Journal of comparative physiology

, Volume 147, Issue 2, pp 179–187 | Cite as

Effects of background structure on the discrimination of configurational moving prey dummies by toadsBufo bufo (L.)

  • J. -P. Ewert
  • H. Burghagen
  • L. Albrecht
  • J. Kepper
Article

Summary

The prey-catching activity of the toadBufo b. bufo was measured in response to various configurational moving two-dimensional black or white stimuli contrasting with various homogeneous or structured backgrounds (Figs. 1 to 5).Area discrimination was tested with squares of different sizes,configurational discrimination with stripes of equal size and different configuration (‘worm’ or ‘antiworm’), andlength discrimination with worm-like stripes of different length.

Signal Extraction. A 2×5 mm2 wormlike stripe of a luminanceLs≈36 cd×m−2, for example, was practically not resolved from a homogeneous background of almost the same luminanceLb = 36. But it could be well detected if the background was structured (\(\bar L_B = 36\)); at a given luminance (\(\bar L_B \approx 36\)) the releasing value of the grey stimulus (Ls= 36) increased to some extent with structure width of the background (Fig. 1 c-e).

Configurational Discrimination. The basic stimulus response relationships were maintained, if Configurational stimuli were moved against black, white, grey or structured backgrounds. However, in the range of short stripes (xl= 5 mm) the worm/antiworm discrimination was significantly better withblack stimuli moving against white background than forwhite against black (curvesa andc in Figs. 2A and 3A). The Configurational selectivity in response to white wormlike and antiwormlike stripes increased if stimuli were moved against a grey or a structured background (curvesa andc in Figs. 2 and 3B-E forxl≧5 mm).

Length Discrimination. Relatively short stripes (close to the square configuration) and relatively long ones were discriminated less well than those in a medium size range. The optimal discrimination range was different for black stimuli (range: 3≦xl1≦6.5 mm) and white stimuli (range: 5≦xl1≦ 10 mm) (Fig. 4A, B). Discrimination in this range could be enhanced to some extent if the background was structured. These results were consistent for artificially (Fig. 4A) and naturally structured backgrounds (Fig. 5A-C).

The results show that stimulus discrimination in toads depends largely on the values of Configurational parameters. The background structure may have distinct effects on stimulus perception, i.e., either masking or facilitation.

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. Autrum H (1959) Das Fehlen unwillkürlicher Augenbewegungen beim Frosch. Naturwissenschaften 46:435Google Scholar
  2. Burghagen H, Ewert J-P (1982) Influence of background structure on perception of non-moving prey dummies by toadsBufo bufo (L.) (submitted)Google Scholar
  3. Cromarty AS, Schürg-Pfeiffer E, Ewert J-P (1982) Influence of Kainic-acid injections into the TP-region on response properties of neurons in the optic tectum inRana temporaria L. (in preparation)Google Scholar
  4. Ewert J-P (1976) The visual system of the toad. Behavioral and physiological studies on a pattern recognition system. In: Fite KV (ed) The amphibian visual system. A multidisciplinary approach. Academic Press, New York San Francisco LondonGoogle Scholar
  5. Ewert J-P (1983) Tectal mechanisms underlying prey-catching and avoidance behaviors in toads. In: Vanegas H (ed) Comparative neurology of the optic tectum. Plenum Press, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  6. Ewert J-P, Seelen W von (1974) Neurobiologie und System-Theorie eines visuellen Mustererkenuungsmechanismus bei Kröten. Kybernetik 14:167–183Google Scholar
  7. Grüsser O-J, Grüsser-Cornehls U (1976) Neurophysiology of the anuran visual system. In: Llinás R, Precht W (eds) Frog neurobiology. Springer, Berlin Heidelberg New YorkGoogle Scholar
  8. Hoffmann KP, Seelen W von (1978) Analysis of neuronal networks in the visual system of the cat using statistical signals; simple and complex cells: Part II. Biol Cybern 31:175–185Google Scholar
  9. Hubel DH, Wiesel TN (1962) Receptive fields, binocular interaction and functional architecture in the cat's visual cortex. J Physiol (Lond) 160:106–154Google Scholar
  10. Hubel DH, Wiesel TN (1965) Receptive fields and functional architecture in two nonstriate visual areas (18 and 19) of the cat. J Neurophysiol 28:229–289Google Scholar
  11. Julesz B (1976) Experiments in the visual perception of texture. In: Held R, Richards W (eds) Recent progress in perception. Readings from Scientific American. Freeman, San FranciscoGoogle Scholar
  12. Kuffler SW (1953) Discharge patterns and functional organization of the mammalian retina. J Neurophysiol 16:37–68Google Scholar
  13. Schürg-Pfeiffer E, Ewert J-P (1981) Investigation of neurons involved in the analysis of Gestalt prey features in the frogRana temporaria. J Comp Physiol 141:139–152Google Scholar
  14. Seelen W von, Hoffmann KP (1976) Analysis of neuronal networks in the visual system of the cat using statistical signals: Part I. Biol Cybern 22:7–20Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer-Verlag 1982

Authors and Affiliations

  • J. -P. Ewert
    • 1
  • H. Burghagen
    • 1
  • L. Albrecht
    • 1
  • J. Kepper
    • 1
  1. 1.Neuroethology and Biocybernetics LaboratoriesUniversity of KasselKasselFederal Republic of Germany

Personalised recommendations