Oecologia

, Volume 67, Issue 3, pp 411–415 | Cite as

Higher survival of an aposematic than of a cryptic form of a distasteful bug

  • Birgitta Sillén-Tullberg
Original Papers

Summary

An experiment was performed to assess the relative survival of two forms of 5th instar larvae of Lygaeus equestris (Heteroptera, Lygaeidae) — the normal red form, called aposematic, and a mutant grey form, called cryptic — when given to hand-raised great tits (Parus major).

Sixteen birds were presented with aposematic larvae and 16 were presented with cryptic larvae in 10 consecutive trials. One attack per trial was allowed. Both larval forms were presented against a background matching the grey larvae, but since both prey types were presented in a specific place known to the predator, detection rate for both was assumed to be unity.

Birds learned to avoid both prey types. However, the survival of the aposematic larvae was higher than that of the cryptic ones due to three aspects of predator behaviour: i) a greater initial reluctance to attack, ii) a more rapid avoidance learning, and iii) a lower frequency of killing in an attack, when the prey was aposematic. Moreover, a greater number of birds learned to avoid prey without killing any individual, when the prey was aposematic. This result is considered to be due to prey coloration alone, since, in a separate test, no difference in prey distastefulness could be detected.

This experiment shows that individual prey can benefit from being aposematic and indicates that individual selection can be a sufficient explanation for the evolution of aposematic coloration. It was concluded that, since the survivorship was 6.4 times higher for the aposematic prey, it could have a detection rate that is correspondingly higher than the cryptic in order for the two forms to have equal fitness.

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. Benson WW (1971) Evidence for the evolution of unpalatability through kin selection in the Heliconiinae (Lepidoptera). Amer Nat 105 (913):213–226CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Brower LP (1984) Chemical defence in butterflies. In: The Biology of Butterflies, RJ Vane-Wright, PR Ackery (eds). Academic Press, LondonGoogle Scholar
  3. Coppinger RP (1969) The effect of experience and novelty on avian feeding behavior with reference to the evolution of warning coloration in butterflies. I. Reactions of Wild-Caught adult Blue Jays to novel insects. Behaviour XXXV:45–60Google Scholar
  4. Coppinger RP (1970) The effect of experience and novelty on avian feeding behavior with reference to the evolution of warning coloration in butterflies. II. Reactions of naive birds to novel insects. Amer Nat 104:323–335CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Cott HB (1940) Adaptive coloration in animals. Methuen, LondonGoogle Scholar
  6. Curio E (1976) The ethology of predation. Springer, Berlin Heidelberg New York TokyoGoogle Scholar
  7. Fisher RA (1958) The genetical theory of natural selection, 2nd ed. Dover, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  8. Gittleman JL, Harvey PH (1980) Why are distasteful prey not cryptic. Nature 286:149–150Google Scholar
  9. Goodale MA, Sneddon J (1977) The effect of distastefulness of the model on the predation of artificial Batesian mimics. Anim Behav 25:660–665Google Scholar
  10. Harvey PH, Greenwood PJ (1978) Anti-predator defence strategies: some evolutionary problems. In: Behavioural ecology, an evolutionary approach, JR Krebs, NB Davies (eds) Blackwell, Oxford, pp 129–151Google Scholar
  11. Järvi T, Sillén-Tullberg B, Wiklund C (1981) The cost of being aposematic. An experimental study of predation on larvae of Papillo machaon by the great tit Parus major. Oikos 36:267–272Google Scholar
  12. Papageorgis C (1975) Mimicry in neotropical butterflies. Am Sci 63:522–532Google Scholar
  13. Remold H (1963) Scent-glands of land-bugs, their physiology and biological function. Nature 198:764–768Google Scholar
  14. Scudder GGE, Duffey SS (1972) Cardiac glycosides in the Lygaeinae (Hem.: Lygaeidae). Can J Zool 50:35–42Google Scholar
  15. Sillén-Tullberg B, Bryant EH (1983) The evolution of aposematic coloration in distasteful prey: an individual selection model. Evolution 37:993–1000Google Scholar
  16. Staddon BW (1979) The scent glands of Heteroptera. In: Advances in insect physiology, JE Treherne, MJ Berridge, VB Wigglesworth (eds) Academic Press, London New YorkGoogle Scholar
  17. Turner JRG (1971) Studies of Müllerian mimicry and its evolution in burnet moths and heliconid butterflies. In: Ecological genetics and evolution, R Creed (ed). Blackwell, Oxford, pp 224–260Google Scholar
  18. Windecker W (1939) Euchelia (Hypocrita) Jacobaea L. und das Schutztrachtenproblem. Z Morph Oekol Tiere 35:84–138Google Scholar
  19. Wiklund C, Järvi T (1982) Survival of distasteful insects after being attacked by naive birds: a reappraisal of the theory of aposematic coloration evolving through individual selection. Evolution 36:998–1002Google Scholar
  20. Wiklund C, Sillén-Tullberg B (1985) Why distasteful butterflies have aposematic larvae and adults but cryptic pupae: evidence from predation experiments on the monarch and the European swallowtail. Evolution (in press)Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer-Verlag 1985

Authors and Affiliations

  • Birgitta Sillén-Tullberg
    • 1
  1. 1.Department of ZoologyUniversity of Stockholm, BiologyStockholmSweden

Personalised recommendations