Advertisement

Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology

, Volume 27, Issue 4, pp 229–237 | Cite as

The evolution of marmot sociality: I. Why disperse late?

  • Walter Arnold 
Article

Summary

Prolonged toleration of offspring in marmots was hypothesized to be (1) a means of preventing dispersal of undersized young (Barash 1974 a) or more generally (2) continued parental investment, increasing the probability of descendant survival and reproduction (Armitage 1981, 1987). These hypotheses are tested in this paper for one of the most social of marmot species, the alpine marmot. The animals studied lived in groups within territories defended by a dominant male and female, or as floaters lacking a well-defined home range. Offspring did not disperse before sexual maturity at age 2 (Fig. 1). Only territorial females bred, whereas territorial males were not able to monopolize reproduction likewise (Table 2). Dispersers had similar spring mass to nondispersers (Table 4). Hence, hypothesis 1 is not supported, at least not for adult-sized, > 2 years old animals. During their residency, 19% of subordinates obtained their natal territory or a neighboring one (Fig. 2). Long distance dispersal bore a high mortality risk. Thus, toleration of mature offspring could well represent parental investment. Other results, however, suggest additional influences on the timing of dispersal. (i) Males dispersed later than females (Fig. 3), possibly because of mate sharing by territorial males (see Emlen 1982). (ii) The higher mass loss of dispersers during the previous winter indicates that weak animals were forced to leave (Table 5) despite presumably lower chances of becoming territorial (Table 3). (iii) Subordinate animals which could not be the offspring of both territorials present were not more likely to disperse (Fig. 3). (iv) Lower dispersal rates when immatures lived in the group (Fig. 3) may indicate benefits from the subordinates' presence for rearing young.

Keywords

Home Range Parental Investment Dominant Male Territorial Male High Mortality Risk 
These keywords were added by machine and not by the authors. This process is experimental and the keywords may be updated as the learning algorithm improves.

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. Armitage KB (1981) Sociality as a life-history tactic of ground squirrels. Oecologia 48:36–49Google Scholar
  2. Armitage KB (1986) Marmot polygyny revisited: determinants of male and female reproductive strategies. In: Rubenstein DS, Wrangham RW (eds) Ecological aspects of social evolution. Princeton University Press, New Jersey, pp 303–331Google Scholar
  3. Armitage KB (1987) Social dynamics of mammals: reproductive success, kinship and individual fitness. Trends Ecol Evol 2: 279–283Google Scholar
  4. Arnold W (1986) Sozioökologie des Alpenmurmeltieres. Dissertation, Ludwig-Maximilians-Universität MünchenGoogle Scholar
  5. Arnold W (1990) The evolution of marmot sociality: II. Costs and benefits of joint hibernation. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 27:239–246Google Scholar
  6. Barash DP (1973) The social biology of the olympic marmot. Anim Behav Monogr 6:171–249Google Scholar
  7. Barash DP (1974a) The evolution of marmot societies: a general theory. Science 185:415–420Google Scholar
  8. Barash DP (1974b) The social behaviour of the hoary marmot (Marmota caligata). Anim Behav 22:256–261Google Scholar
  9. Barash DP (1976) Social behaviour and individual differences in free-living alpine marmots (Marmota marmota). Anim Behav 24:27–35Google Scholar
  10. Berendyaev SA, Kulkova NA (1965) On intraspecies relations of grey marmots. Zool Zh 44:110–116Google Scholar
  11. Bibikow DI (1968) Die Murmeltiere. Die neue Brehm Bücherei. Ziemsen, StuttgartGoogle Scholar
  12. Bronson FH (1963) Some correlates of interaction rate in natural populations of woodchucks. Ecology 44:637–643Google Scholar
  13. Bronson FH (1964) Agonistic behaviour in woodchucks. Anim Behav 12:470–478Google Scholar
  14. Brooker MG, Rowley I, Adams M, Baverstock PR (1990) Promiscuity: an inbreeding avoidance mechanism in a socially monogamous species? Behav Ecol Sociobiol 26:191–199Google Scholar
  15. Clayton JW, Tretiak DN (1972) Amine-citrate buffers for pH control in starch gel electrophoresis. J Fish Res Board Can 29:1169–1172Google Scholar
  16. Davis DE (1981) Mechanism for decline in a woodchuck population. J Wildl Manage 45:658–668Google Scholar
  17. Emlen ST (1982) The evolution of helping: 2. The role of behavioral conflict. Am Nat 119:40–53Google Scholar
  18. Faulkes CG, Abbott DH (1990) Investigation of genetic diversity in wild colonies of naked mole-rats by DNA fingerprinting. J Zool (London) 221:87–97Google Scholar
  19. Ferron J, Ouellet JP (1989) Temporal and intersexual variations in the use of space with regard to social organization in the woodchuck Marmota monax. Can J Zool 67:1642–1649Google Scholar
  20. Forter D (1975) Zur Okologie and Verbreitungsgeschichte des Alpenmurmeltieres im Berner Oberland. Dissertation, Universität BernGoogle Scholar
  21. Frase BA, Hoffmann RS (1980) Marmota flaviventris. Mammalian Species 135:1–8Google Scholar
  22. Grizzell RA (1955) A study of the southern woodchuck, Marmota monax monax. Am Midl Nat 53:257–293Google Scholar
  23. Harris H, Hopkinson DA (1976) Handbook of enzyme electrophoresis in human genetics. American Elsevier, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  24. Hembeck H (1958) Zum Paarungsverhalten der Murmeltiere. Z Jagdwiss 4:40–41PubMedGoogle Scholar
  25. Holmes WG (1984) The ecological basis of monogamy in Alaskan hoary marmots. In: Murie JO, Michener GR (eds) The biology of ground-dwelling squirrels. University of Nebraska Press, Lincoln, pp 250–274Google Scholar
  26. Hoogland JL (1982) Prairie dogs avoid extreme inbreeding. Science 215:1639–1641Google Scholar
  27. Hoogland JL (1985) Infanticide in prairie dogs: lactating females kill offspring of close kin. Science 230:1037–1040Google Scholar
  28. Hoogland JL, Foltz DW (1982) Variance in male and female reproductive success in a harem-polygynous mammal, the blacktailed prairie dog (Sciuridae: Cynomys ludovicianus). Behav Ecol Sociobiol 11:155–163Google Scholar
  29. Hottinger J (1989) Abwanderungsverhalten and dessen soziale Ursachen beim Alpenmurmeltier. Diplomarbeit, Technische Universität MünchenGoogle Scholar
  30. Huck UW, Quinn RP, Lisk RD (1985) Determinants of mating success in the golden hamster (Mesocricetus auratus) IV. Sperm competition. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 17:239–252Google Scholar
  31. Ismagilov MI (1956) Material on the ecology of the Tien-Shan marmot. (Marmota bobac centralis Thom.) Zool Zh 35:908–915Google Scholar
  32. Koenig L (1973) Marmota marmota (Sciuridae) Paarungsverhalten. Wolf G (ed) Encyclopaedia cinematographica E 459/1962. Institut für den wissenschaftl. Film, GöttingenGoogle Scholar
  33. Kratochvil J (1964) Das männliche Genitalsystem des europäischen Bergmurmeltieres Marmota marmota latirostris. Z Säugetierk 29:290–304PubMedGoogle Scholar
  34. Lattmann P (1973) Beiträge zur Ökologie und zum Verhalten des Alpenmurmeltieres (Marmota marmota). Ergebnisse der wissenschaftlichen Untersuchungen im Schweizerischen Nationalpark 11:273–347Google Scholar
  35. Lenti Boero D (1988) Distribuzione degli insediamenti e stima della densita' di marmotta alpine (Marmota marmota L.): metodi di indagine per zone campione. In:Atti del 1 Convegno dei Biologi della Selvaggina. Supplemento alle Ricerche di Biologia della Selvaggina, vol 14, Tipolitografia F.G., Savignano sul Panoro, Modena, pp 253–264Google Scholar
  36. Lenti Boero D, Boero E (1989) Dati preliminari sulla dispersione e predazione nella marmotta alpine (Marmota marmota L.): uno studio a lungo termine. In: Atti dell'8 Congresso della Divisione Ricerca di Base in Psicologia. Imprimatur, Padova, pp 57–59Google Scholar
  37. Markert CL, Faulhaber I (1965) Lactate dehydrogenase isozyme patterns of fish. J Exp Zool 159:319–332Google Scholar
  38. Maschkin VI (1982) New materials on ecology of Marmota menzbieri (Rodentia, Sciuridae). Zool Zh 61:278–288Google Scholar
  39. May B, Wright JE, Stoneking M (1979) Joint segregation of biochemical loci in salmonidae: results from experiments with Salvelinus and review of the literature on other species. J Fish Res Board Can 36:1114–1126Google Scholar
  40. Michener GR (1983) Kin identification, matriarchies, and the evolution of sociality in ground-dwelling sciurids. In: Eisenberg JF, Klieman DG (eds) Advances in the study of mammalian behavior. American Society of Mammalogists, Shippenburg, PA, Spec Publ, No 7, pp 528–572Google Scholar
  41. Müller-Using D (1957) Die Paarungsbiologie des Murmeltieres. Z Jagdwiss 3:24–28Google Scholar
  42. Naef-Daenzer B (1984) Sozialverhalten and räumliche Organisation von Alpenmurmelteren. Dissertation, Universität BernGoogle Scholar
  43. Ouellet JP, Ferron J (1986) L'utilisation de l'espace par la marmotte commune (Marmota monax). Naturaliste can (Rev Ecol Syst) 113:263–273Google Scholar
  44. Psenner H (1957) Neues vom Murmeltier, Marmota m. marmota (Linne, 1758). Saugetierk Mitt 5:4–10Google Scholar
  45. Psenner H (1960) Welche Erkenntnisse brachten die Murmeltierforschungen in den letzten Jahren. Proc Int Symp Methods Mamm Invest: 259–263Google Scholar
  46. Ratti P (1970) Beitrag zur Kenntnis des Alpenmurmeltieres (Marmota m. marmota). Schweizer Archiv für Tierheilkunde 112/6: 283–295Google Scholar
  47. Ridgeway GJ, Sherburne SW, Lewis RD (1970) Polymorphism in the esterase of atlantic herring. Trans Am Fish Soc 99:147–151Google Scholar
  48. Schwartz OA, Armitage KB (1980) Genetic variation in social mammals: the marmot model. Science 207:665–667Google Scholar
  49. Selander RK, Smith MH, Yang SY, Johnson WE, Gentry JB (1971) IV Biochemical polymorphism and systematics in the genus Peromyscus. I. Variation in the old-field mouse (Peromyscus polionotus). Studies in Genetics VI. Univ Texas Publ 7103:49–90Google Scholar
  50. Sokal RR, Rohlf FJ (1981) Biometry. Freeman, San Francisco Suntzov VV (1981) The territorial structure of the population and intraspecies relations of mongolian bobaks (Marmota sibirica) in Tuva. Zool Zh 60:1394–1405Google Scholar
  51. Trivers RL (1972) Parental investment and sexual selection. In: Campbell BG (ed) Sexual selection and the descent of man. Heinemann Educational Books, London, pp 136–179Google Scholar
  52. Zelenka G (1965) Observations sur l'ecologie de la marmotte des alpes. La Terre et la Vie 19:238–256Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer-Verlag 1990

Authors and Affiliations

  • Walter Arnold 
    • 1
  1. 1.Max-Planck-Institut für VerhaltensphysiologieAbteilung WicklerSeewiesenFederal Republic of Germany

Personalised recommendations