Psychopharmacology

, Volume 95, Issue 3, pp 303–312 | Cite as

Low doses of methylphenidate (Ritalin) may alter the delay-of-reinforcement gradient

  • Terje Sagvolden
  • Knut Sl»tta
  • Erik Arntzen
Original Investigations

Abstract

The present study investigated effects of methylphenidate on spatio-temporal distributions of responses generated by a fixed interval 60-s schedule of reinforcement. A response panel with 20 different response locations (holes) made it possible to distinguish effects of the drug on the procedurally-correct response (correct according to the contingencies) and effects on other topographically similar responses acting as functionally-adequate responses. Detailed flow charts show how the dynamics of responding changed with dose and segment of the fixed interval. The number of holes visited was increased after low and medium doses, but decreased after high doses. The spatial differences between responses in the initial and the final parts of the fixed interval gradually disappeared after 6 mg/kg or higher doses as the functionally-adequate responses moved to earlier segments of the fixed interval. Few results could, however, be described as response rate dependent. Distributions of responses around the correct hole illustrated differences between procedurally-correct and functionally-adequate classes. The number of holes included in the latter class changed by dose in an inverted U-shaped fashion. Perseverations and stereotyped responding increased with increasing dose. Most of the effects may be described as motor stimulatory. One way of explaining the stimulatory effect is in terms of a lengthened delay-of-reinforcement gradient. The rate-dependent effect of methylphenidate on the procedurally-correct class after some doses may have been due to the combined effects of a lengthened delay gradient increasing the response rates early in the interval and interference from perseverations having their most detrimental effects on the high rates late in the interval.

Key words

Ritalin Methylphenidate Rate dependency Delay of reinforcement Temporal response distribution Spatial response distribution Nose-poke responses Rats 

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. Boren JJ, Moerschbaecher JM, Whyte AA (1978) Variability of response location on fixed-ratio and fixed-interval schedules of reinforcement. J Exp Anal Behav 30:63–67Google Scholar
  2. Catania AC (1971) Reinforcement schedules: the role of responses preceding the one that produces the reinforcer. J Exp Anal Behav 15:271–287Google Scholar
  3. Catania AC (1973) The concept of the operant in the analysis of behavior. Behaviorism 1:103–116Google Scholar
  4. Catania AC, Keller K (1981) Contingency, contiguity, correlation, and the concept of causation. In: Harzem P, Zeiler MD (eds) Predictability, correlation, and contiguity. Wiley, New York, pp 125–167Google Scholar
  5. Catania AC, Sagvolden T, Keller KJ (1988) Reinforcement schedules: retroactive and proactive effects of reinforcers inserted into fixed-interval performances. J Exp Anal Behav 49:49–73Google Scholar
  6. Dews PB (1955) Studies on behavior. III. Effects of pentobarbital, methamphetamine and scopolamine on performances in pigeons involving discriminations. J Pharmacol Exp Ther 115:380–389Google Scholar
  7. Dews PB (1958) Studies on behavior. IV. Stimulant actions of methamphetamine. J Pharmacol Exp Ther 122:137–147Google Scholar
  8. Dews PB (1960) Free-operant behavior under conditions of delayed reinforcement: I. CRF type-schedules. J Exp Anal Behav 3:221–234Google Scholar
  9. Dews PB (1962) The effect of multiple SD periods on responding on a fixed-interval schedule. J Exp Anal Behav 5:369–374Google Scholar
  10. Dews PB, Wenger GR (1977) Rate-dependency of the behavioral effects of amphetamine. In: Thompson CT, Dews PB (eds) Advances in behavioral pharmacology, vol 1. Academic Press, New York, pp 167–227Google Scholar
  11. Dixon WJ, Brown MB, Engleman L, Frane JW, Hill MA, Jennrich RI, Toporek JD (1981) BMDP Statistical Software. University of California Press, Berkeley, CaliforniaGoogle Scholar
  12. Ettenberg A, Koob GF, Bloom FE (1981) Response artifact in the measurement of neuroleptic-induced anhedonia. Science 213:357–359Google Scholar
  13. Evenden JL, Robbins TW (1985) Effects of d-amphetamine, chlordiazepoxide and alpha-flupenthixol on food-reinforced tracking of a visual stimulus by rats. Psychopharmacology 85:361–366Google Scholar
  14. Ferster CB, Hammer C (1965) Variables determining the effects of delay of reinforcement. J Exp Anal Behav 8:243–254Google Scholar
  15. Fog R (1969) Stereotyped and non-stereotyped behavior in rats induced by various stimulant drugs. Psychopharmacology 14:299–304Google Scholar
  16. Goodman LS, Gilman A (1985) The pharmacological basis of therapeutics, 7th edn. Macmillan, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  17. Grilly DM (1977) Rate-dependent effects of amphetamine resulting from behavioral competition. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 1:87–93Google Scholar
  18. Hill RT (1970) Facilitation of conditioned reinforcement as a mechanism of psychomotor stimulation. In: Costa E, Garattini S (eds) Amphetamines and related compounds. Raven Press, New York, pp 781–795Google Scholar
  19. Iversen IH (1982) Contiguity and response generalization. Behav Anal Lett 2:185Google Scholar
  20. Iversen IH, Mogenson J (1984) d-Amphetamine affects one-trial appetitive conditioning in rats. Paper presented at the 10th Annual Convention of the Association for Behavior AnalysisGoogle Scholar
  21. Kelleher RT, Morse WH (1968a) Determinants of the specificity of behavioral effects of drugs. Ergeb Physiol 60:1–56Google Scholar
  22. Kelleher RT, Morse WH (1968b) Schedules using noxious stimuli. J Exp Anal Behav 11:819–838Google Scholar
  23. Lyon M, Robbins TW (1975) The action of central nervous system stimulant drugs: a general theory concerning amphetamine effects. In: Essman W, Valzelli L (eds) Current developments in psychopharmacology. Spectrum, New York, pp 79–163Google Scholar
  24. Pechnick R, Janowsky DS, Judd L (1979) Differential effects of methylphenidate and d-amphetamine in the rat. Psychopharmacology 65:311–315Google Scholar
  25. Randrup A, Munkvad I (1967) Stereotyped activities produced by amphetamine in several animal species and man. Psychopharmacologia 11:300–311Google Scholar
  26. Rutter M (1983) Developmental neuropsychiatry. Churchill Livingstone, EdinburghGoogle Scholar
  27. Sagvolden T, Jenssen JR, Brorson IW (1983) Rate-dependent effects of methylphenidate (Ritalin) on fixed-interval behavior in rats. Scand J Psychol 24:231–236Google Scholar
  28. Sanger DJ, Blackman DE (1976) Rate-dependent effects of drugs: a review of the literature. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 4:73–83Google Scholar
  29. Schwartz B (1980) Development of complex, stereotyped behavior in pigeons. J Exp Anal Behav 33:153–166Google Scholar
  30. Schwartz B (1982) Failure to produce response variability with reinforcement. J Exp Anal Behav 37:171–181Google Scholar
  31. Stein L (1964) Amphetamine and neural reward mechanisms. In: Steinberg H, de Reuck AVS, Knight J (eds) Animal behavior and drug action. Churchill, London, pp 91–118Google Scholar
  32. Stretch R, Skinner N (1967) Methylphenidate and stimulus control of avoidance behavior. J Exp Anal Behav 16:89–104Google Scholar
  33. Vogel R, Annau Z (1973) An operant discrimination task allowing variability of reinforced response patterning. J Exp Anal Behav 20:1–20Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer-Verlag 1988

Authors and Affiliations

  • Terje Sagvolden
    • 1
  • Knut Sl»tta
    • 1
  • Erik Arntzen
    • 1
  1. 1.Institute of NeurophysiologyUniversity of OsloOslo 1Norway

Personalised recommendations