Skip to main content
Log in

An ERP investigation of dichotic repetition priming with temporally overlapping stimuli

  • Brief Report
  • Published:
Psychonomic Bulletin & Review Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

In the present study, we used event-related potentials (ERPs) to examine the effects of prime–target repetition using a dichotic priming paradigm. Participants monitored a stream of target words in the right, attended ear for occasional animal names, and ERPs were recorded to nonanimal words that were either unrelated to or a repetition of prime words presented to the left ear. The prime words were spoken in a different voice and had a lower intensity than did the target words, and the prime word onset occurred 50 ms before target word onset. Repetition-priming effects were observed in the ERPs starting around 150 ms post-target-onset and continued to influence processing for the duration of the target stimuli. These priming effects provide further evidence in favor of parallel processing of overlapping dichotic stimuli, at least up to the level of some form of sublexical phonological representation, a likely locus for the integration of the two sources of information.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Given the evidence for a right-ear advantage in prior research on dichotic listening (e.g., Kimura, 1961), we presented to-be-attended stimuli to that ear in order to further bias processing toward the attended stimuli.

  2. It has not gone unnoticed that the early dichotic repetition-priming effect found in the present work resembles the mismatch negativity (MMN) effect found in ERP studies using the so-called “oddball” paradigm (e.g., Näätänen, 2001), in terms of both the timing and the spatial distribution of these effects. The MMN is thought to require habituation to a standard (i.e., massively repeated) stimulus, prior to presentation of the deviant stimulus that generates an increased negativity. Clearly, this was not the case in the present study, in which repeated and different stimuli were equiprobable.

References

  • Bentin, S., Kutas, M., & Hillyard, S. A. (1995). Semantic processing and memory for attended and unattended words in dichotic listening: Behavioral and electrophysiological evidence. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 21, 54–67. doi:10.1037/0096-1523.21.1.54

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cutting, J. E. (1976). Auditory and linguistic processes in speech perception: Inferences from six fusions in dichotic listening. Psychological Review, 83, 114–140. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.83.2.114

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Darwin, C. J., Brungart, D. S., & Simpson, D. B. (2003). Effects of fundamental frequency and vocal-tract length on attention to one of two simultaneous talkers. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 114, 2913–2922.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dufour, S. (2008). Phonological priming in auditory word recognition: When both controlled and automatic processes are responsible for the effects. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 62, 33–41. doi:10.1037/1196-1961.62.1.33

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dupoux, E., Kouider, S., & Mehler, J. (2003). Lexical access without attention? Explorations using dichotic priming. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 29, 172–184.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Grainger, J., Lopez, D., Eddy, M., Dufau, S., & Holcomb, P. J. (2012). How word frequency modulates masked repetition priming: An ERP investigation. Psychophysiology, 49, 604–616. doi:10.1111/j.1469-8986.2011.01337.x

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hannagan, T., Magnuson, J., & Grainger, J. (2013). Spoken word recognition without a TRACE. Frontiers in Language Sciences, 4, 563.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hillyard, S. A., Hink, R. F., Schwent, V. L., & Picton, T. W. (1973). Electric signs of selective attention in the human brain. Science, 182, 177–179.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Holcomb, P. J., & Grainger, J. (2006). On the time course of visual word recognition: An event-related potential investigation using masked repetition priming. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 18, 1631–1643. doi:10.1162/jocn.2006.18.10.1631

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Holender, D. (1986). Semantic activation without conscious identification in dichotic listening, parafoveal vision, and visual masking: A survey and appraisal. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 9, 1–23. doi:10.1017/S0140525X00021269. disc. 23–66.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kimura, D. (1961). Cerebral dominance and the perception of verbal stimuli. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 15, 166–171.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kolinsky, R., Morais, J., & Cluytens, M. (1995). Intermediate representations in spoken word recognition: Evidence from word illusions. Journal of Memory and Language, 34, 19–40.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lachter, J., Forster, K. I., & Ruthruff, E. (2004). Forty-five years after Broadbent (1958): Still no identification without attention. Psychological Review, 111, 880–913. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.111.4.880

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mattys, S., & Melhorn, J. F. (2005). How do syllables contribute to the perception of spoken English? Insight from the migration paradigm. Language and Speech, 48, 223–253.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Näätänen, R. (2001). The perception of speech sounds by the human brain as reflected by the mismatch negativity (MMN) and its magnetic equivalent (MMNm). Psychophysiology, 38, 1–21. doi:10.1111/1469-8986.3810001

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Okita, T., & Jibu, T. (1998). Selective attention and N400 attenuation with spoken word repetition. Psychophysiology, 35, 260–271.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rivenez, M., Darwin, C. J., & Guillaume, A. (2006). Processing unattended speech. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 119, 4027–4040.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sexton, M. A., & Geffen, G. (1981). Phonological fusion in dichotic monitoring. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 7, 422–429. doi:10.1037/0096-1523.7.2.422

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Treisman, A. M. (1970). Perception and recall of simultaneous speech stimuli. Acta Psychologica, 33, 132–148.

  • Treisman, A. M. (1971). Shifting attention between the ears. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 23, 157–167. doi:10.1080/14640747108400236

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author note

This research was supported by Grant Numbers HD25889 and ERC 230313.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Jonathan Grainger.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Grainger, J., Holcomb, P.J. An ERP investigation of dichotic repetition priming with temporally overlapping stimuli. Psychon Bull Rev 22, 289–296 (2015). https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-014-0677-3

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-014-0677-3

Keywords

Navigation