Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology

, Volume 18, Issue 4, pp 297–301 | Cite as

Infrasonic calls of the Asian elephant (Elephas maximus)

  • Katharine B. Payne
  • William R. LangbauerJr.
  • Elizabeth M. Thomas
Article

Summary

Calls at frequencies below the range of human hearing were recorded from two groups of captive Asian elephants (Elephas maximus). Most of the calls ranged in frequency from 14 to 24 Hz, with durations of 10–15 s (Fig. 1). With the nearest elephant 5 m from the microphone, sound pressure levels were 85 to 90 dB (re 20 μPa). These calls occurred in a variety of circumstances. Elephants are the first terrestrial mammals reported to produce infrasound. These calls may be important in the coordination of behavior in thick vegetation or among separated groups of elephants.

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. Berg JK (1983) Vocalizations and associated behaviors of the African elephant (Loxodonta africana) in captivity. Z Tierpsychol 63:63–79Google Scholar
  2. Dneprovskaya IA, Iofe VK, Levitas FI (1963) On the attenuation of sound as it propagates through the atmosphere. Sov Phys Acoust 8:235–239Google Scholar
  3. Eisenberg JF (1980) Recent research on the biology of the Asiatic elephant (Elephas m. maximus) on Sri Lanka. Spolia Zeylan 35:213–218Google Scholar
  4. Eltringham SK (1982) Elephants. Blanford Press, Dorset, EnglandGoogle Scholar
  5. Eyring CF (1946) Jungle acoustics. J Acoust Soc Am 18:257–270Google Scholar
  6. Gould E, Negus A, Novick A (1964) Evidence for echolocating in shrews. J Exp Zool 156:19–38Google Scholar
  7. Heffner R, Heffner H (1980) Hearing in the elephant (Elephas maximus). Science 208:518–520Google Scholar
  8. Hirsh IJ (1952) The measurement of hearing. McGraw-Hill, New York, p 107Google Scholar
  9. Jainundeen MR, McKay GM, Eisenberg JF (1972) Observations on musth in the domesticated Asiatic elephant (Elephas maximus). Mammal 36:247–261Google Scholar
  10. Kurt F (1974) Remarks on the social structure and ecology of the Ceylon elephant in the Yala National Park. In: Geist V, Walther F (eds) The behavior of ungulates and its relation to management. IUCN Publications, New Series 24: 618–634Google Scholar
  11. McKay GM (1973) Bchavior and ecology of the Asiatic elephant in Southeastern Ceylon. Smithso Contrib Zool 125:67–69Google Scholar
  12. Moss R, Lockic I (1979) Infrasonic components in the song of the Capercaillie Tetrao urogallus. Ibis 121:95–97Google Scholar
  13. Norris KS, Prescott JH, Asa-Dorian PV, Perkins P (1961) An experimental demonstration of echolocation behavior in the porpoise (Tursiops truncatus). Biol Bull 120:454–455Google Scholar
  14. Payne RS, Webb D (1971) Orientation by means of long-range acoustic signaling in baleen whales. In: Adler HE (ed) Orientation: Sensory basis. Ann NY Acad Sci 188: 110–141Google Scholar
  15. Pierce GW, Griffin DR (1938) Experimental determination of supersonic notes emitted by bats. J Mammal 19:454–455Google Scholar
  16. Rasmussen LE, Buss IO, Hess DL, Schmidt MJ (1984) Testosterone and dihydrotestosterone concentrations in elephant serum and temporal gland secretions. Biol Reprod 30:352–362Google Scholar
  17. Schevill WE, Backus WA, Backus RH (1964) Underwater sounds of cetaceans. In: Tavolga WN (ed) Marine Bio-Acoustics. Pergamon Press, New York, pp 147–152Google Scholar
  18. Theurich M, Langner G, Scheich H (1984) Infrasound responses in the midbrain of the Guinea Fowl. Neurosci Lett 49:81–86Google Scholar
  19. Yodlowski ML, Kreithen ML, Keeton WT (1977) Detection of atmospheric infrasound by pigeons. Nature 265:725–726Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer-Verlag 1986

Authors and Affiliations

  • Katharine B. Payne
    • 1
  • William R. LangbauerJr.
    • 1
  • Elizabeth M. Thomas
    • 1
  1. 1.Laboratory of OrnithologyCornell UniversityIthacaUSA

Personalised recommendations