Skip to main content
Log in

Holistic processing does not require configural variability

  • Brief Report
  • Published:
Psychonomic Bulletin & Review Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Using the Garner speeded classification task, Amishav and Kimchi (Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 17, 743–748, 2010) found that participants could selectively attend to face features: Classifying faces based on the shape of the eyes was not influenced by task-irrelevant variation in the shape of the mouth, and vice versa. This result contrasts with a large body of work using another selective attention task, the composite task, in which participants are unable to selectively attend to face parts: Same/different judgments for one-half of a composite face are influenced by the same/different status of the task-irrelevant half of that composite face. In Amishav and Kimchi, faces all shared a common configuration of face features. By contrast, configuration is typically never controlled in the composite task. We asked whether failures of selective attention observed in the composite task are caused by faces varying in both features and configuration. In two experiments, we found that participants exhibited failures of selective attention to face parts in the composite task even when configuration was held constant, which is inconsistent with Amishav and Kimchi’s conclusion that face features can be processed independently unless configuration varies. Although both measure failures of selective attention, the Garner task and composite task appear to measure different mechanisms involved in holistic face perception.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. There are two versions of the composite task in the literature (complete and partial design), and debate over which is more appropriate. Here we use the complete design, which we have empirically demonstrated to be a more reliable and valid measure of holistic face processing (see Richler & Gauthier, 2014, for a review).

  2. Although we are manipulating one aspect of configural information between the DC and SC conditions to determine whether this influences holistic processing (i.e., ability to selectively attend to parts), this should not be interpreted as a manipulation of configural processing (i.e., sensitivity to spatial relations between features), which our experiments are not designed to measure.

  3. The findings were qualitatively the same if all participants (except those discarded due to a programming error) were included in the analyses (SC holistic processing: p = .01, η2 p = .17; DC holistic processing: p < .001, η2 p = .45; congruency × alignment × configuration group: p = .28, η2 p = .02;).

  4. We initially ran an experiment where target part was blocked using the same timing parameters as Experiment 1. The congruency × alignment × configuration interaction was not significant (F 1,64 = 1.72, MSE = .17, p = .20, η2 p = .03). Holistic processing was significant for the DC (N = 33; p = .001, η2 p = .28) but not SC (N = 33; p = .23, η2 p = .04) group. However, these results were difficult to interpret due to a pronounced ceiling effect in the SC condition; blocking by cued part made the task easier because participants knew which part of the study face was relevant, and could (try to) devote all their attention to that part at study. Presentation time was reduced in Experiment 2 to resolve this issue, bringing performance off ceiling levels.

  5. Holistic processing in the DC group did reach statistical significance when all participants (with the exception of those discarded due to a computer error) were included in the analyses (p = .04, η2 p = .09). Including these data did not qualitatively change the other critical effects (SC holistic processing: p = .03, η2 p = .10; alignment × congruency × configuration group interaction: p = .90, η2 p < .00).

References

  • Amishav, R., & Kimchi, R. (2010). Perceptual integrality of componential and configural information in faces. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 17, 743–748.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cabeza, R., & Kato, T. (2000). Features are also important: Contributions of featural and configural processing to face recognition. Psychological Science, 11, 429–433.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Chua, K.-W., Richler, J. J., & Gauthier, I. (2014). Becoming a Lunari or Taiyo expert: Learned attention to parts drives holistic processing of faces. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 40, 1174–1182.

    PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • DeGutis, J., Wilmer, J., Mercado, R. J., & Cohan, S. (2013). Using regression to measure holistic face processing reveals a strong link with face recognition ability. Cognition, 126, 87–100.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Farah, M. J., Wilson, K. D., Drain, M., & Tanaka, J. N. (1998). What is" special" about face perception? Psychological Review, 105, 482–498.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Freud, E., Avidan, G., & Ganel, T. (2013). Holistic processing of impossible objects: Evidence from Garner’s speeded-classification task. Vision Research, 93, 10–18.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Garner, W. R. (1974). The processing of information and structure. New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hole, G. J. (1994). Configurational factors in the perception of unfamiliar faces. Perception, 23, 65–74.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kimchi, R., & Amishav, R. (2010). Faces as perceptual wholes: The interplay between component and configural properties in face processing. Visual Cognition, 18, 1034–1062.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kimchi, R., Behrmann, M., Avidan, G., & Amishav, R. (2012). Perceptual separability of featural and configural information in congenital prosopagnosia. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 29, 447–463.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • McKone, E., & Yovel, G. (2009). Why does picture-plane inversion sometimes dissociate perception of features and spacing in faces, and sometimes not? Toward a new theory of holistic processing. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 16, 778–797.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Melara, R. D., & Mounts, J. R. (1993). Selective attention to Stroop dimensions: Effects of baseline discriminability, response mode, and practice. Memory & Cognition, 21(5), 627–645.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pomerantz, J. R., Pritstach, E. A., & Carson, C. E. (1989). Attention and object perception. In 8. Shepp & S. Ballesteros (Eds.), Object perception: Structure and process (pp. 53–89). Hillsdale: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Richler, J. J., & Gauthier, I. (2014). A meta-analysis and review of holistic face processing. Psychological Bulletin, 140, 1281–1302.

  • Richler, J. J., Cheung, O. S., Wong, A. C.-N., & Gauthier, I. (2009). Does response interference contribute to face composite effects? Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 16, 258–263.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Richler, J. J., Mack, M. L., Palmeri, T. J., & Gauthier, I. (2011). Inverted faces are (eventually) processed holistically. Vision Research, 51, 333–342.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Richler, J. J., Palmeri, T. J., & Gauthier, I. (2012). Meanings, mechanisms, and measures of holistic processing. Frontiers in Psychology, 3.

  • Ross, D.A., Richler, J.J., & Gauthier, I. (2014) Reliability of composite task measurements of holistic face processing. Behavior Research Methods.

  • Rossion, B. (2008). Picture-plane inversion leads to qualitative changes of face perception. Acta Psychologica, 128, 274–289.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Searcy, J. H., & Bartlett, J. C. (1996). Inversion and processing of component and spatial-relational information in faces. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 22, 904–915.

    Google Scholar 

  • Susilo, T., Rezlescu, C., & Duchaine, B. (2013). The composite effect for inverted faces is reliable at large sample sizes and requires the basic face configuration. Journal of Vision, 14, 1–9.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tanzer, M., Freud, E., Ganel, T., & Avidan, G. (2013). General holistic impairment in congenital prosopagnosia: Evidence from Garner’s speeded-classification task. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 30, 429-445.

  • Van Leeuwen, C., & Bakker, L. (1995). Stroop can occur without Garner interference: Strategic and mandatory influences in multidimensional stimuli. Perception & Psychophysics, 57, 379–392.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Young, A. W., Hellawell, D., & Hay, D. C. (1987). Configurational information in face perception. Perception, 16, 747–759.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

This work was supported by the NSF (Grants SMA-1041755 and SBE-1257098) and NEI (Grants R01-EY013441 and P30-EY008126). The authors thank Jackie Floyd, Amit Khandhadia, Kaleb Lowe, David Nelwan, Emily Sauder, and Bikang Zhang for assistance with data collection, and Ruth Kimchi for providing her stimuli.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Jennifer J. Richler.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Richler, J.J., Palmeri, T.J. & Gauthier, I. Holistic processing does not require configural variability. Psychon Bull Rev 22, 974–979 (2015). https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-014-0756-5

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-014-0756-5

Keywords

Navigation