Configurational pattern discrimination responsible for dishabituation in common toads Bufo bufo (L.): Behavioral tests of the predictions of a neural model
- 49 Downloads
- 8 Citations
Summary
Recently, a neural model of visual pattern discrimination for stimulus-specific habituation was developed, based on previous behavioral studies which demonstrated that toads exhibit a dishabituation hierarchy for different worm-like stimuli. The model suggests that visual objects are represented by temporal coding and predicts that the dishabituation hierarchy changes when the stimulus/background contrast direction is reversed or the stimulus size is varied. The behavioral experiments reported in this paper were designed to test these predictions, (1) For a pair of stimuli from the contrast reversal prediction, the experimental results validated the theory. (2) For a pair of stimuli from the size reduction prediction, the experimental results failed to validate the theory. Further experiments concerning size effects suggest that configurai visual pattern discrimination in toads exhibits size invariance. (3) Inspired by the Groves-Thompson account of habituation, we found that dishabituation by a second stimulus has a separate process from habituation to a first stimulus. This paper serves as an example of a fruitful dialogue between experimentation and modeling, crucial for understanding brain functions.
Key words
Habituation Model testing Visual pattern recognition Non-associative learning ToadAbbreviations
- a-h
worm-like stimulus patterns
- AT
anterior thalamus
- ERF
excitatory receptive field
- IRF
inhibitory receptive field
- RF
receptive field
- R2 to R4
retinal ganglion cell types
- vMP
posterior ventromedial pallium
Preview
Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.
References
- Arbib MA (1989) The metaphorical brain 2: neural networks and beyond. Wiley Interscience, New York Chichester Brisbane Toronto SingaporeGoogle Scholar
- Birukow G (1955) Angeborene und erworbene Anteile relativ einfacher Verhaltenseinheiten. Verh Deutsch Zool Ges 1955:32–47Google Scholar
- Birukow G, Meng M (1955) Eine neue Methode zur Prüfung des Gesichtssinnes bei Amphibien. Naturwissenschaften 42:652–653Google Scholar
- Boice R, Quanty CB, Williams RC (1974) Competition and possible dominance in turtles, toads, and frogs. J Comp Physiol Psychol 86:1116–1131Google Scholar
- Burghagen H, Ewert J-P (1982) Question of “head preference” in response to worm-like dummies during prey-capture of toads Bufo bufo, Behav Processes 7:295–306Google Scholar
- Carew TJ, Castellucci VF, Kandel ER (1971) An analysis of dishabituation and sensitization of the gill-withdrawal reflex in Aplysia. Int J Neurosci 2:79–98Google Scholar
- Cerella J (1975) Studies of pattern perception in pigeons. Ph.D. Dissertation, Harvard University, Cambridge MassGoogle Scholar
- Eikmanns KH (1955) Verhaltensphysiologische Untersuchungen über den Beutefang und das Bewegungssehen der Erdkröte (Bufo bufo L). Z Tierpsychol 12:229–253Google Scholar
- Ewert J-P (1967) Der Einfluß von Störreizen auf die Antwortbereitschaft bei der Richtbewegung der Erdkröte (Bufo bufo L). Z Tierpsychol 24:298–312Google Scholar
- Ewert J-P (1984) Tectal mechanisms that underlie prey-catching and avoidance behaviors in toads. In: Vanegas H (ed) Comparative neurology of the optic tectum. Plenum Press, New York London, pp 246–416Google Scholar
- Ewert J-P (1987) Neuroethology of releasing mechanism: preycatching in toads. Behav Brain Sci 10:337–405Google Scholar
- Ewert J-P (1992) Neuroethology of an object features relating algorithm and its modification by learning. Rev Neurosci (in press)Google Scholar
- Ewert J-P, Hock FJ (1972) Movement sensitive neurons in the toad's retina. Exp Brain Res 16:41–59Google Scholar
- Ewert J-P, Ingle DJ (1971) Excitatory effects following habituation of prey-catching activity in frogs and toads. J Comp Physiol Psychol 77:369–374Google Scholar
- Ewert J-P, Kehl W (1978) Configurational prey-selection by individual experience in the toad Bufo bufo. J Comp Physiol 126:105–114Google Scholar
- Finkenstädt T (1989) Visual associative learning: searching for behaviorally relevant brain structures in toads: In: Ewert J-P, Arbib MA (eds) Visuomotor coordination: amphibians, comparisons, models, and robots. Plenum Press, New York London, pp 799–832Google Scholar
- Finkenstädt T, Ewert J-P (1988a) Stimulus-specific long-term habituation of visually guided orienting behavior toward prey in toads: a 14C-2DG study. J Comp Physiol A 163:1–11Google Scholar
- Finkenstädt T, Ewert J-P (1988b) Effects of visual associative conditioning on behavior and cerebral metabolic activity in toads. Naturwissenschaften 75:85–97Google Scholar
- Fukushima K (1988) Neocognitron: a hierarchical neural network capable of visual pattern recognition. Neural Networks 1:119–130Google Scholar
- Gellerman LW (1933) Form discrimination in chimpanzees and two-year-old children: 1. Form (triangularity) per se. J Genet Psychol 42:2–27Google Scholar
- Groves PM, Thompson RF (1970) Habituation: a dual-process theory. Psychol Rev 77:419–450Google Scholar
- Harvey CB, Moore M, Lindsay L (1981) Passive-avoidance learning in bullfrogs, grass frogs, and toads. Psychol Rep 49:1003–1006Google Scholar
- Hateren JH van, Srinivasan MV, Wait PB (1990) Pattern recognition in bees: orientation discrimination. J Comp Physiol A 167:649–654Google Scholar
- Herrnstein RJ, Loveland BH (1964) Complex visual concept in the pigeon. Science 146:549–551Google Scholar
- Ingle D (1968) Visual releasers of prey-catching behavior in frogs and toads. Brain Behav Evol 1:500–518Google Scholar
- Ingle D (1971) Vision: The experimental analysis of visual behavior. In: Hoar WS, Randall DJ (eds) Fish Physiology, Vol. V. Academic Press, New York London, pp 59–71Google Scholar
- Ingle D (1978) Mechanisms of shape-recognition among vertebrates. In: Held R, Leibowitz HW, Teuber H-L (eds) Perception (Handbook of sensory physiology, Vol. VIII). Springer, Berlin Heidelberg New York, pp 259–287Google Scholar
- Ingle DJ, McKinley D (1978) Effects of stimulus configuration on elicited prey catching by the marine toad (Bufo marinus). Anim Behav 26:885–891Google Scholar
- Jones WA, Falkenberg VP (1980) Test for effects of visual and position cues on T-maze learning in toads. Percept Motor Skills 50:455–460Google Scholar
- Kandel E (1976) Cellular basis of behavior: an introduction to behavioral neurobiology. Freeman Comp, San FranciscoGoogle Scholar
- Karplus I, Algom D, Samuel D (1981) Acquisition and retention of dark avoidance by the toads, Xenopus laevis (Dudin). Anim Learning Behav 9:45–59Google Scholar
- Merkel-Harff C, Ewert J-P (1991) Learning-related modulation of toad's responses to prey by neural loops involving the forebrain. In: Arbib MA, Ewert J-P (eds) Visual structures and integrated functions. Research notes in neural computing, Vo13. Springer, Berlin Heidelberg New York, pp 417–426Google Scholar
- Pache J (1932) Formensehen bei Fröschen. Z Vergl Physiol 17:423–463Google Scholar
- Schleidt W (1962) Die historische Entwicklung der Begriffe “angeborenes auslösendes Schema” und “angeborener Auslösemechanismus” in der Ethologie. Z Tierpsychol 19:697–722Google Scholar
- Schmajuk NA, Segura ET (1980) Behavioral changes along escape learning in toads. Acta Physiol Lat Am 30:211–215Google Scholar
- Schmajuk NA, Segura ET, Reboreda JC (1980) Appetitive conditioning and discriminatory learning in toads. Behav Neural Biol 28:392–397Google Scholar
- Smith KU (1934) Visual discrimination in the cat: II. A further study of the capacity of the cat for visual figure discrimination. J Genet Psychol 45:336–357Google Scholar
- Sokolov E (1975) Neuronal mechanisms of the orienting reflex. In: Sokolov E, Vinogradova O (eds) Neuronal mechanisms of the orienting reflex. Lawrence Erlbaum Hillsdale, New York, 217–235Google Scholar
- Sutherland NS (1969) Shape discrimination in rat, octopus, and goldfish: a comparative study. J Comp Physiol Psychol 67:160–176Google Scholar
- Sutherland NS, Carr AE (1968) Shape discrimination by rats: squares and rectangles. Brit J Psychol 55:39–48Google Scholar
- Tinbergen N (1951) The study of instinct. Clarendon Press, OxfordGoogle Scholar
- Thompson PA, Boice R (1975) Attempts to train frogs: review and experiments. J Biol Psychol 17:3–13Google Scholar
- Thompson RF, Spencer WA (1966) Habituation: a model phenomenon for the study of neuronal substrates of behavior. Psychol Rev 73:16–43Google Scholar
- Thorpe WH (1963) Learning and instinct in animals. Harvard University Press, Cambridge MassGoogle Scholar
- Tsai HJ, Ewert J-P (1987) Edge preference of retinal and tectal neurons in common toads (Bufo bufo) in response to worm-like moving stripes: the question of behaviorally relevant position indicators. J Comp Physiol A 161:295–304Google Scholar
- Wang DL (1991) Neural networks for temporal order learning and stimulus specific habituation. Ph.D. Dissertation, Univ Southern CaliforniaGoogle Scholar
- Wang DL, Arbib MA (1991) How does the toad's visual system discriminate different worm-like stimuli? Biol Cybern 64:251–261Google Scholar
- Wang DL, Hsu CC (1990) SLONN: a simulation language for modeling of neural networks. Simulation 55:69–83Google Scholar
- Wehner R (1981) Spatial vision in arthropods. In: Autrum H (ed) Vision in invertebrates (Handbook of sensory physiology, vol VII/6C). Springer, Berlin Heidelberg New York, pp 287–616Google Scholar
- Wells MJ (1978) Octopus: physiology and behavior of an advanced invertebrate. Chapman and Hall, LondonGoogle Scholar