Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology

, Volume 33, Issue 6, pp 355–370 | Cite as

The erect ‘penis’ is a flag of submission in a female-dominated society: greetings in Serengeti spotted hyenas

  • Marion L. East
  • Heribert Hofer
  • Wolfgang Wickler
Article

Abstract

In East Africa, spotted hyenas live in large clans in a highly structured society dominated by females. A clan is a fission-fusion society where members are often solitary or in small groups. Spotted hyenas have a ritualized greeting during which two individuals stand parallel and face in opposite directions. Both individuals usually lift their hind leg and sniff or lick the anogenital region of the other. The unique aspect of greetings between individuals is the prominent role of the erect ‘penis’ in animals of both sex. Female spotted hyenas have fused outer labiae and a ‘pseudo-penis’ formed by the clitoris which closely resembles the male penis and can be erected. During greetings subordinates signalled submission with gestures which were not necessarily reciprocated by the dominant participant. Asymmetries were most pronounced in greetings between adult females where the probability of asymmetries increased with the divergence in rank between partners. Greetings between adult females and males were uncommon and restricted to males above median rank, principally the alpha male. Models of primate affiliative behavior assume that benefits derived from social relationships with different individuals are not equal and that individuals are selected to maximize the benefits they receive from social relationships with others. The observed distribution of greetings between partners of different rank matched the predictions of these models. An examination of non-adaptive hypotheses on the evolution of the ‘pseudo-penis’ demonstrated that the conventional scenario linking (initial) virilization of female genitalia with selection for female dominance does not explain either the initial virilization, nor the evolution of the ‘pseudo-penis’ to its current form and use. We sketch a new scenario that links (1) initial virilization to the occurrence of neonatal siblicide amongst members of a twin litter, and (2) costs of maintenance, ‘pseudo-penile’ control over copulation and male submission. Our analysis confirms previous adaptive hypotheses on the function of the ‘pseudo-penis’ in greetings and suggests new hypotheses to account for hitherto unexplained features.

Key words

Hyena communication Female-dominated Penis submission 

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. Alberts SC, Sapolsky RM, Altmann J (1992) Behavioural, endocrine and immunological correlates of immigration by an aggressive male into a natural primate group. Horm Behav 26:167–178PubMedGoogle Scholar
  2. Armitage KB (1962) Social behavior of a colony of the yellow-bellied marmot (Marmota flaviventris). Anim Behav 10:319–331Google Scholar
  3. Anthoney TR (1968) The ontogeny of greeting, grooming, and sexual motor patterns in captive baboons (superspecies Papio cynocephalus). Behaviour 31:358–372Google Scholar
  4. Breedlove SM, Arnold AP (1980) Hormone accumulation in a sexually dimorphic motor nucleus of the rat spinal cord. Science 210:564–566Google Scholar
  5. Brown DH, Norris KS (1956) Observations of captive and wild cetaceans. J Mammal 37:311–326Google Scholar
  6. Buechner HK, Schloeth R (1965) Ceremonial mating behaviour in Ugandan kob (Adenota kob thomasi, Neumann). Z Tierpsychol 22:209–225Google Scholar
  7. Chapais B (1988) Rank maintenance in female Japanese macaques: Experimental evidence for social dependency. Behaviour 104:41–59Google Scholar
  8. Cheney DL, Seyfarth RM (1990) How monkeys see the world. University of Chicago Press, ChicagoGoogle Scholar
  9. Colmenares F (1990) Greeting behaviour in male baboons, I: Communication, reciprocity and symmetry. Behaviour 113:81–114Google Scholar
  10. Conover WJ (1980) Practical non-parametric statistics. 2nd ed, John Wiley, ChichesterGoogle Scholar
  11. Datta SB (1983) Relative power and the maintenance of dominance. In: Hinde RA (ed) Primate social relationships: an integrated approach. Blackwell Scientific, Oxford, pp 103–112Google Scholar
  12. Douglas-Hamilton I (1972) On the ecology and behaviour of the African elephant: the elephants of Lake Manyara. DPhil thesis, Oxford UniversityGoogle Scholar
  13. Dunbar RIM (1980) Determinants and evolutionary consequences of dominance among female gelada baboons. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 7:253–265Google Scholar
  14. East ML, Hofer H (1991a) Loud calling in a female-dominated mammalian society: I. Structure and composition of whooping bouts of spotted hyaenas, (Crocuta Crocuta). Anim Behav 42:637–649Google Scholar
  15. East ML, Hofer H (1991b) Loud calling in a female-dominated mammalian society: II. Behavioural contexts and functions of whooping of spotted hyaenas, (Crocuta crocuta). Anim Behav 42:651–669Google Scholar
  16. East ML, Hofer H, Türk A (1989) Funcitons of birth dens in spotted hyaenas (Crocuta crocuta). J Zool Lond 219:690–697Google Scholar
  17. Eisermann K (1992) Long-term heart rate response to social stress in wild European rabbits: predominant effect of rank position. Physiol Behav 52:33–36Google Scholar
  18. Estes RD (1991) The behavior guide to African mammals. University of California Press, BerkeleyGoogle Scholar
  19. Fairbanks LA (1980) Relationships among adult females in captive vervet monkeys: Testing a model of rank related attractiveness. Anim Behav 28:853–859PubMedGoogle Scholar
  20. Forger NG, Breedlove SM (1986) Sexual dimorphism in human and canine spinal cord: role of early androgen. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 83:7527–7531Google Scholar
  21. Fox MW (1971a) Ontogeny of a social display in Hyaena hyaena: anal protrusion. J Mammal 52:467–469Google Scholar
  22. Fox MW (1971b) Socio-infantile and socio-sexual signals in canids: a comparative and ontogenetic study. Z Tierpsychol 28:185–210Google Scholar
  23. Frank LG (1986a) Social organisation of the spotted hyaena (Crocuta crocuta). I. Demography. Anim Behav 35:1500–1509Google Scholar
  24. Frank LG (1986b) Social organisation of the spotted hyaena (Crocuta Crocuta). II. Dominance and reproduction. Anim Behav 34:1510–1527Google Scholar
  25. Frank LG, Davidson JM, Smith ER (1985) Androgen levels in the spotted hyaena Crocuta Crocuta: the influence of social factors. J Zool London A206:525–531Google Scholar
  26. Frank LG, Glickman SE, Zabel CJ (1989) Ontogeny of female dominance in the spotted hyaena: perspectives from nature and captivity. Symp Zool Soc London 61:127–146Google Scholar
  27. Frank LG, Glickman SE, Powch I (1990) Sexual dimorphism in the spotted hyaena (Crocouta Crocuta). J Zool London 221:308–313Google Scholar
  28. Frank LG, Glickman SE, Licht P (1991) Fatal sibling aggression, precocial development and androgens in neonatal spotted hyaenas. Science 252:702–704Google Scholar
  29. Gittleman JL, Oftedal OT (1987) Comparative growth and lactation energetics in carnivores. Symp Zool See London 57:41–77Google Scholar
  30. Glickman SE, Frank LG, Davidson JM, Smith ER, Siiteri PK (1987) Androstenedione may organize or activate sex-reversed traits in female spotted hyaena. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 84:3444–3447Google Scholar
  31. Glickman SE, Frank LG, Licht P, Yalcinkaya T, Siiteri PK, Davidson J (1992a) Sexual differentiation of the female spotted hyaena. Ann N Y Acad Sci 662:135–159Google Scholar
  32. Glickman SE, Frank LG, Pavgi S, Licht P (1992b) Hormonal correlates of ‘masculinization’ in female spotted hyaenas (Crocuta crocuta). 1. Infancy to sexual maturity. J Reprod Fert 95:451–462Google Scholar
  33. Gould SJ (1981) Hyena myths and realities. Nat Hist 90:16–24Google Scholar
  34. Gould SJ, Lewontin RC (1979) The spandrels of San Marco and the Panglossian paradigm: a critique of the adaptionist program. Proc R Soc London B205:581–598Google Scholar
  35. Gould SJ, Vrba ES (1982) Exaptation — a missing term in the science of form. Paleobiology 8:4–15Google Scholar
  36. Hall KRL (1967) Social interactions of the adult male and adult females of a patas monkey group. In: Altmann SA (ed) Social communication among primates. University of Chicago Press, Chicago, pp 261–280Google Scholar
  37. Hamilton WJ III, Tilson RL, Frank LG (1986) Sexual monomorphism in spotted hyaenas, Crocuta crocuta. Ethology 71:63–73Google Scholar
  38. Harper DGC (1991) Communication. In: Krebs JR, Davies NB (eds) Behavioural ecology: an evolutionary approach. Blackwell Scientific, Oxford, pp 374–397Google Scholar
  39. Harvey PH, Martin RD, Clutton-Brock TH (1987) Life histories in comparative perspectives. In: Smuts BB, Cheney DL, Seyfarth RM, Wrangham RW, Struhsaker TT (eds) Primate societies. University of Chicago Press, Chicago, pp 181–196Google Scholar
  40. Hofer H, East ML (1992) Siblicide of spotted hyaenas is a consequence of allocation of maternal effort and a cause of skewed litter sex ratios. Abstracts. The Fourth International Behavioural Ecology Congress, Princeton University, Princeton 1992, p 119Google Scholar
  41. Hofer H, East ML (1993a) The commuting system of Serengeti spotted hyaenas: how a predator copes with migratory prey. I. Social organization. Anim Behav 46:547–557Google Scholar
  42. Hofer H, East ML (1993b) The commuting system of Serengeti spotted hyaenas: how a predator copes with migratory prey. II. Intrusion pressure and commuters' space use. Anim Behav 46:559–574Google Scholar
  43. Hofer H, East ML (1993c) The commuting system of Serengeti spotted hyaenas: how a predator copes with migratory prey. III. Attendance and maternal care. Anim Behav 46:575–589Google Scholar
  44. Holekamp KE, Smale L (1991) Dominance acquisition during mammalian social development: The “inheritance” of maternal rank. Am Zool 31: 306–317Google Scholar
  45. Hrdy SB (1981) The woman that never evolved. Harvard University Press, CambridgeGoogle Scholar
  46. Jaarsveld AS van, Skinner JD (1987) Spotted hyaena monomorphism: an adaptive ‘phallusy’? S Afr J Sci 83:612–615Google Scholar
  47. Jaarsveld AS van, Skinner JD (1991) Plasma androgens in spotted hyaenas (Crocuta crocuta): influence of social and reproductive development. J Reprod Fert 93:195–201Google Scholar
  48. Jolly A (1972) The evolution of primate behaviour. Macmillan, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  49. Jolly A (1984) The puzzle of female feeding priority. In: Small M (ed) Female primates: studies by women primatologists. Alan R. Liss, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  50. Kano T (1992) The last ape. Pygmy chimpanzee behavior and ecology. Stanford Unviersity Press, StanfordGoogle Scholar
  51. Kaplan JR, Zucker E (1980) Social organization of a group of free-ranging Patas monkeys. Folia Primatol 34:196–213Google Scholar
  52. Kappeler PM (1990) Female dominance in Lemur catta: more than just female feeding priority? Folia Primatol 55:92–103Google Scholar
  53. Kiecolt-Glaser JK, Glaser R (1991) Stress and immune function in humans. In: Ader R, Felten DL, Cohen N (eds) Psychoneuroimmunology, 2nd edn. Academic Press, New York, pp 849–867Google Scholar
  54. Klingel H (1972) Social behaviour of African Equidae. Zool Afr 7:175–185Google Scholar
  55. Kruuk H (1972) The spotted hyaena. University of Chicago Press, ChicagoGoogle Scholar
  56. Kühme W (1965a) Communal food distribution and division of labour in African hunting dogs. Nature 205:443–444Google Scholar
  57. Kühme W (1956b) Freilandstudien zur Soziologie des Hyänen-hundes (Lycaon pictus lupinus Thomas 1902). Z Tierpsychol 22:495–541Google Scholar
  58. Kummer H (1968) Social organisation of hamadryas baboons; a field study (Bibl Primatol 6). Karger, BaselGoogle Scholar
  59. Leyhausen P (1960) Verhaltensstudien an Katzen. Z Tierpsychol Beih 2:1–120PubMedGoogle Scholar
  60. Licht P, Frank LG, Pavgi S, Yalcinkaya TM, Siiteri PK, Glickman SE (1992) Hormonal correlates of ‘masculinization’ in female spotted hyaenas (Crocuta crocuta) 2. Maternal and fetal steriods. J Reprod Fert 95:463–474Google Scholar
  61. Lindeque M, Skinner JD (1982) Fetal androgens and sexual mimicry in spotted hyaena (Crocuta crocuta). J Reprod Fert 65:405–410Google Scholar
  62. Lindeque M, Skinner JD, Millar RP (1986) Adrenal and gonadal contribution to circulating androgens in spotted hyaenas (Crocuta crocuta) as revealed by LHRH, hCG and ACTH stimulation. J Reprod Fert 78:211–217Google Scholar
  63. Machlis L, Dodd PW, Fentress JC (1985) The pooling fallacy: problems arising when individuals contribute more than one observation to the data set. Z Tierpsychol 69:201–214Google Scholar
  64. Matthews LH (1935) The oestrous cycle and intersexuality in the female mole Talpa europaea Linn. Proc Zool Soc Lond 106:347–383Google Scholar
  65. Matthews LH (1939) Reproduction in the spotted hyaena (Crocuta crocuta, Erxleben). Philos Trans R Soc Lond B230:1–78Google Scholar
  66. Maynard Smith J (1982) Evolution and the theory of games. Cambridge University Press, CambridgeGoogle Scholar
  67. Mech LD (1970) The wolf. University of Minnesota Press, MinneapolisGoogle Scholar
  68. Mehlman PT, Chapais B (1988) Differential effects of kinship, dominance, and the mating season on female allogrooming in a captive group of Macaca fuscata. Primates 29:195–217Google Scholar
  69. Mills MGL (1985) Related spotted hyaenas forage together but do not cooperate in rearing young. Nature 316:61–62Google Scholar
  70. Mills MGL (1990) Kalahari hyaenas. The comparative behavioural ecology of two species. Unwin Hyman, LondonGoogle Scholar
  71. Mills MGL, Gorman ML, Mills MEJ (1980) The scent-marking behaviour of the brown hyaena Hyaena brunnea. S Afr J Zool 15:240–248PubMedGoogle Scholar
  72. Munck A, Guyre PM (1991) Glucocorticoids and immune function. In: Ader R, Felten DL, Cohen N (eds) Psychoneuroimmunology, 2nd edn. Academic Press, New York, pp 447–474Google Scholar
  73. Neaves WB, Griffin JE, Wilson JD (1980) Sexual dimorphism of the phallus in spotted hyaena (Crocuta crocuta). J Reprod Fert 59:509–513Google Scholar
  74. Niethammer J (1990) Talpa europaea Linnaeus, 1758 — Maulwurf. In: Niethammer J, Krapp F (eds) Handbuch der Säugetiere Europas 3/I: Insektenfresser, Primaten. Aula, Wiesbaden, pp 99–133Google Scholar
  75. Nishida T (1970) Social behaviour and relationships among wild chimpanzees of the Mahale Mountains. Primates 11:47–87Google Scholar
  76. Oppenheimer JR (1977) Communication in New World monkeys. In: Sebeok TA (ed) How animals communicate. Indiana University Press, Bloomington, pp 851–889Google Scholar
  77. Ploog DW (1967) The behaviour of squirrel monkeys (Saimiri sciureus) as revealed by sociometry, bioacoustics, and brain stimulation. In: Altmann SA (ed) Social communication among primates. University of Chicago Press, Chicago, pp 149–184Google Scholar
  78. Ploog DW, MacLean PD (1963) Display of penile erection in squirrel monkeys (Saimiri sciureus). Anim Behav 11: 32–39Google Scholar
  79. Racey PA, Skinner JD (1979) Endocrine aspects of sexual mimicry in spotted hyaenas (Crocuta crocuta). J Zool London 187:315–326Google Scholar
  80. Rails K (1976) Mammals in which females are larger than males. Q Rev Biol 51:245–276Google Scholar
  81. Richard AF (1987) Malagasy prosimians: female dominance. In: Smuts BB, Cheney DL, Seyfarth RM, Wrangham RW, Struhsaker TT (eds) Primate societies. University of Chicago Press, Chicago, pp 25–33Google Scholar
  82. Robinson JG, Wright PC, Kinzey WG (1987) Monogamous cebids and their relatives: intergroup calls and spacing. In: Smuts BB, Cheney DL, Seyfarth RM, Wrangham RW, Struhsaker TT (eds) Primate societies. University of Chicago Press, Chicago, pp 44–53Google Scholar
  83. Rowell TH (1974) The concept of social dominance. Behav Biol 11:131–154Google Scholar
  84. Sapolsky RM (1982) The endocrine stress-response and social status in the wild baboon. Horm Behav 15:279–292Google Scholar
  85. Schenkel R (1967) Submission: its features and functions in the wolf and dog. Am Zool 7:319–330Google Scholar
  86. Seyfarth RM (1977) A model of social grooming among adult female monkeys. J Theor Biol 65:671–698Google Scholar
  87. Seyfarth RM (1983) Grooming and social competition in primates. In: Hinde RA (ed) Primate social relationships: an integrated approach. Blackwell Scientific Press, London, pp 182–190Google Scholar
  88. Seyfarth RM, Cheney DL (1984) Grooming, alliances and reciprocal altruism in vervet monkeys. Nature 308:541–543Google Scholar
  89. Sherman PW (1988) The levels of analysis. Anim Behav 36:616–618Google Scholar
  90. Silk JB, Samuels A, Rodman P (1981) The influence of kinship, rank, and sex on affiliation and aggression between adult female and immature bonnet macaques Macaca radiata. Behaviour 78:111–177Google Scholar
  91. Smuts BB (1987) Gender, aggression, and influence. In: Smuts BB, Cheney DL, Seyfarth RM, Wrangham RW, Struhsaker TT (eds) Primate societies. University of Chicago Press, Chicago, pp 400–412Google Scholar
  92. Stammbach E (1978) On social differentiation in groups of captive female hamadryas baboons. Behaviour 67:322–338Google Scholar
  93. Steiner AL (1975) ‘Greeting’ behaviour in some Sciuridae from ontogenetic, evolutionary and socio-behavioural perspective. Nat Can 120:737–751Google Scholar
  94. Sugiyama K (1971) Characteristics of the social life of bonnet macaques (Macaca radiata). Primates 12:247–266Google Scholar
  95. Waal FBM de (1982) Chimpanzee politics. Jonathan Cape, LondonGoogle Scholar
  96. Waal FBM de (1988) The communicative repertoire of captive bonobos (Pan paniscus) compared to that of chimpanzees. Behaviour 106:183–251Google Scholar
  97. Weigel RM (1979) The facial expression of the brown capuchin monkey (Cebus apella). Behaviour 68:250–276Google Scholar
  98. Wickler W (1964) Vom Gruppenleben einiger Säugetiere Afrikas. Mitt Max-Planck-Ges 56:296–309Google Scholar
  99. Wickler W (1965a) Die äußeren Genitalien als soziale Signale bei einigen Primaten. Naturwissenschaften 52:269–270Google Scholar
  100. Wickler W (1965b) Die Evolution von Mustern der Zeichnung und des Verhaltens. Naturwissenschaften 52:335–341Google Scholar
  101. Wickler W (1966) Ursprung und biologische Deutung des Genitalpräsentierens männlicher Primaten. Z Tierpsychol 23:422–437Google Scholar
  102. Wickler W (1967) Socio-sexual signals and their intra-specific imitation among primates. In: Morris D (ed) Primate ethology. Weidenfeld & Nicholson, London, pp 69–147Google Scholar
  103. Wilkinson L (1990) SYSTAT: the system for statistics. SYSTAT, Evanston, IllinoisGoogle Scholar
  104. Wurster DH, Benirschke K, Gray WC (1970) Determination of sex in the spotted hyaena Crocuta crocuta. Int Zoo Yrbk 10:143–144Google Scholar
  105. Yost R (1977) Cytological sex determination in the spotted hyaena Crocuta crocuta. Int Zoo Yrbk 17:212–213Google Scholar
  106. Young AL, Richard AF, Aiello LC (1990) Female dominance and maternal investment in strepsirrhine primates. Am Nat 135:473–488Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer-Verlag 1993

Authors and Affiliations

  • Marion L. East
    • 1
  • Heribert Hofer
    • 1
  • Wolfgang Wickler
    • 1
  1. 1.Max-Planck-Institut fur VerhaltensphysiologieSeewiesen Post StarnbergGermany

Personalised recommendations