Biological Cybernetics

, Volume 66, Issue 3, pp 241–251 | Cite as

On the computational architecture of the neocortex

II The role of cortico-cortical loops
  • D. Mumford
Article

Abstract

This paper is a sequel to an earlier paper which proposed an active role for the thalamus, integrating multiple hypotheses formed in the cortex via the thalamo-cortical loop. In this paper, I put forward a hypothesis on the role of the reciprocal, topographic pathways between two cortical areas, one often a ‘higher’ area dealing with more abstract information about the world, the other ‘lower’, dealing with more concrete data. The higher area attempts to fit its abstractions to the data it receives from lower areas by sending back to them from its deep pyramidal cells a template reconstruction best fitting the lower level view. The lower area attempts to reconcile the reconstruction of its view that it receives from higher areas with what it knows, sending back from its superficial pyramidal cells the features in its data which are not predicted by the higher area. The whole calculation is done with all areas working simultaneously, but with order imposed by synchronous activity in the various top-down, bottom-up loops. Evidence for this theory is reviewed and experimental tests are proposed. A third part of this paper will deal with extensions of these ideas to the frontal lobe.

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. Brodal A (1981) Neurological anatomy. Oxford University Press, OxfordGoogle Scholar
  2. Carpenter G, Grossberg S (1987) A massively parallel architecture for a self-organizing neural pattern recognition machine. Comp Vision Graphics Image Proc 37:54–115Google Scholar
  3. Cavanagh P (1991) What's up in top-down processing. In: Gorei A (ed) Representations of vision. Camb. University Press, Cambridge, pp 295–304Google Scholar
  4. Crick F, Knock C (1990) Towards a neurobiological theory of consciousness. Semin Neurosci (in press)Google Scholar
  5. Deacon T (1988) Holism and associationism in neurophyschology: an anatomical synthesis. In: Perecman E (ed) Integrating theory and practice in clinical neuropsychology. Erlbaum, Hillsdale NJGoogle Scholar
  6. Deacon T (in preparation) Laminar organization of frontal corticocortical connections in the monkey brainGoogle Scholar
  7. DeFelipe J, Jones E (1988) Cajal on the cerebral cortex. Oxford University Press, OxfordGoogle Scholar
  8. Desimone R (1991) Face selective cells in the temporal cortex of monkeys. J Cogn Neurosci 3:1–8Google Scholar
  9. DeValois KK, DeValois R, Yund EW (1979) Responses of striate cortex cells to grating and checkerboard patterns. J Physiol (London) 291:483–505Google Scholar
  10. Dobbins A, Zucker S, Cynader M (1987) Endstopping in the visual cortex: a neural substrate for calculating curvature. Nature 329:96–103Google Scholar
  11. Felleman DJ, Van Essen DC (1991) Distributed hierarchical processing in primate cerebral cortex. Cerebral Cortex 1:1–47Google Scholar
  12. Fischler M, Elschlager RA (1973) The representation and matching of pictorial structures. IEEE Trans Comp 22:67–92Google Scholar
  13. Fodor J, Pylyshin X (1988) Connectionism and cognitive architecture. Cognition 28:3–71Google Scholar
  14. Freeman W, Skarda CA (1985) Spatial EEG patterns, non-linear dynamics and perception. Brain Res Rev 10:147–175Google Scholar
  15. Gray C, Singer W (1989) Stimulus-specific neuronal oscillations in orientation columns of cat visual cortex. Proc Natl Acad Sci 86:1698–1702Google Scholar
  16. Huber P (1981) Robust statistics. Wiley, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  17. Ito M (1984) The cerebellum and neural control. Raven Press, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  18. Mamelak A, Hobson JA (1988) Dream bizarreness as the cognitive correlate of altered neuronal behavior in REM sleep. J Cogn Neurosci 1:201–222Google Scholar
  19. Pavlidis T (1988) Image analysis. Ann Rev Comput Sci 3:121–146Google Scholar
  20. Pearson RCA, Brodal P, Gatter KC, Powell TPS (1982) The organization of the connections between the cortex and the claustrum in the monkey. Brain Res 234:435–441Google Scholar
  21. Perkel DJ, Bullier J, Kennedy H (1986) Topography of the afferent connectivity of area 17 in the Macaque monkey. J Comp Neurol 253:374–402Google Scholar
  22. Poggio T (1990) A theory of how the brain might work. In: The Brain. Proc Cold Spring Harbor Symp 55Google Scholar
  23. Poggio T, Girosi F (1990) A theory of networks for learning. Science 247:978–982Google Scholar
  24. Primrose D, Strick P (1985) The organization of interconnections between the premotor areas of the primate frontal lobe and the arm area of the primary motor cortex. Soc Neurosci (abstr) 11:1274Google Scholar
  25. Rock I (1984) Perception. Sci. Am. Books, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  26. Rockland KR, Virga A (1989) Terminal arbors of individual ‘feed-back’ axons projecting from area V2 to VI in the macaque monkey. J Comp Neurol 285:54–72Google Scholar
  27. Rolls ET (1990) The representation of information in the temporal lobe visual cortical arteas of macaques. In: Eckmiller R (ed) Advanced neural computers, Elsevier, New York Amsterdam pp 69–78Google Scholar
  28. Shepard R, Cooper LA (1982) Mental images and their transformations. MIT Press, LancasterGoogle Scholar
  29. Van Essen DC, Newsome WT, Maunsell JHR, Bixby JL (1986) The projections from striate cortex to areas V2 and V3 in the Macaque monkey. J Comp Neurol 244:451–480Google Scholar
  30. Winfield DA, Gatter KC, Powell TPS (1990) An electron microscopic study of the types and proportions of neurons in the cortex of the motor and visual areas of the cat and rat. Brain 103:245–258Google Scholar
  31. Yuille A (1991) Deformable templates for face recognition. J Cogn Neurosci 3:59–70Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer-Verlag 1992

Authors and Affiliations

  • D. Mumford
    • 1
  1. 1.Mathematics DepartmentHarvard UniversityCambridgeUSA

Personalised recommendations