Advertisement

Experimental Brain Research

, Volume 107, Issue 3, pp 497–503 | Cite as

Influence of skin temperature on heat pain threshold in humans

  • Antti Pertovaara
  • Timo Kauppila
  • Minna M. Hämäläinen
Research Article

Abstract

We compared the effect of skin temperature on the critical threshold temperature eliciting heat pain with the effect of skin temperature on the response latency to the first heat pain sensation in healthy human subjects. Also, we determined the effect of the duration of a heat stimulus ramp on pain threshold. Furthermore, we determined the effect of skin temperature on mechanically induced pain. We found that the latency to the first pain sensation induced by a radiant heat stimulus was significantly decreased with an increase in the skin temperature (25–35 °C). However, independent of the rate of the stimulus rise (3–10 °C/s) and independent of the stimulus location (hairy vs glabrous skin), the threshold temperature for eliciting the heat pain sensation, determined with a contact thermostimulator, was not changed by a change in the skin temperature in the same subjects. With a fast rate of stimulus rise, a higher pain threshold was obtained than with a slow rise of stimulus temperature. However, this difference was found only with subject-controlled ascending stimuli (method of limits) but not with experimenter-controlled, predetermined stimulus ramps (method of levels). The heat pain threshold was higher in the glabrous skin of the hand than in the hairy skin of the forearm. With increasing stimulus duration (2.5–10s), the threshold temperature eliciting the heat pain sensation was significantly decreased. The mechanically induced pain threshold was not influenced by the skin temperature. The results indicate that the critical temperature for eliciting heat pain is independent of the skin temperature in humans. However, a change in skin temperature is an important source of an artefactual change in heat pain sensitivity when the radiant heat method (latency or energy) is used as an index of pain sensitivity. With a method dependent on reaction time (the method of limits), the heat pain threshold was artefactually increased, with fast rates of stimulus rise due to the long delay of slowly conducting heat pain signals in reaching the brain. With an increase in the duration of the heat stimulus, the critical temperature for eliciting pain sensation was significantly decreased, which may be explained by central neuronal mechanisms (temporal summation).

Key words

Heat pain threshold Adapting skin temperature Temporal summation Rate of stimulus rise Mechanical pain threshold Human 

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. Adriaensen HJ, Gybels J, Handwerker HO, VanHees J (1983) Response properties of thin myelinated (A-δ) fibres in human skin nerves. J Neurophysiol 49: 111–122Google Scholar
  2. Adriaensen HJ, Gybels J, Handwerker HO, VanHees J (1984) Suppression of C-fibre discharges upon repeated heat stimulation may explain characteristics of concomitant pain sensations. Brain Res 302: 203–211Google Scholar
  3. Arendt-Nielsen L, Bjerring P (1988) Sensory and pain threshold characteristics to laser stimuli. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 51: 35–42Google Scholar
  4. Benjamin FB (1959) Spatial summation of pain. Proc Soc Exp Biol Med 101: 380–382Google Scholar
  5. Berge OG, Garcia-Cabrera I, Hole K (1988) Response latencies in the tail-flick test depend on tail skin temperature. Neurosci Lett 86: 284–288Google Scholar
  6. Birren JE, Casperson RC, Botwinick J (1951) Pain measurement by the radiant heat method: individual differences in pain sensitivity, the effects of skin temperature, and stimulus duration. J Exp Psychol 41: 419–424Google Scholar
  7. Bromm B, Jahnke MT, Treede RD (1984) Responses of human cutaneous afferents to CO2 laser stimuli causing pain. Exp Brain Res 55: 158–166Google Scholar
  8. Campbell JN, LaMotte RH (1983) Latency to detection of first pain. Brain Res 266: 203–208Google Scholar
  9. Casey KL, Butler J, Lewis KG, Morrow TJ (1987) Evidence that the distribution of polymodal nociceptors does not determine both thermal and mechanical pain thresholds of human glabrous and hairy skin. Soc Neurosci Abstr 13: 189Google Scholar
  10. Croze S, Duclaux R (1978) Thermal pain in humans: influence of the rate of stimulation. Brain Res 157: 418–421Google Scholar
  11. Croze S, Duclaux R, Kenshalo DR (1976) The thermal sensitivity of the polymodal nociceptors in the monkey. J Physiol (Lond) 263: 539–562Google Scholar
  12. Croze S, Duclaux R, Russek M (1977) Constancy of heat pain characteristics to changes in skin and body temperature. Brain Res 131: 367–372Google Scholar
  13. Douglass DK, Carstens E, Watkins LR (1992) Spatial summation in human thermal pain perception: comparison with and between dermatomes. Pain 50: 197–202Google Scholar
  14. Duggan AW, Griersmith BT, Headley PM, Maher JB (1978) The need to control skin temperature when using radiant heat in tests of analgesia. Exp Neurol 61: 471–478Google Scholar
  15. Greene LC, Hardy JD (1958) Spatial summation of pain. J Appl Physiol 13: 457–164Google Scholar
  16. Hall KR (1955) Relation of skin temperature to pain threshold. Q J Exp Psychol 7: 74–81Google Scholar
  17. Hallin RG, Torebjörk HE, Wiesenfeld Z (1981) Nociceptors and warm receptors innervated by C fibres in human skin. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 45: 313–319Google Scholar
  18. Hardy JD, Goodell H, Wolff HG (1951) The influence of skin temperature upon the pain threshold as evoked by thermal radiation. Science 114: 149–150Google Scholar
  19. Hardy JD, Wolff HG, Goodell H (1952) Pain sensations and reactions. Williams & Wilkins, BaltimoreGoogle Scholar
  20. Hole K, Tjolsen A (1993) The tail-flick and formalin tests in rodents: changes in skin temperature as a confounding factor. Pain 53: 247–254PubMedGoogle Scholar
  21. Kojo I, Pertovaara A (1986) Effect of tourniquet-induced ischemia on cutaneous thermal thresholds. Acta Neurol Scand 74: 383–386Google Scholar
  22. Kojo I, Pertovaara A (1987) The effects of stimulus area and adaptation temperature on warm and heat pain thresholds in man. Int J Neurosci 32: 875–880Google Scholar
  23. Lichtman AH, Smith FL, Martin BR (1993) Evidence that the antinociceptive tail-flick response is produced independently from changes in either tail-skin temperature or core temperature. Pain 55: 283–295Google Scholar
  24. Luukko M, Konttinen Y, Kemppinen P, Pertovaara A (1994) Influence of various experimental parameters on the incidence of thermal and mechanical hyperalgesia induced by a constriction mononeuropathy of the sciatic nerve in lightly anesthetized rat. Exp Neurol 128: 143–154Google Scholar
  25. Lynn B (1979) The heat sensitization of polymodal nociceptors in the rabbit and its independence of the local blood flow. J Physiol (Lond) 287: 493–507Google Scholar
  26. Machet-Pietropaoli H, Chery-Croze S (1979) Spatial summation of thermal pain in human beings. Sensory Process 3: 183–187Google Scholar
  27. Mendell LM (1966) Physiological properties of unmyelinated fibre projections to the spinal cord. Exp Neurol 16: 316–332Google Scholar
  28. Milne RJ, Gamble GD (1989) Habituation to sham testing procedures modifies tail-flick latencies: effects on nociception rather than vasomotor tone. Pain 39: 103–107Google Scholar
  29. Pertovaara A, Kojo I (1985) Influence of the rate of temperature change on thermal thresholds in man. Exp Neurol 87: 439–445Google Scholar
  30. Pertovaara A, Reinikainen K, Hari R (1984) The activation of unmyelinated or myelinated afferent fibers by brief infrared laser pulses varies with skin type. Brain Res 307: 341–343Google Scholar
  31. Pertovaara A, Morrow TJ, Casey KL (1988) Cutaneous pain and detection thresholds to short CO2 laser pulses in humans: evidence on afferent mechanisms and the influence of varying stimulus conditions. Pain 34: 261–269Google Scholar
  32. Price DD, Hu JW, Dubner R, Gracely RH (1977) Peripheral suppression of first pain and central summation of second pain evoked by noxious heat pulses. Pain 3: 57–68CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  33. Price DD, McHaffie JG, Larson MA (1989) Spatial summation of heat-induced pain: influence of stimulus area and spatial separation of stimuli on perceived sensation intensity and unpleasentness. J Neurophysiol 62: 1270–1279Google Scholar
  34. Torebjörk HE, LaMotte RH, Robinson CJ (1984) Peripheral neural correlates of magnitude of cutaneous pain and hyperalgesia: imultaneous recordings in humans of sensory judgments of pain and evoked responses in nociceptors with C-fibres. J Neurophysiol 51: 325–339Google Scholar
  35. Treede RD, Oakland DB, Meyer RA, Campbell JN (1990) The apparent heat threshold of C-fiber nociceptors is dependent on stimulus parameters. Soc Neurosci Abstr 16 416Google Scholar
  36. VanHees J, Gybels J (1981) C nociceptor activity in human nerve during painful and non-painful skin stimulation. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 44: 600–607Google Scholar
  37. Wilcox GL, Giesler GL (1984) An instrument using a multiple layer Peltier device to change skin temperature rapidly. Brain Res Bull 12: 143–146Google Scholar
  38. Yarnitsky D, Ochoa JL (1990) Studies of heat pain sensation in man: perception thresholds, rate of stimulus rise and reaction time. Pain 40: 85–91Google Scholar
  39. Yarnitsky D, Ochoa JL (1991) Differential effect of compressionischaemia block on warm sensation and heat-induced pain. Brain 114: 907–913Google Scholar
  40. Yarnitsky D, Simone DA, Dotson RM, Cline MA, Ochoa JL (1992) Single C nociceptor responses and psychophysical parameters of evoked pain: effect of rate of rise of heat stimuli in humans. J Physiol (Lond) 450: 581–592Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer-Verlag 1996

Authors and Affiliations

  • Antti Pertovaara
    • 1
    • 2
  • Timo Kauppila
    • 2
  • Minna M. Hämäläinen
    • 2
  1. 1.Department of PhysiologyUniversity of TurkuTurkuFinland
  2. 2.Department of PhysiologyUniversity of HelsinkiFinland

Personalised recommendations