Journal of comparative physiology

, Volume 153, Issue 1, pp 3–15 | Cite as

Coordination of lateral giant and non-giant systems in crayfish escape behavior

  • Heinrich Reichert
  • Jeffrey J. Wine
Article

Summary

  1. 1.

    The modal escape response of a wellrested crayfish to a tap on the abdomen consists of an initial lateral giant-mediated flexion, a movement-triggered reflex extension, and then a series of centrally patterned, non-giant mediated, extension-flexion cycles (Figs. 1 and 2).

     
  2. 2.

    We investigated three models to explain how the non-giant system is triggered (Fig. 3). Direct stimulation of the giant axons in unrestrained animals showed that neither their central activity nor the sensory consequences of the tailflip they produce are sufficient to trigger the non-giant system (Fig. 4).

     
  3. 3.

    In well-rested animals, weak taps often triggered the non-giant system alone, i.e., without activating the giant axons (Fig. 5). This may have occurred rarely in earlier studies since the non-giant system habituates more rapidly than the lateral giant system (Fig. 6).

     
  4. 4.

    Although firing the giant axons does not trigger the non-giant system, the giant axons do cause subthreshold excitation which can summate with sensory input to trigger the non-giant system (Fig. 7).

     
  5. 5.

    The combined results show that the giant and non-giant systems are usually activated in parallel. The orderly transition from giant to non-giant escape behavior is in large part the result of marked differences in the latencies of each system: the median latency for the lateral giant axon response is 6 ms compared to approximately 140 ms for the non-giant response (Fig. 8).

     
  6. 6.

    The long latency of the non-giant system may be inherent to the processing time required to set up the nervous system for an optimal response. Direct observations establish that the non-giant system uses at least one bit of information that the giant axon system ignores: it reacts to the laterality of the stimulus as well as to its anterior-posterior location (Fig. 9).

     

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. Aréchiga H, Huberman A (1980) Peptide modulation of neuronal activity in crustaceans. In: Baker JF, Smith TH (eds) The role of peptides in neural function. Dekker, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  2. Brown TG (1941) On the nature of the fundamental activity of the nervous centers. J Physiol (Lond) 48:18–46Google Scholar
  3. Bryan JS, Krasne FB (1977) Presynaptic inhibition: the mechanism of protection from habituation of the crayfish lateral giant escape response. J Physiol (Lond) 271:369–390Google Scholar
  4. Fentress JC (1976) Simpler networks and behavior. Sinauer, Sunderland (Massachusetts)Google Scholar
  5. Gallistel CR (1980) The organization of action: a new synthesis. Erlbaum, Hillsdale (New Jersey)Google Scholar
  6. Holst E von (1937) Vom Wesen der Ordnung im Zentralnervensystem. Naturwissenschaften 25:625–631, 641–647. (Translated by R. Martin and reprinted in Holst E von (1973) The behavioral physiology of animals and man, vol 1. University of Miami Press, Coral Gables (Florida), pp 3–32Google Scholar
  7. Hoyle G (1977) Identified neurons and behavior of arthropods. Plenum Press, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  8. Kandel ER (1976) The cellular basis of behavior. Freeman, San FranciscoGoogle Scholar
  9. Kennedy D, Calabrese RL, Wine JJ (1974) Presynaptic inhibition: primary afferent depolarization in crayfish neurons. Science 186:451–454Google Scholar
  10. Kramer AP (1978) Some non-giant circuits mediating fast tail flexions in crayfish. PhD dissertation, University of California, Los AngelesGoogle Scholar
  11. Krasne FB, Bryan JS (1973) Habituation: regulation through presynaptic inhibition. Science 182:590–592Google Scholar
  12. Krasne FB, Wine JJ (1975) Extrinsic modulation of crayfish escape behaviour. J Exp Biol 63:433–450Google Scholar
  13. Kupfermann I, Weiss KR (1978) The command neuron concept. Behav Brain Sci 1:3–39Google Scholar
  14. Kuwada JY, Wine JJ (1979) Crayfish escape behavior: commands for fast movement inhibit postural tone and reflexes, and prevent habituation of slow reflexes. J Exp Biol 79:205–224Google Scholar
  15. Kuwada JY, Hagiwara G, Wine JJ (1980) Postsynaptic inhibition of crayfish tonic flexor motor neurones by escape commands. J Exp Biol 85:343–347Google Scholar
  16. Larimer JL, Eggleston AC, Masukawa LM, Kennedy D (1971) The different connections and motor outputs of lateral and medial giant fibres in the crayfish. J Exp Biol 54:391–402Google Scholar
  17. Lashley KS (1951) The problem of serial order in behavior. In: Jeffress LA (ed) Cerebral mechanisms in behavior: the Hixon symposium. Wiley, New York, pp 112–146Google Scholar
  18. Mittenthal JE, Wine JJ (1973) Connectivity patterns of crayfish giant interneurons: visualization of synaptic regions with cobalt dye. Science 179:182–184Google Scholar
  19. Mittenthal JE, Wine JJ (1978) Segmental homology and variation in flexor motoneurons of the crayfish abdomen. J Comp Neurol 177:311–334Google Scholar
  20. Reichert H, Wine JJ (1982) Neural mechanisms for serial order in a stereotyped behavior sequence. Nature 296:86–87Google Scholar
  21. Reichert H, Wine JJ, Hagiwara G (1981) Crayfish escape behavior: neurobehavioral analysis of phasic extension reveals dual systems for motor control. J Comp Physiol 142:281–294Google Scholar
  22. Roberts A (1968) Recurrent inhibition in the giant-fibre system of the crayfish and its effect on the excitability of the escape response. J Exp Biol 48:545–567Google Scholar
  23. Schaeffer AA (1928) Spiral movement in man. J Morphol Physiol 45:293–398Google Scholar
  24. Schrameck JE (1970) Crayfish swimming: alternating motor output and giant fiber activity. Science 169:698–700Google Scholar
  25. Tinbergen N (1948) Social releasers and the experimental method required for their study. Wilson Bull 60:6–52Google Scholar
  26. Tinbergen N (1951) The study of instinct. Clarendon, OxfordGoogle Scholar
  27. Wiersma CAG (1947) Giant nerve fiber system of the crayfish. A contribution to comparative physiology of synapse. J Neurophysiol 10:23–38Google Scholar
  28. Wiersma CAG (1961) Reflexes and the central nervous system. In: Waterman TH (ed) Physiology of Crustacea, vol II. Academic Press, New York, pp 241–279Google Scholar
  29. Wine JJ (1977) Crayfish escape behavior. II. Command-derived inhibition of abdominal extension. J Comp Physiol 121:173–186Google Scholar
  30. Wine JJ, Hagiwara G (1977) Crayfish escape behavior. I. The structure of efferent and afferent neurons involved in abdominal extension. J Comp Physiol 121:145–172Google Scholar
  31. Wine JJ, Hagiwara G (1978) Durations of unitary synaptic potentials help time a behavioral sequence. Science 199:557–559Google Scholar
  32. Wine JJ, Krasne FB (1972) The organization of escape behavior in the crayfish. J Exp Biol 56:1–18Google Scholar
  33. Wine JJ, Krasne FB (1982) The cellular organization of crayfish escape behavior. In: Bliss DE (ed) The biology of Crustacea, vol 4. Academic Press, New York, pp 241–292Google Scholar
  34. Wine JJ, Mistick DC (1977) Temporal organization of crayfish escape behavior: delayed recruitment of peripheral inhibition. J Neurophysiol 40:904–925Google Scholar
  35. Zucker RS (1972) Crayfish escape behavior and central synapses. I. Neural circuit exciting lateral giant fiber. J Neurophysiol 35:599–620Google Scholar
  36. Zucker RS, Kennedy D, Selverston AI (1971) Neuronal circuit mediating escape responses in crayfish. Science 173:645–650Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer-Verlag 1983

Authors and Affiliations

  • Heinrich Reichert
    • 1
  • Jeffrey J. Wine
    • 1
  1. 1.Department of PsychologyStanford UniversityStanfordUSA

Personalised recommendations