Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology

, Volume 5, Issue 1, pp 1–15

Vocal regulation of use of space by groups of titi monkeys Callicebus moloch

  • John G. Robinson
Article

Summary

The daily movements and the resulting activity and calling fields are described for three groups of titi monkeys Callicebus moloch. Groups move to the periphery of their home range in the early morning and then call. Playback experiments confirmed that both proximity and location of calling by neighboring animals affect the group's response. Subsequent countercalling often regulates the approach of neighboring groups to a boundary. After encountering adjacent groups, animals return to areas near the home range center. Movements, calling behavior, and the response to neighboring groups all increase the probability of intergroup encounters in the early morning. Encounters define and reinforce the conventional location of boundaries. Boundaries decrease the probability of intergroup encounters at other times and allow exclusive use of space.

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. Altmann, S.A., Altmann, J.: Baboon ecology. Chicago: University of Chicago 1970Google Scholar
  2. Baldwin, J.D., Baldwin, J.I.: Vocalizations of howler monkeys (Alouatta palliata) in southwestern Panama. Folia Primatol. 26,81–108 (1976)Google Scholar
  3. Bates, B.C.: Territorial behavior in primates: A review of recent field studies. Primates 11, 271–284 (1970)Google Scholar
  4. Bliss, C.I.: An analysis of some insect trap records. In: Classical and contagious discrete distributions. Patil, G.P. (ed), pp. 385–397. Calcutta: Statistical Publishing Society 1965Google Scholar
  5. Brooks, R.J., Falls, J.B.: Individual recognition by song in white-throated sparrows. I. Discrimination of songs of neighbors and strangers. Can. J. Zool. 53, 879–888 (1975)Google Scholar
  6. Carpenter, C.R.: Naturalistic behavior of non-human primates. University Park: Pennsylvania State University 1964Google Scholar
  7. Charles-Dominique, P.: Aggression and territoriality in nocturnal prosimians. In: Primate aggression, territoriality and xenophobia. Holloway, R.L. (ed.), pp. 31–48. London, New York: Academic Press 1974Google Scholar
  8. Charles-Dominique, P.: Urine marking and territoriality in Galago alleni (Waterhouse, 1837-Lorisoidea, Primates)—A field study by radio telemetry. Z. Tierpsychol. 43, 113–138 (1977)Google Scholar
  9. Chivers, D.J.: On the daily behaviour and spacing of howling monkey groups. Folia Primatol. 10, 48–102 (1969)Google Scholar
  10. Chivers, D.J.: The siamang in Malaya: A field study of a primate in tropical rain forest. Contrib. Primatol. 4, 1–335 (1974)Google Scholar
  11. Clutton-Brock, T.H.: Ranging behaviour of red colobus (Colobus badius tephrosceles) in the Gombe National Park. Anim. Behav. 23, 706–722 (1975)Google Scholar
  12. Cohen, A.C.: Simplified estimators for the normal distribution when samples are singly censored or truncated. Technometrics 1, 217–237 (1959)Google Scholar
  13. Cohen, A.C.: Tables for maximum likelihood estimates: Singly truncated and singly censored samples. Technometrics 3, 535–541 (1961)Google Scholar
  14. DeVore, I., Hall, K.R.L.: Baboon ecology. In: Primate behavior. DeVore, I. (ed.), pp. 20–52. New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston 1965Google Scholar
  15. Ellefson, J.O.: Territorial behavior in the common white-handed gibbon, Hylobates lar Linn. In: Primates. Jay, P. (ed.), pp. 180–199. New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston 1968Google Scholar
  16. Ellefson, J.O.: A natural history of white-handed gibbons in the Malayan peninsula. Gibbon and Siamang 3, 1–136 (1974)Google Scholar
  17. Fossey, D.: Observations on home range of one group of mountain gorillas (Gorilla gorilla beringei). Anim. Behav. 22, 568–581 (1974)Google Scholar
  18. Freese, C.: A census of non-human primates. In: Primate censusing studies in Peru and Colombia, pp. 17–41. Washington, DC: PAHO, WHO 1975Google Scholar
  19. Gautier, J.P., Gautier-Hion, A.: Les association polyspecifiques chez les Cercopithecidae du Gabon. La Terre et la Vie 116, 164–201 (1969)Google Scholar
  20. Goldman, P.: Song recognition by field sparrows. Auk 90, 106–113 (1973)Google Scholar
  21. Hays, W.L.: Statistics for the social sciences, 2nd ed. New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston 1973Google Scholar
  22. Holmes, R.T., Pitelka, F.A.: Food overlap among co-existing sandpipers on northern Alaskan tundra. Syst. Zool. 17, 305–318 (1968)Google Scholar
  23. Ickes, R.A., Ficken, M.S.: An investigation of territorial behavior in the American redstart utilizing recorded songs. Wilson Bull. 82, 167–176 (1970)Google Scholar
  24. Jay, P.: The common langur of north India. In: Primate behavior. DeVore, I. (ed.), pp. 197–249. New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston 1965Google Scholar
  25. Kinzey, W.G., Rosenberger, A.L., Heisler, P.S., Prowse, D.L., Trilling, J.S.: A preliminary field investigation of the yellow-handed titi monkey, C. torquatus torquatus, in northern Peru. Primates 18, 159–181 (1977)Google Scholar
  26. Krebs, J.: Territory and breeding density in the great tit, Parus major L. Ecology 52, 2–22 (1971)Google Scholar
  27. Marler, P.: Territory and individual distance in the chaffinch Fringilla coelebs. Ibis 98, 496–501 (1956)Google Scholar
  28. Marler, P.: Aggregation and dispersal: Two functions in primate communication. In: Primates. Jay, P. (ed.), pp. 420–438. New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston 1968Google Scholar
  29. Marler, P.: Vocalizations of East African monkeys. II. Black and white colobus. Behaviour 42, 175–197 (1972)Google Scholar
  30. Mason, W.A.: Social organization of the South American monkey, Callicebus moloch: A preliminary report. Tulane Stud. Zool. 13, 23–28 (1966)Google Scholar
  31. Mason, W.A.: Use of space by Callicebus groups. In: Primates. Jay, P. (ed.), pp. 200–216. New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston 1968Google Scholar
  32. May, R.M.: Some notes on estimating the competition matrix α. Ecology 56, 737–741 (1975)Google Scholar
  33. Moynihan, M.: Communication in the titi monkey Callicebus. J. Zool. Lond. 150, 77–127 (1966)Google Scholar
  34. Neville, M.: Census of primates' in Peru. In: Primate censusing studies in Peru and Colombia, pp. 3–15. Washington, DC: PAHO, WHO 1975Google Scholar
  35. Owen-Smith, N.: On territoriality in ungulates and an evolutionary model. Q. Rev. Biol. 52, 1–38 (1977)Google Scholar
  36. Poirier, F.E.: Nilgiri langur (Presbytis johnii) territorial behavior. Primates 9, 351–364 (1968)Google Scholar
  37. Poirier, F.E.: Colobine aggression: A review. In: Primate aggression, territoriality and xenophobia. Holloway, R.L. (ed.). London, New York: Academic Press 1974Google Scholar
  38. Preston, F.W.: The commonness and rarity of species. Ecology 29, 254–283 (1948)Google Scholar
  39. Preston, F.W.: The canonical distribution of commonness and rarity. Ecology 43, 185–215, 410–432 (1962)Google Scholar
  40. Ripley, S.: Intertroop encounters among Ceylon gray langurs (Presbytis entellus). In: Social communication among primates. Altmann, S.A. (ed.), pp. 237–253. Chicago: University of Chicago 1967Google Scholar
  41. Robinson, J.G.: Vocal regulation of spacing in the titi monkey Callicebus moloch. Unpublished Ph. D. Dissertation. Chapel Hill: University of North Carolina 1977Google Scholar
  42. Robinson, J.G.: An analysis of the organization of vocal communication in the titi monkey Callicebus moloch. Z. Tierpsychol. (in press) (1979)Google Scholar
  43. Rudran, R.: Adult male replacement in one-male troops of purple-faced langurs (Presbytis senex senex) and its effect on population structure. Folia Primatol. 19, 166–192 (1973)Google Scholar
  44. Slocomb, J., Stauffer, B., Dickson, K.L.: On fitting the truncated lognormal distribution to species abundance data using maximum likelihood estimation. Ecology 58, 693–696 (1977)Google Scholar
  45. Struhsaker, T.T.: Behavior of vervet monkeys (Cercopithecus aethiops). Univ. Calif. Publ. Zool. 82, 1–64 (1967)Google Scholar
  46. Struhsaker, T.T.: Correlates of ecology and social organization among African ceropithecines. Folia Primatol. 11, 80–118 (1969)Google Scholar
  47. Struhsaker, T.T.: The red colobus monkey. Chicago: University of Chicago 1975Google Scholar
  48. Tenaza, R.R.: Songs, choruses and countersinging of Kloss' gibbons (Hylobates klossii) in Siberut Island, Indonesia. Z. Tierpsychol. 40, 37–52 (1976)Google Scholar
  49. Waser, P.M.: Experimental playbacks show vocal mediation of intergroup avoidance in a forest monkey. Nature 255, 56–58 (1975)Google Scholar
  50. Waser, P.M.: Cercocebus albigena: Site attachment, avoidance, and intergroup spacing. Am. Nat. 110, 911–935 (1976)Google Scholar
  51. Waser, P.M.: Individual recognition, intragroup cohesion and intergroup spacing: Evidence from sound playback to forest monkeys. Behaviour 60, 28–74 (1977)Google Scholar
  52. Waser, P.M., Waser, M.S.: Experimental studies of primate vocalization: Specializations for long-distance propagation. Z. Tierpsychol. 43, 239–263 (1977)Google Scholar
  53. Waser, P.M., Wiley, R.H.: Mechanisms and evolution of spacing behavior. In: Handbook of behavioral neurobiology: Social behavior and communication. Marler, P., Vandenbergh, J.G. (eds.). New York: Plenum (in press) 1979Google Scholar
  54. Weeden, J.S.: Territorial behavior of the tree sparrow. Condor 67, 193–209 (1965)Google Scholar
  55. Wiley, R.H.: Territoriality and non-randon mating in sage grouse, Centrocercus urophasianus. Anim. Behav. Monogr. 6, 87–169 (1973)Google Scholar
  56. Winkle, W. van: Comparison of several probabilistic home range models. J. Wldl. Manag. 39, 118–123 (1975)Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer-Verlag 1979

Authors and Affiliations

  • John G. Robinson
    • 1
  1. 1.Department of ZoologyUniversity of North CarolinaChapel HillUSA
  2. 2.Office of Zoological Research, National Zoological ParkSmithsonian InstitutionWashington, DCUSA

Personalised recommendations