Advertisement

Sex Roles

, Volume 11, Issue 3–4, pp 315–331 | Cite as

Instructional control of sex-related motor behavior in extremely masculine or feminine adults

  • Steven C. Hayes
  • Rosemery O. Nelson
  • David L. Steele
  • Marie E. Meeler
  • David H. Barlow
Article

Abstract

The main purpose of this study was to examine the degree to which apparently sex-typed subjects manipulated behaviors on the Sex Role Motor Behavior Checklist in response to instructions to convey specific sex-role impressions. Extremely masculine and feminine males and females (10–12 per group) were assessed for their ability to change sex-related motor behavior upon request. Subjects were able to produce significant increases in masculine and feminine motor behavior, particularly in the areas of walking and sitting. Many individual behaviors, however, showed small or inconsistent changes. Apparently, many of the motor differences naturally displayed by males and females are either not consciously part of sex-typed adults' concept of given sex roles or are relatively inflexible in this population. Secondary purposes of the study were to identify specific sex-related motor behavior (a) to alter when changes in sex role seem desirable and (b) to utilize as dependent measures in measuring changes in sex role. Walking and sitting were shown to be especially important areas.

Keywords

Social Psychology Dependent Measure Individual Behavior Motor Behavior Secondary Purpose 
These keywords were added by machine and not by the authors. This process is experimental and the keywords may be updated as the learning algorithm improves.

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. Barlow, D. H., Reynolds, J., & Agras, S. Gender identity change in a transsexual. Archives of General Psychiatry, 1973, 28, 569–576.Google Scholar
  2. Barlow, D. H., Hayes, S. C., Nelson, R. O., Steele, D., Meeler, M., & Mills, J. Sex role specific motor behavior: A behavioral checklist. Behavioral Assessment, 1979, 1, 119–138.Google Scholar
  3. Barlow, D. H., Mills, J. R., Agras, W. S., & Steinman, D. L. Comparison of sex-typed motor behavior in male-to-female transsexuals and women. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 1980, 9, 245–253.Google Scholar
  4. Bates, J. E., Bentler, P. M., & Thompson, S. K. Gender-deviant boys compared with normal and clinical control boys. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 1979, 7, 243–259.Google Scholar
  5. Bem, S. L. The measurement of psychological androgyny. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 1974, 42, 155–162.Google Scholar
  6. Birdwhistell, R. Kinesics and context: Essays on body motion communication. Philadelphia: University of Pennsylvania Press, 1970.Google Scholar
  7. Cutting, J. E., & Kozlowski, L. T. Recognizing friends by their walk: Gait perception without familiarity cues. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 1977, 9, 353–356.Google Scholar
  8. Hayes, S. C., & Leonard, S. R. Sex-related motor behavior: Effects on social impressions and social cooperation. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 1983, 12, 415–426.Google Scholar
  9. Hayes, S. C., Nelson, R. O., Steele, D. L., Meeler, M. E., & Barlow, D. H. The development of the display and knowledge of sex related motor behavior in children. Child Behavior Therapy, 1981, 3, 1–24.Google Scholar
  10. Heckerman, C. L., Schoen, S., & Barlow, D. H. Situational specificity of sex role motor behavior: A preliminary investigation. Behavioral Assessment, 1981, 3, 43–51.Google Scholar
  11. Hewes, G. W. The anthropology of posture. Scientific American, 1957, 196, 123–132.Google Scholar
  12. Jenni, D. A., & Jenni, M. Carrying behavior in humans: Analysis of sex differences. Science, 1976, 188, 859–860.Google Scholar
  13. Leonard, S. R. Sex related motor behavior change: A demonstration of empirical clinical practice. Behavioral Assessment, 1981, 3, 403–410.Google Scholar
  14. Mehrabian, A. Nonverbal communication. Chicago: Aldine-Atherton, 1972.Google Scholar
  15. Rekers, G. A., Amaro-Plotkin, H. D., & Low, B. P. Sex-typed mannerisms in normal boys and girls and as a function of sex and age. Child Development, 1977, 48, 275–278.Google Scholar
  16. Rekers, G. A., Lovaas, O. I., & Low, B. The behavioral treatment of a transsexual. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 1974, 2, 99–115.Google Scholar
  17. Rekers, G. A., Yates, C. E., Willis, T. J., Rosen, A. C., & Taubman, M. Childhood gender identity change: Operant control over sex-typed play and mannerisms. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 1976, 7, 51–57.Google Scholar
  18. Robbins, L. M., & Coleman, W. L. Fundamentals of footprint analysis. Springfield, Ill.: Charles C Thomas, in press.Google Scholar
  19. Spence, J. T., Helmreich, R., & Stapp, J. Ratings of self and peers on sex role attributes and their relation to self-esteem and conceptions of masculinity and femininity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 1975, 32, 29–39.Google Scholar
  20. Wolf, M. M. Social validity: The case for subjective measurement, or how applied behavior analysis is finding its heart. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 1978, 11,, 203–214.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Plenum Publishing Corporation 1984

Authors and Affiliations

  • Steven C. Hayes
    • 1
  • Rosemery O. Nelson
    • 1
  • David L. Steele
    • 1
  • Marie E. Meeler
    • 1
  • David H. Barlow
    • 2
  1. 1.Department of PsychologyUniversity of North Carolina at GreensboroGreensboro
  2. 2.State University of New York at AlbanyUSA

Personalised recommendations