: Evidence for magnetic sensitivity. J Comp Physiol A 154:649–658Google Scholar
  28. Rubens SM (1945) Cube surface coil for producing uniform magnetic field. Rev Sci Instr 16:243–245Google Scholar
  29. Taylor DH, Auburn TS (1978) Orientation of amphibians by linearly polarized light. In: Schmidt-Koenig K Keeton WT (eds) Animal Migration, Navigation and Homing. Springer, Berlin Heidelberg New YorkGoogle Scholar
  30. Tesch FW (1974) Influence of geomagnetism and salinity on the directional choice of eels. Helgol Wiss Meeresunters 26:382–395Google Scholar
  31. Visalberghi E, Alleva E (1979) Magnetic influences on pigeon homing. Biol Bull 156:246–256Google Scholar
  32. Walcott C, Green RP (1974) Orientation of homing pigeons altered by a change in the direction of an applied magnetic field. Science 184:180–182Google Scholar
  33. Wiltschko W (1983) Compasses used by birds. Comp Biochem Physiol 76A:709–717Google Scholar
  34. Wiltschko W, Wiltschko R (1976) Interrelationship of magnetic compass and star orientation in night-migrating birds. J Comp Physiol 109:91–99Google Scholar
  35. Wiltschko W, Wiltschko R, Keeton WT, Madden R (1983) Growing up in an altered magnetic field affects the initial orientation of young homing pigeons. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 12:135–142Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer-Verlag 1986

Authors and Affiliations

  • John B. Phillips
    • 1
  1. 1.Section of Neurobiology & BehaviorCornell UniversityIthacaUSA
  2. 2.Department of BiologyYale UniversityNew HavenUSA

Personalised recommendations