Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology

, Volume 17, Issue 4, pp 313–323 | Cite as

Alarm calls of Belding's ground squirrels to aerial predators: nepotism or self-preservation?

  • Paul W. Sherman
Article

Summary

Belding's ground squirrels (Spermophilus beldingi) give acoustically distinct alarm calls to aerial and terrestrial predators. The animals typically give multiple-note trills to predatory mammals, and single-note whistles to flying hawks. During a 9-year study of free-living S. beldingi at Tioga Pass, California, the adaptive significance of the whistle call was investigated. Data were gathered on 664 ground squirrel-hawk interactions, most of which were induced by flying trained raptors over individually marked study animals of known sex and age. The sight of a flying hawk and the sound of whistles stimulated widespread calling and running to shelter by the ground squirrels (Fig. 1). Wild raptors were rarely successful at capturing the rodents once a whistle had been given, and fewer callers than noncallers were killed (Table 1). Individuals of both sexes and all ages whistled equally often (Fig. 4), and females' tendencies to whistle were not affected by the presence of relatives, including offspring (Fig. 5). The most frequent callers were animals in exposed positions: far from cover and close to the predatory bird (Table 2). Taken together the data suggest that unlike trills, which increase vulnerability to terrestrial predators (Table 1) and function to warn relatives, whistle directly benefit callers by increasing their chances of escaping from hawks.

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. Balph DM, Balph DF (1966) Sound communication of Uinta ground squirrels. J Mammal 47:440–450Google Scholar
  2. Barash DP (1975) Marmot alarm-calling and the question of altruistic behavior. Am Midl Nat 94:468–470Google Scholar
  3. Barash DP (1976) Social behaviour and individual differences in free-living alpine marmots (Marmota marmota). Anim Behav 24:27–35Google Scholar
  4. Batschelet E (1981) Circular statistics in biology. Academic Press, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  5. Brown CH (1982) Ventroloquial and locatable vocalization in birds. Z Tierpsychol 59:338–350Google Scholar
  6. Charnov EL, Krebs JR (1975) The evolution of alarm calls: altruism or manipulation? Am Nat 109:107–112Google Scholar
  7. Cheney DL, Seyfarth RM (1981) Selective forces affecting the predator alarm calls of vervet monkeys. Behaviour 76:25–61Google Scholar
  8. Davis LS (1984) Alarm calling in Richardson's ground squirrels (Spermophilus richardsonii). Z Tierpsychol 66:152–164Google Scholar
  9. Dobson FS (1982) Competition for mates and predominant juvenile male dispersal in mammals. Anim Behav 30:1183–1192Google Scholar
  10. Drabek CM (1970) Ethoecology of the round-tailed ground squirrel, Spermophilus tereticaudus. PhD thesis, University of Arizona, TucsonGoogle Scholar
  11. Dunford C (1977) Kin selection for ground squirrel alarm calls. Am Nat 111:782–785Google Scholar
  12. Evans FC (1951) Notes on a population of the striped ground squirrel (Citellus tridecemlineatus) in an abandoned field in southeastern Michigan. J Mammal 32:437–449Google Scholar
  13. Fitch HS (1948) Ecology of the California ground squirrel on grazing lands. Am Midl Nat 39:513–596Google Scholar
  14. French AR (1982) Intraspecific differences in the pattern of hibernation in the ground squirrel Spermophilus beldingi. J Comp Physiol 148:83–91Google Scholar
  15. Greenwood PJ (1980) Mating systems, philopatry and dispersal in birds and mammals. Anim Behav 28:1140–1162Google Scholar
  16. Hamilton WD (1964) The genetical evolution of social behaviour, I and II. J Theor Biol 7:1–52Google Scholar
  17. Hamilton WD (1971) Geometry for the selfish herd. J Theor Biol 31:295–311Google Scholar
  18. Hanken J, Sherman PW (1981) Multiple paternity in Belding's ground squirrel litters. Science212:351–353Google Scholar
  19. Harris MA, Murie JO, Duncan JA (1983) Responses of Columbian ground squirrels to playback of recorded calls. Z Tierpsychol 63:318–330Google Scholar
  20. Holekamp KE (1983) Proximal mechanisms of natal dispersal in Belding's ground squirrel (Spermophilus beldingi beldingi). PhD thesis, University of California, BerkeleyGoogle Scholar
  21. Holekamp KE (1984a) Natal dispersal in Belding's ground squirrels (Spermophilus beldingi). Behav Ecol Sociobiol 16:21–30Google Scholar
  22. Holekamp KE (1984b) Dispersal in ground-dwelling sciurids. In: Murie JO, Michener GR (eds) The biology of ground-dwelling squirrels. University of Nebraska Press, Lincoln, Neb, pp 297–320Google Scholar
  23. Holmes WG, Sherman PW (1982) The ontogeny of kin recognition in two species of ground squirrels. Am Zool 22:491–517Google Scholar
  24. Hoogland JL (1983) Nepotism and alarm calling in the blacktailed prairie dog (Cynomys ludovicianus). Anim Behav 31:472–479Google Scholar
  25. Humphries DA, Driver PM (1970) Protean detense by prey animals. Oecologia (Berl) 5:285–302Google Scholar
  26. Ivins BL, Smith AT (1983) Responses of pikas (Ochotona princeps. Lagomorpha) to naturally occurring terrestrial predators. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 13:277–285Google Scholar
  27. Jenkins SH, Eshelman BD (1983) Spermophilus beldingi.Am Soc Mammal, Mammalian Sp 221:1–8Google Scholar
  28. Koeppl JW, Hoffmann RS, Nadler CF (1978) Pattern analysis of acoustical behavior in four species of ground squirrels. J Mammal 59:677–696Google Scholar
  29. Krebs JR, Dawkins R (1984) Animal signals: mind reading and manipulation. In: Krebs JR, Davies NB (eds) Behavioural ecology, 2nd edn. Sinauer, Sunderland, Mass, pp 380–402Google Scholar
  30. Leger DW, Owings DH (1978) Responses to alarm calls by California ground squirrels: effects of call structure and maternal status. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 3:177–186Google Scholar
  31. Leger DW, Owings DH, Boal LM (1979) Contextual information and differential responses to alarm whistles in California ground squirrels. Z Tierpsychol 49:142–155Google Scholar
  32. Leger DW, Owings DH, Gelfand DL (1980) Single-note vocalizations of California ground squirrels: graded signals and situation-specificity of predator and socially evoked calls. Z Tierpsychol 52:227–246Google Scholar
  33. Leger DW, Berney-Key SD, Sherman PW (1984) Vocalizations of Belding's ground squirrels (Spermophilus beldingi). Anim Behav 32:753–764Google Scholar
  34. Mader WJ (1975) Biology of the Harris' hawk in southern Arizona. Living Bird 14:59–85Google Scholar
  35. Markl H (1985) Manipulation, modulation, information, cognition: some of the riddles of communication. Fortschr Zool 31:163–194Google Scholar
  36. Marler P (1955) Characteristics of some animal calls.Nature 176:6–8Google Scholar
  37. Marler P (1956) Behaviour of the chaffinch (Fringilla coelebs). Behaviour (Suppl) 5:1–184Google Scholar
  38. Marler P (1959) Developments in the study of animal communication. In: Bell PR (ed), Darwin's biological work. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp 150–206Google Scholar
  39. Matocha KG (1977) The vocal repertoire of Spermophilus tridecemlineatus. Am Midl Nat 98:482–487Google Scholar
  40. Maynard Smith J (1965) The evolution of alarm calls. Am Nat 99:59–63Google Scholar
  41. Melchior HR (1971) Characteristics of Arctic ground squirrel alarm calls. Oecologia (Berl) 7:184–190Google Scholar
  42. Michener GR (1983) Kin identification, matriarchies, and the evolution of sociality in ground-dwelling sciurids. In: Eisenberg JF, Kleiman D (eds) Advances in the study of mammalian behavior. Am Soc Mammal Sp Publ 7, pp 528–572Google Scholar
  43. Morton ML (1975) Seasonal cycles of body weights and lipids in Belding ground squirrels. Bull South Calif Acad Sci 74:128–143Google Scholar
  44. Morton ML, Sherman PW (1978) Effects of a spring snowstorm on behavior, reproduction, and survival of Belding's ground squirrels. Can J Zool 56:2578–2590Google Scholar
  45. Müller-Schwarze D, Müller-Schwarze C (1971) Responses of chipmunks to models of aerial predators. J Mammal 52:456–458Google Scholar
  46. Neill SR St J, Cullen JM (1974) Experiments on whether schooling by their prey affects the hunting behavior of cephalopods and fish predators. J Zool (Lond) 172:549–569Google Scholar
  47. Noyes DH, Holmes WG (1979) Behavioral responses of freeliving hoary marmots to a model golden eagle. J Mammal 60:408–411Google Scholar
  48. Owens NW, Goss-Custard JD (1976) The adaptive significance of alarm calls given by shorebirds on their winter feeding grounds. Evolution 30:397–398Google Scholar
  49. Owings DH, Hennessy DF (1984) The importance of variation in sciurid visual and vocal communication. In: Murie JO, Michener GR (eds) The biology of ground dwelling squirrels. University of Nebraska Press, Lincoln, Neb, pp 169–200Google Scholar
  50. Owings DH, Leger DW (1980) Chatter vocalizations of California ground squirrels: Predator-and social-role specificity. Z Tierpsychol 54:163–184Google Scholar
  51. Owings DH, Virginia RA (1978) Alarm calls of California ground squirrels (Spermophilus beecheyi). Z Tierpsychol 46:58–70Google Scholar
  52. Perrins C (1968) The purpose of the high-intensity alarm call in small passerines. Ibis 110:200–201Google Scholar
  53. Pfeifer S (1980) Aerial predation on Wyoming ground squirrels. J Mammal 61:371–372Google Scholar
  54. Robinson SR (1980) Antipredator behaviour and predator recognition in Belding's ground squirrels. Anim Behav 28:840–852Google Scholar
  55. Robinson SR (1981) Alarm communication in Belding's ground squirrels. Z Tierpsychol 56:150–168Google Scholar
  56. Schwagmeyer PL (1980) Alarm calling behavior of the thirteenlined ground squirrel, Spermophilus tridecemlineatus. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 7:195–200Google Scholar
  57. Schwagmeyer PL, Brown CH (1981) Conspecific reaction to playback of thirteen-lined ground squirrel vocalizations. Z Tierpsychol 56:25–32Google Scholar
  58. Sherman PW (1976) Natural selection among some group-living organisms. PhD thesis, University of Michigan, Ann ArborGoogle Scholar
  59. Sherman PW (1977) Nepotism and the evolution of alarm calls. Science 197:1246–1253Google Scholar
  60. Sherman PW (1980a) The limits of ground squirrel nepotism. In: Barlow GW, Silverberg J (eds) Sociobiology: beyond nature/nurture? Westview, Boulder, Col, pp 505–544Google Scholar
  61. Sherman PW (1980b) The meaning of nepotism. Am Nat 116:604–606Google Scholar
  62. Sherman PW (1981a)Kinship, demography, and Belding's ground squirrel nepotism. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 8:251–259Google Scholar
  63. Sherman PW (1981b) Reproductive competition and infanticide in Belding's ground squirrels and other animals. In: Alexander RD, Tinkle DW (eds)Natural selection and social behavior: Recent research and new theory. Chiron, New York, pp 311–331Google Scholar
  64. Sherman PW, Morton ML (1984) Demography of Belding's ground squirrels. Ecology 65:1617–1628Google Scholar
  65. Shields WM (1980) Ground squirrel alarm calls: nepotism or parental care? Am Nat 116:599–603Google Scholar
  66. Smith SF (1978) Alarm calls, their origin and use in Eutamias sonomae. J Mammal 59:888–893Google Scholar
  67. Smythe N (1970) On the existence of“pursuit invitation” signals in mammals. Am Nat 104:491–494Google Scholar
  68. Sokal RR, Rohlf FJ (1981) Biometry, 2nd edn. Freeman, San FranciscoGoogle Scholar
  69. Tilson RL, Norton PM (1981) Alarm duetting and pursuit deterrence in an African antelope. Am Nat 118:455–462Google Scholar
  70. Turner LW (1973) Vocal and escape responses of Spermophilus beldingi to predators. J Mammal 54:990–993Google Scholar
  71. Vestal BM, McCarley H (1984) Spatial and social relations of kin in thirteen-lined and other ground squirrels. In: Murie JO, Michener GR (eds) The biology of ground-dwelling squirrels. University of Nebraska Press, Lincoln, Neb, pp 404–423Google Scholar
  72. Williams GC (1966) Adaptation and natural selection. Princeton University Press, PrincetonGoogle Scholar
  73. Woodland DJ, Jaafar Z, Knight M (1980) The “pursuit deterrent” function of alarm signals. Am Nat 115:748–753Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer-Verlag 1985

Authors and Affiliations

  • Paul W. Sherman
    • 1
  1. 1.Section of Neurobiology and BehaviorCornell UniversityIthacaUSA

Personalised recommendations