Advertisement

Psychological Research

, Volume 47, Issue 3, pp 125–133 | Cite as

The stereoscopic views of Wheatstone and Brewster

  • N. J. Wade
  • H. Ono
Article

Summary

The study of stereopsis, made possible by the invention of the stereoscope, freed binocular vision from the yoke of monocular phenomena. Wheatstone used this freedom to determine the factors involved in the perception of size and distance, and interpreted them within a cognitive framework. He devised an adjustable stereoscope which allowed him to apply the systematic experimental procedures of physics to the phenomena of depth perception. Brewster, by contrast, tried to force the newly discovered binocular phenomena back into the mould of monocular vision, using the lever of visible direction. His interpretations of visual phenomena, be they monocular or binocular, could be reduced to the two fundamental ‘laws’ of visible direction and distinct vision. While Wheatstone's cognitive approach influenced Helmholtz, and thereby modern cognitive theorists. Brewster's interpretations, based as they were in analyses of the retinal projection, find an echo in modern direct theorists.

Keywords

Cognitive Theorist Visible Direction Depth Perception Binocular Vision Cognitive Approach 
These keywords were added by machine and not by the authors. This process is experimental and the keywords may be updated as the learning algorithm improves.

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. Asher, H. (1953). Suppression theory of binocular vision. British Journal of Ophthalmology, 37, 37–49.Google Scholar
  2. Bell, C. (1823). On the motions of the eye, in illustration of the uses of the muscles and of the orbit. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society, 113(2), 166–186.Google Scholar
  3. Berkeley, G. (1709). An Essay towards a New Theory of Vision Dublin.Google Scholar
  4. Bishop, P.O., & Henry, G.H. (1972). Spatial vision. Annual Review of Psychology, 22, 119–160.Google Scholar
  5. Boring, E.G. (1942). Sensation and Perception in the History of Experimental Psychology. New York: Appleton Century.Google Scholar
  6. Brewster, D. (1844a). On the law of visible position in single and binocular vision, and on the representation of solid figures by the union of dissimilar plane pictures on the retina. Transactions of the Royal Society of Edinburgh, 15, 349–368.Google Scholar
  7. Brewster, D. (1844b). On the knowledge of distance given by binocular vision. Transactions of the Royal Society of Edinburgh, 15, 663–674.Google Scholar
  8. Brewster, D. (1849). Account of a new stereoscope. Report of the British Association. Transactions of the Sections, pp.6–7.Google Scholar
  9. Brewster, D. (1852). Binocular vision and the stereoscope. North British Review, 33, 165–204. (published anonymously).Google Scholar
  10. Brewster, D. (1856). The Stereoscope. Its History, Theory, and Construction. London: John Murray.Google Scholar
  11. Brücke, E. (1841). Über die stereoscopischen Erscheinungen und Wheatstone's Angriff auf die Lehre von den identischen Stellen der Netzhäute. Archiv für Anatomie. Physiologie und Wissenschaftliche Medicin, 8, 459–476.Google Scholar
  12. Charnwood, J.R.B. (1954). An answer to the suppression theory. British Orthoptic Journal, 11, 119–160.Google Scholar
  13. Dodwell, P.C., & Engel, G.R. (1963). The nature of binocular fusion. Nature, 198, 39–40.Google Scholar
  14. Dove, H. (1841). Die Combination der Eindrücke beider Ohren und beider Augen zu einem Eindruck. Monatsberichte der Berliner Akademie, 251–252.Google Scholar
  15. Fox, R., & Check, R. (1966). Binocular fusion: A test of the suppression theory. Perception & Psychophysics, 1, 331–334.Google Scholar
  16. Gibson, J.J. (1950). The Perception of the Visual World. Boston: Houghton-Mifflin.Google Scholar
  17. Gulick, W.H., & Lawson, R.B. (1976). Human Stereopsis. A Psychophysical Approach. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  18. Harris, J. (1775). A Treatise of Opticks. London: B. White.Google Scholar
  19. Held, R. (1976). Single vision with double images: an historic problem. In M. Henle (Ed.) Vision and Artifact, (pp.5–18). New York: Springer.Google Scholar
  20. Helmholtz, H. (1925). Physiological Optics (English translation by J.P.C. Southall.) New York: Optical Society of America. Volume 3. (Originally published in 1866.)Google Scholar
  21. Hochberg, J. (1964). Contralateral suppressive fields of binocular combination. Psychonomic Science, 1, 157–158.Google Scholar
  22. Howard, I.P. (1982). Human Visual Orientation. Chichester: Wiley.Google Scholar
  23. Hurvich, L.M., & Jameson, D. (1979). Helmholtz's vision: Looking backward. Contemporary Psychology, 24, 910–914.Google Scholar
  24. Julesz, B. (1971). Foundations of Cyclopean Perception. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
  25. Kaufman, L. (1963). On the spread of suppression and binocular rivalry. Vision Research, 3, 401–415.Google Scholar
  26. Le Conte, J. (1881). Sight: An Exposition of the Principles of Monocular and Binocular Vision. London: Kegan Paul.Google Scholar
  27. Michaels, C.F., & Carello, C. (1981). Direct Perception. Englewood Cliffs, N.J.: Prentice-Hall.Google Scholar
  28. Müller, J. (1826). Zur Vergleichenden Physiologie des Gesichtssinnes des Menschen und der Thiere, nebst einen Versuch über die Bewegungen der Augen und über den menschlichen Blick. Leipzig: Cnobloch.Google Scholar
  29. Ono, H. (1981). On Wells's (1792). Law of visual direction. Perception and Psychophysics, 30, 403–406.Google Scholar
  30. Ono, H., Angus, R., & Gregor, P. (1977). Binocular single vision achieved by fusion and suppression. Perception and Psychophysics, 21, 513–521.Google Scholar
  31. Ono, H., & Comerford, J. (1977). Stereoscopic depth constancy. In W. Epstein (Ed.) Stability and Constancy in Visual Perception (pp. 91–128) New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
  32. Ono, H., & Wade, N.J. (1985). Resolving discrepant results of the Wheatstone experiment. Psychological Research, 47 (this issue)Google Scholar
  33. Panum, P.L. (1858). Physiologische Untersuchungen über das Sehen mit zwei Augen. Kiel: Schwerssche Buchhandlung.Google Scholar
  34. Pastore, N. (1972). Sebastien Le Clerc on retinal disparity. Journal of the History of the Behavioral Sciences, 8, 336–339.Google Scholar
  35. Polyak, S. (1957). The Vertebrate Visual System. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
  36. Porta, J.B. (1593). De Refractione, Optices Parte. Naples: Carlinum and Pacem.Google Scholar
  37. Sperling, G. (1970). Binocular vision: A physical and neural model. American Journal of Psychology, 83, 461–534.Google Scholar
  38. du Tour, E.F. (1760). Discussion d'une question d'optique. Memoires de mathematique et de physique, 3, 514–530.Google Scholar
  39. Ullman, S. (1980). Against direct perception. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 3, 373–415.Google Scholar
  40. Wade, N.J. (1978). Sir Charles Bell on visual direction. Perception, 7, 359–362.Google Scholar
  41. Wade, N.J. (1983). Brewster and Wheatstone on Vision. London: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  42. Wells, W.C. (1792). An Essay on Single Vision with Two Eyes: Together with Experiments and Observations on several other Subjects in Optics. London: Cadell.Google Scholar
  43. Westheimer, G. (1983). Hermann Helmholtz and origins of sensory physiology. Trends in NeuroSciences, 6, 1–4.Google Scholar
  44. Wheatstone, C. (1838). Contributions to the physiology of vision — Part the first. On some remarkable, and hitherto unobserved, phenomena of binocular vision. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society, 128, 371–394.Google Scholar
  45. Wheatstone, C. (1852). Constributions to the physiology of vision — Part the second. On some remarkable, and hitherto unobserved, phenomena of binocular vision. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society, 142, 1–17.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer-Verlag 1985

Authors and Affiliations

  • N. J. Wade
    • 1
  • H. Ono
    • 2
  1. 1.University of DundeeDundeeScotland DD1 4HN, UK
  2. 2.York UniversityNorth YorkCanada

Personalised recommendations