Animals and Dosing
Timed-pregnant Long-Evans hooded rats (Charles River Laboratory, Raleigh, NC) were obtained (four shipments over 6 months) on gestational day (GD) 4 and individually housed in ventilated cages (Techniplast, West Chester, PA) with autoclaved, hardwood bedding (PJ Murphy, Montville, NY) within a semi-barrier room (40–60% humidity; 12-h light/dark cycle, 6:00–18:00; 20–24 °C). Dams were randomly assigned to exposure group. Two drinking water control groups were maintained on reverse osmosis drinking water (RO-H2O). One group (G1) was maintained on a standard rodent chow (Teklad 2918; Envigo, Madison, WI) and the second (G2) was maintained on a low-fluoride chow (Teklad Custom Diet TD.160173). To examine the effect of fluoride in the drinking water while controlling for dietary fluoride, rats were exposed to the low-fluoride chow and water supplemented with either 10 ppm F− (G3) or 20 ppm F− (G4). Dosing solutions were prepared fresh weekly with sodium fluoride (NaF; lot no. X0044851; 99.9%; Materion, Milwaukee, WI). Fluoride levels in drinking water were confirmed [RO-H2O, < 0.2 ppm; 10 and 20 ppm F−within ≤ 5% of target] (analytical method: EPA 300.0; Pace Analytical, Huntersville, NC). The Teklad 2918 diet contained 20.5 ppm F− and the TD.160173 custom diet contained 3.24 ppm F− (Official Methods of Analysis Methods 944.08 and 978.03; AOAC International, Gaithersburg, MD; Covance, Madison, WI). Food and water were available ad libitum. Exposure to the dams began on GD6 and continued throughout lactation. Pups were allowed free access to drinking water, beginning consumption around PND14, and continued on the same level of exposure after weaning until study termination. All animal procedures were conducted in accordance with protocols approved by the NIEHS Animal Care and Use Committee within AAALAC approved animal facilities.
The day of birth, postnatal day (PND) 0, occurred within 12 h with similar pup numbers across groups. On PND4, pups within each group were cross-fostered to establish litters of ten pups (six male and four female pups). Male pups were toe-tattooed (BD PrecisionGlide 27G needle) and randomly assigned to four behavioral testing groups (cohorts) ensuring only one male pup per gestational and postnatal litter assigned for any one endpoint. [Cohort 1: running wheel (RW; PND24), elevated plus maze (EPM; PND30), passive avoidance (PA; PND55), hot plate; Cohort 2: EPM (PND29); Y-maze (PND38); Cohort 3: motor activity (MA; PND40), light/dark place preference (L/D; PND43), Morris water maze (MWM; PND60); Cohort 4: pre-pulse startle inhibition (PPI; PND61–63), adult EPM (PND70)]. Excess male pups were identified as unassigned. Female pups were not used. Age of eye-opening for both eyes was similar across groups. Male pups weaned on PND21 were group-housed 2–3 per cage depending on the final terminal age on study to adhere to housing guidelines. When examined as adults, (> PND90), rats in the 20 ppm F− dose group showed evidence of mild fluorosis (Supplementary Fig. S1) similar to that reported by Catani et al. (2010) after 78 days of 25 ppm F− exposure.
Behavioral Testing
Handling of rats for behavioral testing followed NTP guidelines for neurobehavioral testing (NTP 2015). All testing was conducted between 10:00–15:00 h. Assignment of rats to testing apparatus and time of testing was counterbalanced. Body weights were similar across groups prior to the start of testing for PA, PPI, or MWM (Supplementary Table S1). Accurate camera tracking by Ethovision XT 11.5 (Noldus, Wageningen, the Netherlands) was confirmed for each rat and incorrect points were edited and corrected per Noldus manual.
Running Wheel Activity
PND24 rats were transferred to testing room and allowed to acclimate under identical housing conditions for 24 h. Individual rats were transferred to a filter-top cage (396 × 215 × 172 mm) with a stainless steel RW (Mini-Mitter®; Respironics Co., Bend, OR) that limited light levels to 91 and 8 lm for light/dark periods, respectively. Computer-assisted recording of wheel revolutions in 15-min epochs (Vital View Data Acquisition, Respironics Co.) were collected over dark (18:00–6:00 EST) and light (6:00–18:00 EST) phases. Original food and water exposure was maintained. A linear mixed-effects repeated measures ANOVA (RM ANOVA) was used to analyze total daily RW rotations for the dark and light phases.
Elevated Plus Maze
To assess exploratory activity and anxiety-related performance (Walf and Frye 2007), naïve rats (PND30), rats with prior RW experience (PND31), and a separate group of naïve adult rats (PND72) were assessed in the EPM (a dark plexiglass apparatus: central area (10 × 10 cm), two opposing open arms (50 × 10 cm), two opposing enclosed arms (50 × 10 × 40cm)) elevated 50 cm above a dark floor. Each rat was placed in the central area facing an open arm and allowed 7 min to freely explore. Based on previous reports of a diminished EPM “cautious response” in fluoride exposed rats (Bartos et al. 2015), an additional testing paradigm was used to examine PND30 rats of G2 and G4. The paradigm was modified to include a linear-shadow cast over the maze as representative of a predator. Behavior was video-captured (Ethovision XT 11.5 Applications Manual) and the number of entries, percent time spent, and total distance traveled in arms were recorded. Wilcoxon Rank Sum tests were used to analyze number of entries and duration. Student’s t tests were used to analyze distance traveled.
Locomotor Activity
On PND40, exploratory motor activity was measured in an open field chamber (42 × 42 cm; Columbus Instruments, Columbus, OH) outfitted with photocell detectors (0.32 cm diameter) spaced 5 cm from floor and 1.27 cm linearly apart around the chamber. Ambulatory activity was recorded in 5-min epochs over a 45-min test session. Total ambulatory activity, time spent in the margin (one-photocell width from the wall), and activity within the center area (20 × 20 cm) were recorded. Student’s t tests were used to analyze total ambulatory activity, total ambulatory time, total distance traveled, and ambulatory activity acclimation. Wilcoxon Rank Sum tests were used to analyze activity total margin time, margin distance, and center distance traveled. RM ANOVA was used to analyze ambulatory activity in 5-min epochs.
Light/Dark Place Preference
To examine exploratory activity and preference for the dark chamber, a PND43 rat was placed in the lighted side of a 2-sided plexiglass chamber (68 × 21 × 34 cm; with a clear and a dark chamber, 34 × 21 × 34 cm). For 5 min, entries into and total time spend in lighted side were video-captured (Ethovision XT) and analyzed by Wilcoxon Rank Sum tests.
Passive Avoidance
The ability of a rat to learn to withhold a normally preferred response was assessed using a Gemini Avoidance System (San Diego Instruments, San Diego, CA). On PND55, a rat was placed into the start chamber modified by a white covering on back and side walls with the gate closed. After 120 s, the house light and cue light were turned on and the gate raised. Upon crossing to the dark side, the gate closed and a 3-s 0.5-mA floor-grid shock was delivered. The rat was removed after 10 s. This sequence was repeated every 24 h. Response latency was recorded with a maximum of 300 s. RM ANOVA was used to analyze latency over sessions, excluding day 1. The relative change from the first trial to the last trial was analyzed by Kruskal-Wallis rank sum test and Wilcoxon rank sum test. The percent reaching maximum was analyzed by Fisher’s exact test.
Hot-Plate Latency
Forty-eight hours after cessation of PA, pain threshold was determined as the latency to respond (jump or link of hindpaw; 2-min cutoff) to being placed on a 55 °C hot-plate platform (IITC Life Science, Woodland Hills, CA). Latency was analyzed by Student’s t test.
Startle Response and Pre-Pulse Startle Inhibition
PND 61–62 rats were assessed for auditory startle response, habituation, and PPI as a measure of sensorimotor gating using a computer-assisted SR-LAB startle apparatus (San Diego Instruments). Background noise level was set at 65 dB. Following a 5-min habituation period, the session began with a 120-dB trial, followed by 5120 dB trials; 2 blocks of 31 trials [2 no-stimulus trials, 6 acoustic startle stimuli (40-msec null period followed by 40-msec 120 dB pulse) trials alone, 18 pre-pulse stimulus trials (40-msec null period followed by 20-msec pre-pulse of 68, 71, 77, and 80 dB followed by a 100-msec null period and a 40-msec 120-dB pulse; for an entire recording period of 200 msec) presented in a random order, followed by 120-dB trials. Trials were presented at 15 s variable inter-trial intervals (ITI; 5–25 s). Habituation was calculated as difference between first and the last block of 120-dB trials. Pre-pulse startle inhibition was calculated as a percentage of the median 120 dB startle response. Wilcoxon Rank Sum tests were used to analyze the first 120 dB (Vmax) responses and startle habituation. RM ANOVA was used to analyze 120-dB startle responses (Vmax). Pre-pulse startle inhibition was analyzed by two-way ANOVA with dose and pre-pulse type as factors. Negative PPI values were set to 0.
Morris Water Maze
PND60 rats were transferred to the testing room 24 h prior to testing and maintained under normal home-cage conditions. A circular plastic tank (183 × 62 cm) filled with opaque water to 52 cm (10 cm distance from the water surface to the lip of tank) was allowed to equilibrate to room temperature (25 °C). Water was changed every 2 days. For cued learning, days 1 and 2, white curtains surrounded the tank (NTP 2015) and a clear visible plexiglass platform (10-cm diameter; Stoelting; Wood Dale, IL) was placed within one quadrant of the pool, 36 cm from the tank wall and raised 1.5 cm above the water and identified with a flag. The rat was placed into the pool in the quadrant opposite the platform, facing the wall of the tank and allowed 90 s to find the platform. The rat remained on the platform for 20 s before removal. Three trials, 10 min ITI, were run daily for 2 days. The platform location changed daily. For spatial learning, four geometrical figures (1 × 1 m) were placed on curtain walls at tank rim level as cues. The platform was submerged (1.5 cm) in a quadrant different from cued learning. Rats were placed in the tank within a pre-assigned quadrant and allowed 90 s to find platform. Three daily trials, alternating start location, were administered (10-min ITI). Rats were trained seven consecutive days, reaching the criteria of > 85% of control animals showing a > 50% decrease from original latency. A probe test was conducted 24 h following the final training trial to examine reference memory. The platform was removed and rats placed within quadrant opposite to goal quadrant (GQ) for 90 s. Reversal learning was initiated 48 h later. With the platform in a new quadrant, hidden platform training was conducted (three trials/day; 4 days). A probe test was conducted 24 h later. Video-captured images were analyzed using Ethovision XT. Acquisition was measured by latency to reach the platform zone (PZ; expanded to 15 cm (Blokland et al. 2004; Vorhees and Williams 2006). In the probe tests, latency to the first entry into GQ or PZ was recorded. The number of visits to the PZ, number of visits to each quadrant, and total time in each quadrant were recorded for each 30-s epoch. Wilcoxon Rank Sum tests were used to analyze latency on the first trial of cued learning. For each day, mean daily latencies were calculated and analyzed within each test phase of the MWM by RM ANOVAs employing a mixed model with autoregressive (1) (AR(1)) covariance due to the correlation between days. Comparisons across dose groups for any 1 day were analyzed by ANOVA. In the hidden platform sessions, the percentage of improvement in learning was calculated as the change in latency from first to last day and analyzed by Wilcoxon rank sum test. In the probe tests, latency to the first entry into GQ or PZ was analyzed by Kruskal-Wallis test and Wilcoxon rank sum test. Number of entries and duration spent in each quadrant were analyzed by a RM-ANOVA with epoch and quadrant as factors.
Y-Maze
With shipment 4, the Y-maze task was included as an additional assessment of spatial learning using a continuous spontaneous alternation paradigm (Lalonde 2002). PND38 rats from G2 and G4 were placed in the center intersection of a black, plexiglass Y-maze with 3 equal arms (56 × 10 x25cm) and allowed to explore for 7 min. Video-captured images were analyzed for arm entries using Ethovision XT. Sequential reentries into an arm were excluded and percent alternation was calculated as the number of triads containing entries into all three arms/maximum possible alternations. Wilcoxon Rank Sum tests were used to analyze the number of entries and % alternation.
Tissue Collection and Fluoride Analysis
Following completion of MWM testing, samples were collected from rats randomly selected from shipments 3 and 4. Blood was collected via a cardiac puncture from rats deeply anesthetized under CO2 and plasma separated by centrifugation in EDTA-free, heparin coated plasma tubes (# 367874, Becton Dickinson, Franklin Lakes, NJ). The brain and femur were excised, immediately frozen on dry ice, and stored at -80 °C. Cleaned femur samples (5–8 mg) were ashed (8 h; 590 °C), pulverized, and weighed. Brain samples were homogenized in 3-ml di-H2O. For PND25 assessment, the brain and femur samples were obtained from randomly selected unassigned male rats. Duplicate samples were analyzed using a modification of the hexamethyldisiloxane microdiffusion method of Taves and Neuman (1964) and detected with a fluoride ion-specific electrode and a pH/ISE meter. Urine was collected in metabolism cages (Techniplast) from individual rats (n = 10) in G2 and G4 between 9:00–13:00 h and frozen. No water was provided over this interval. Urine samples < 200 μl were excluded. A 100-μl aliquot of urine was analyzed for creatinine using a kinetic modification of the Jaffe procedure (Moore and Sharer 2017). The rate of change at 520/800 nm was determined using the Olympus AU400e clinical analyzer (Beckman-Coulter Irving, TX).
Tissue Collection, Staining, and Histological Analysis
The kidney, liver, and reproductive system organs were collected following CO2 euthanasia at the termination of behavioral testing from randomly selected adult rats (> PND80) in cohorts 3 and 4 of shipment 3 (n = 8–13). The liver and kidney were immersion fixed in 10% neutral buffered formalin. Testes and epididymis were immersion fixed 24 h in Davidson’s. Samples were trimmed, ethanol dehydrated, embedded in paraffin, and 5 μm sections cut and stained with Hematoxylin and eosin (H&E). Randomly selected rats from G2 and G4 (n = 6; cohort 4) were deeply anesthetized with Fatal-Plus (Vortech Pharmaceuticals, Ltd., Dearborn, MI), whole body perfused with saline followed by 4% paraformaldehyde/phosphate buffered saline, and post-fix overnight at 4 °C. Brains were excised, transected in the mid-sagittal plane, and processed for paraffin embedding. H&E stained sections from each hemisphere (8 μm) were selected to represent a consistent plane of cut containing the hippocampus (lateral 1.35–1.95 mm) and immunostained for astrocytes and microglia. Endogenous peroxidase activity was quenched with 3% H2O2 followed by heat-induced epitope retrieval (0.01 M citrate buffer pH 6.0; Biocare Medical, Concord, CA). Non-specific binding was blocked with avidin/biotin (Vector Labs, Burlingame, CA) and 10% normal goat serum (Jackson Immunoresearch, West Grove, PA). Sections were incubated with rabbit anti-cow glial fibrillary acidic protein (Dako GFAP; 1:7000; RT; 30 min; Agilent Technologies, Carpinteria, CA) then incubated with biotinylated goat anti-rabbit IgG (1:500; Vector-Labs) and detected with Vectastain Elite ABC R.T.U. (Vector Labs); 3,3-diaminobenzidine (DAB, Agilent Technologies). Microglia were identified with a rabbit polyclonal antibody to ionized calcium-binding adaptor molecule 1 (Iba-1, 1:600, 1 h, 24 °C; Wako Chemicals, Richmond, VA) following microwave antigen retrieval. Defined regions of interest (ROI) of the suprapyramidal blade of the dentate gryus and the CA1 pyramidal layer were evaluated. Brain sections were scanned under × 20 magnification (Aperio ScanScope T2 scanner, Aperio Technologies, Inc., Vista, CA) and viewed using Aperio ImageScope v.6.25.0.1117. Stained slides were assigned random numbers and blinded for evaluation.
Thyroid Hormone Analysis
From cohort 1 in shipment 3, six rats (PND56) per group were randomly selected and blood collected via cardiac puncture under CO2 anesthesia. Serum triiodothyronine (T3) and thyroxine (T4) analyses were performed using I125 radioimmunoassays (MP biomedicals LLC; Costa Mesa CA) following manufacturer’s instructions. A 100-μl aliquot of serum was incubated with 1 ml of T3 tracer (37 °C; 60 min) and radioactivity determined (APEX gamma counter; ICN Microbiomedic Systems, Huntsville, AL). For T4 determination, samples and standards (2–20 ng/dl) were incubated with T4 tracer (RT; 60 min) and processed as described for T3. The percent trace level was calculated and ng/dl determined from standard curve as adjusted for non-specific binding. Serum samples were assayed for thyroid stimulating hormone (TSH) (Rat Pituitary Magnetic Bead Panel Kit (Cat. no. RPTMAG-86K); EMD Millipore Corporation, Billerica, MA) according to manufacturer’s protocol. Briefly, 25-μl aliquot (diluted 1:3) was incubated in 200 μl of assay buffer with TSH magnetic beads (25 μl) overnight at 4 °C. Samples were washed, incubated with 50-μl detection antibody (RT, 30 min) followed by 50 μl of streptavidin-phycoerythrin (RT; 30 min), washed, 100 μl of Sheath Fluid added (5 min, RT) then a specific spectral address detected by phycoerythrin immediately analyzed (Luminex xPONENT; Luminex Corp., Austin, TX) using a fluorescent bead-based multiplexing system (LiquiChip-200; QIAGEN, Valencia, CA).
Statistical Analysis
While fluorine is considered an essential element in human diet, how this applies to rodents is not known; however, lowering F− levels in the specialized low-F− chow to approximately 3 ppm had no impact on gestational outcome, eye-opening, and body weight. This allowed us to make comparisons across the drinking water dose groups maintained on the vehicle low-F− chow. The experimental design for statistical analysis adhered to the following. Differences between G1 (control chow) and G2 (low-F− chow) rats receiving RO-H2O for behavioral endpoints were assessed to confirm that the lower level of fluoride in the diet did not alter the normal expected pattern of behavior. Differences between G2 and G3 (10 ppm F−) or G4 (20 ppm F−) were tested to evaluate effects of fluoride in the drinking water on rats maintained on the low-F− chow. Data were tested for homogeneity of variance using Levene’s tests and for non-normality using Shapiro-Wilk tests. Statistical significance was set at two-tailed p < 0.05. Group sizes were statistically determined as sufficient for detecting significant differences from controls at p < 0.05. In the absence of a significant interaction between shipment and exposure, shipment was not included as a factor in the final analysis. Analyses of F− levels and levels of T3, T4, and TSH were conducted with Wilcoxon rank sum tests (G1 vs. G2) and Kruskal Wallis tests (G2, 3, and 4). Post-hoc comparisons were conducted with Dunn’s multiple comparisons tests. Specific analyses conducted for behavioral tests are reported in relevant methods sections. Statistical analyses were performed using GraphPad Prism 7 (GraphPad Software, Inc., La Jolla, CA); SAS 9.37 (SAS Institute, Cary, NC), and R (R Core Team 2016).