Advertisement

Journal of comparative physiology

, Volume 147, Issue 4, pp 511–522 | Cite as

The active space of red-winged blackbird song

  • Eliot A. Brenowitz
Article

Summary

Active space, that distance from the source over which signal amplitude remains above the detection threshold of potential receivers, was determined for Red-winged Blackbird (Agelaius phoeniceus) song in an upland pasture near Ithaca, New York. Song amplitude, the rate of signal attenuation, the amplitude of ambient noise, and the sensitivity of redwings to song masked by noise determine active space and were measured in the field:
  1. 1.

    Maximum root-mean-square song amplitude at 1 m from a singing bird ranged from 88.5–93.5 dB SPL (\(\bar X\)± SE = 90.8±0.21 dB SPL).

     
  2. 2.

    Close to the source, song attenuates at a rate which closely matches that predicted by spherical spreading (6 dB/doubling of distance) alone. At distances beyond about 30 m, however, excess attenuation becomes important (Fig. 4). The rate of attenuation varies with relative wind direction (Table 1).

     
  3. 3.

    Ambient noise level is relatively low in early morning, rises in late morning and afternoon as air turbulence increases, and then drops again in the evening as turbulence decreases (Table 2 and Appendix). Measured on a 4 kHz octave filter scale (center frequency = 4 kHz, octave pass-band = 2.83– 5.66 kHz), noise during one day ranges from 15 dB SPL (at 06∶00 h) to 36 dB SPL (at 11∶00 h), more than a 10-fold variation in ambient sound pressure.

     
  4. 4.

    A 3 dB signal to noise ratio is sufficient for detection of song masked by noise in the field (Figs. 5 and 6). Also, redwings are able to detect differences in this ratio as small as 3 dB.

     
  5. 5.

    In the absence of wind, the maximum active space of redwing song is about 189 m for a signal SPL of 93.5 dB at 1 m. This distance varies with changes in ambient noise level during the day, and with relative wind direction.

     

Maximum measurements of active space correspond closely to the maximum distances across 2 individual upland redwing territories. This suggests that redwing song is adapted in its structure and amplitude at the source to communicate a male's presence throughout the extent of his neighbors' territories.

Keywords

Active Space Ambient Noise Late Morning Ambient Noise Level Filter Scale 
These keywords were added by machine and not by the authors. This process is experimental and the keywords may be updated as the learning algorithm improves.

Abbreviations

B & K

Brüel and Kjaer

RMS

root-mean-square

S/N

signal to noise ratio

SPL

sound pressure level (re: 20 μPa)

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. Bowman RI (1979) Adaptive morphology of song dialects in Darwin's finches. J Ornithol 120:353–389Google Scholar
  2. Brackenbury JH (1979) Power capabilities of the avian sound-producing system. J Exp Biol 78:163–166Google Scholar
  3. Brenowitz EA (1981a) The effect of stimulus presentation sequence on the response of Red-winged Blackbirds in playback studies. Auk 98:355–360Google Scholar
  4. Brenowitz EA (1981b) ‘Territorial song’ as a flocking signal in Red-winged Blackbirds. Anim Behav 29:641–642Google Scholar
  5. Brenowitz EA (1982) Long-range communication of species identity by song in the Red-winged Blackbird. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 10:29–38Google Scholar
  6. Cody ML, Brown JH (1969) Song asynchrony in neighboring bird species. Nature 222:778–780Google Scholar
  7. Dabelsteen TC (1981) The sound pressure level in the dawn song of the Blackbird (Turdus merula) and a method for adjusting the level in experimental song to the level in natural song. Z Tierpsychol 56:137–149Google Scholar
  8. Dooling RJ (1980) Behavior and psychophysics of hearing in birds. In: Popper AN, Fay RR (eds) Comparative studies of hearing in vertebrates. Springer, Berlin Heidelberg New York, pp 261–288Google Scholar
  9. Dooling RJ, Saunders JC (1975) Hearing in the parakeet (Melopsittacus undulatus): absolute thresholds, critical ratios, frequency difference limens, and vocalizations. J Comp Physiol Psychol 88:1–20Google Scholar
  10. Ehret G (1977) Comparative psychoacoustics: perspectives of peripheral sound analysis in mammals. Naturwissenschaften 64:461–470Google Scholar
  11. Hassall JR, Zaveri K (1979) Acoustic noise measurements, 4th ed. Brüel and Kjaer Instruments, Inc., Marlborough, MassachusettsGoogle Scholar
  12. Heinz RD, Sinnott JM, Sachs MB (1977) Auditory sensitivity of the Redwing Blackbird (Agelaius phoeniceus) and Brown-headed Cowbird (Molothrus ater). J Comp Physiol Psychol 91:1365–1376Google Scholar
  13. Henwood K, Fabrick A (1979) A quantitative analysis of the dawn chorus: temporal selection for communicatory optimization. Am Nat 114:260–274Google Scholar
  14. Howard RA Jr (1977) Habitat structure, polygyny, and the evolution of upland nesting in Red-winged Blackbirds. PhD dissertation, Cornell University, Ithaca, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  15. Ingard U (1953) A review of the influence of meteorological conditions on sound propagation. J Acoust Soc Am 25:405–411Google Scholar
  16. King AP, West MJ, Eastzer DH, Staddon JER (1981) An experimental investigation of the bioacoustics of cowbird song. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 9:211–217Google Scholar
  17. Marten K, Marier P (1977) Sound transmission and its significance for animal vocalization. I. Temperate habitats. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 2:271–290Google Scholar
  18. Marten K, Quine D, Marier P (1977) Sound transmission and its significance for animal vocalization. II. Tropical forest habitats. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 2:291–302Google Scholar
  19. Morton ES (1975) Ecological sources of selection on avian sounds. Am Nat 108:17–34Google Scholar
  20. Orians GH, Christman GM (1968) A comparative study of the behavior of Red-winged, Tricolored, and Yellow-headed Blackbirds. Univ Calif Publ Zool 84:1–85Google Scholar
  21. Peek FW (1972) An experimental study of the territorial function of vocal and visual display in the male Red-winged Blackbird (Agelaius phoeniceus). Anim Behav 20:112–118Google Scholar
  22. Richards DG, Wiley RH (1980) Reverberations and amplitude fluctuations in the propagation of sound in a forest: implications for animal communication. Am Nat 115:381–399Google Scholar
  23. Sachs MB, Sinnott JM, Heinz RD (1978) Behavioral and physiological studies of hearing in birds. Fed Proc 37:2329–2335Google Scholar
  24. Saunders JC, Denny RM, Bock GR (1978) Critical bands in the parakeet (Melopsittacus undulatus). J Comp Physiol 125:359–365Google Scholar
  25. Smith DG (1979) Male singing ability and territory integrity in Red-winged Blackbirds (Agelaius phoeniceus). Behaviour 68:193–206Google Scholar
  26. Waser PM, Waser MS (1977) Experimental studies of primate vocalization: specializations for long distance propagation. Z Tierpsychol 43:239–263Google Scholar
  27. Wasserman FE (1977) Intraspecific acoustical interference in the White-throated Sparrow (Zonotrichia albicollis). Anim Behav 25:949–952Google Scholar
  28. Wiley RH, Richards DG (1978) Physical constraints on acoustic communication in the atmosphere: implications for the evolution of animal vocalizations. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 3:69–94Google Scholar
  29. Yasukawa K (1981) Song repertoires in the Red-winged Blackbird (Agelaius phoeniceus): a test of the Beau Geste hypothesis. Anim Behav 29:114–125Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer-Verlag 1982

Authors and Affiliations

  • Eliot A. Brenowitz
    • 1
  1. 1.Section of Neurobiology and Behavior, Division of Biological SciencesCornell UniversityIthacaUSA

Personalised recommendations