Skip to main content
Log in

Why is knowledge updating after task experience incomplete? Contributions of encoding experience, scaling artifact, and inferential deficit

  • Published:
Memory & Cognition Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Knowledge updating occurs when people learn about the impacts of variables on memory after experiencing their effects. For instance, judgments of learning (JOLs) for encoding strategies (e.g., imagery and repetition) show no difference during a first study–test trial; however, during a second trial, JOLs better reflect the benefits of the more effective strategy. Although this outcome indicates some knowledge updating, JOLs on a second trial rarely update to reflect the full impact of a given variable. We investigated several explanations for this incomplete updating. Evidence using prestudy JOLs from Experiments 1 and 2 disconfirmed the encoding-disrupts-updating (EDU) hypothesis, which is that the experience of encoding items on the second trial disrupts the use of new knowledge in making JOLs. In Experiment 3, we used binary JOLs to evaluate whether the lack of updating is an artifact of people not wanting to use extreme ratings, which accounted for some—but not all—of the incomplete updating. Finally, in Experiment 4, immediately after the test on the initial trial, participants received feedback about how many items they had recalled for each level of the focal variable, and their JOLs on the second trial still showed incomplete updating. Taken together, the evidence suggests that incomplete knowledge updating on JOLs arises from multiple factors, including a scaling artifact and the deficient use of accurate knowledge when making JOLs.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4
Fig. 5

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. This version of the EDU hypothesis—in which encoding experiences overshadow new knowledge—can explain incomplete updating, and hence it is our focus here. However, it is possible that encoding experiences on the second trial could reinforce knowledge about the strategies learned on the first trial, which would predict that prestudy JOLs would show less updating. The evidence from the present experiments is inconsistent with this hypothesis, so we do not consider it further.

  2. Two details of this experiment are worth noting. First, we removed six participants from the sample for responding on the JOL scale from 0 to 10, instead of 0 to 100, for the first trial. This invalid responding was likely a carryover from the PEP questionnaire instructions, which had participants make ratings of strategy effectiveness on a scale from 1 to 10. Second, two pairs had the same target words. Removing these pairs, however, did not change the results; therefore, data based on the original set of 60 pairs were analyzed.

  3. Analysis of relative accuracy was relevant to evaluating the EDU hypothesis and not relevant to evaluating the remaining hypotheses. Thus, for brevity, we will no longer highlight relative accuracy, but we do provide the relevant gamma correlations for Experiments 3 and 4 in Table 2.

References

Articles that contributed values for Fig. 1 are preceded by an asterisk (*).

  • Ariel, R., & Dunlosky, J. (2011). The sensitivity of judgment-of-learning resolution to past test performance, new learning, and forgetting. Memory & Cognition, 39, 171–184. doi:10.3758/s13421-010-0002-y

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Arnold, M. M., Higham, P. A., & Martín-Luengo, B. (2013). A little bias goes a long way: The effects of feedback on the strategic regulation of accuracy on formula-scored tests. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 19, 383–402. doi:10.1037/a0034833

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Begg, I., Duft, S., LaLonde, P., Melnick, R., & Sanvito, J. (1989). Memory predictions are based on ease of processing. Journal of Memory & Language, 28, 610–632. doi:10.1016/0749-596X(89)90016-8

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • *Bieman-Copland, S., & Charness, N. (1994). Memory knowledge and memory monitoring in adulthood. Psychology and Aging, 9, 287–302. doi:10.1037/0882-7974.9.2.287

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bjork, R. A., Dunlosky, J., & Kornell, N. (2013). Self-regulated learning: Beliefs, techniques, and illusions. Annual Review of Psychology, 64, 417–444. doi:10.1146/annurev-psych-113011-143823

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Brigham, M. C., & Pressley, M. (1988). Cognitive monitoring and strategy choice in older and younger adults. Psychology and Aging, 3, 249–257. doi:10.1037/0882-7974.3.3.249

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Budescu, D. V., Erev, I., & Wallsten, T. S. (1997). On the importance of random error in the study of probability judgment. Part I: New theoretical developments. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 10, 157–171. doi:10.1002/(SICI)1099-0771(199709)10:3<157::AID-BDM260>3.0.CO;2-#

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Castel, A. D. (2008). Metacognition and learning about primacy and recency effects in free recall: The utilization of intrinsic and extrinsic cues when making judgments of learning. Memory & Cognition, 36, 429–437. doi:10.3758/MC.36.2.429

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Craik, F. I. M., & Watkins, M. J. (1973). The role of rehearsal in short-term memory. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 12, 599–607. doi:10.1016/S0022-5371(73)80039-8

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • *Dunlosky, J., & Hertzog, C. (2000). Updating knowledge about strategy effectiveness: A componential analysis of learning about strategy effectiveness from task experience. Psychology and Aging, 15, 462–474. doi:10.1037/0882-7974.15.3.462

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dunlosky, J., Serra, M. J., Matvey, G., & Rawson, K. A. (2005). Second order judgments about judgments of learning. Journal of General Psychology, 132, 335–346. doi:10.3200/GENP.132.4.335-346

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dunlosky, J., & Tauber, S. K. (2014). Understanding people’s metacognitive judgments: An isomechanism framework and its implications for applied and theoretical research. In T. Perfect & S. Lindsay (Eds.), Handbook of applied memory (pp. 444–464). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • England, B. D., & Serra, M. J. (2012). The contributions of anchoring and past-test performance to the underconfidence-with-practice effect. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 19, 715–722. doi:10.3758/s13423-012-0237-7

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Erev, I., Wallsten, T. S., & Budescu, D. V. (1994). Simultaneous over- and underconfidence: The role of error in judgment processes. Psychological Review, 101, 519–527. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.101.3.519

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hanczakowski, M., Zawadzka, K., Pasek, T., & Higham, P. A. (2013). Calibration of metacognitive judgments: Insights from the underconfidence-with-practice effect. Journal of Memory and Language, 69, 429–444. doi:10.1016/j.jml.2013.05.003

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hertzog, C., & Dunlosky, J. (2004). Aging, metacognition, and cognitive control. In B. H. Ross (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation: Advances in research and theory (pp. 215–252). San Diego, CA: Elsevier Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hertzog, C., Dunlosky, J., Robinson, A. E., & Kidder, D. P. (2003). Encoding fluency is a cue used for judgments about learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 29, 22–34. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.29.1.22

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hertzog, C., Kidder, D. P., Powell-Moman, A., & Dunlosky, J. (2002). Aging and monitoring associative learning: Is monitoring accuracy spared or impaired? Psychology and Aging, 17, 209–225. doi:10.1037/0882-7974.17.2.209

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • *Hertzog, C., Price, J., Burpee, A., Frentzel, W. J., Feldstein, S., & Dunlosky, J. (2009). Why do people show minimal knowledge updating with task experience: Inferential deficit or experimental artifact? Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 62, 155–173. doi:10.1080/17470210701855520

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • *Hertzog, C., Price, J., & Dunlosky, J. (2008). How is knowledge generated about memory encoding strategy effectiveness? Learning and Individual Differences, 18, 430–445. doi:10.1016/j.lindif.2007.12.002

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • *Koriat, A., & Bjork, R. A. (2006). Illusions of competence during study can be remedied by manipulations that enhance learners’ sensitivity to retrieval conditions at test. Memory & Cognition, 34, 959–972. doi:10.3758/BF03193244

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Koriat, A., Sheffer, L., & Ma’ayan, H. (2002). Comparing objective and subjective learning curves: Judgments of learning exhibit increased underconfidence with practice. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 131, 147–162. doi:10.1037/00963445.131.2.147

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • *Matvey, G., Dunlosky, J., Shaw, R. J., Parks, C., & Hertzog, C. (2002). Age-related equivalence and deficit in knowledge updating of cue effectiveness. Psychology and Aging, 17, 589–597. doi:10.1037/0882-7974.17.4.589

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Nelson, T. O. (1984). A comparison of current measures of the accuracy of feeling-of-knowing predictions. Psychological Bulletin, 95, 109–133. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.95.1.109

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • *Price, J., Hertzog, C., & Dunlosky, J. (2008). Age-related differences in strategy knowledge updating: Blocked testing produces greater improvements in metacognitive accuracy for younger than older adults. Aging, Neuropsychology, and Cognition, 15, 601–626. doi:10.1080/13825580801956225

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Scheck, P., & Nelson, T. O. (2005). Lack of pervasiveness of the underconfidence-with-practice effect: Boundary conditions and an explanation via anchoring. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 134, 124–128. doi:10.1037/0096-3445.134.1.124

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • *Tauber, S. K., & Rhodes, M. G. (2010). Metacognitive errors contribute to the difficulty in remembering proper names. Memory, 18, 522–532. doi:10.1080/09658211.2010.481818

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tullis, J. G., & Benjamin, A. S. (2012). The effectiveness of updating metacognitive knowledge in the elderly: Evidence from metamnemonic judgments of word frequency. Psychology and Aging, 27, 683–690. doi:10.1037/a0025838

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wallsten, T. S., & González-Vallejo, C. (1994). Statement verification: A stochastic model of judgment and response. Psychological Review, 101, 490–504. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.101.3.490

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author note

We thank Melissa Bishop for her assistance with data collection. This research was supported by the James S. McDonnell Foundation 21st Century Science Initiative in Bridging Brain, Mind, and Behavior Collaborative Award.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Michael L. Mueller.

Appendix

Appendix

Table 3 Complete 2 ×2 ×2 (item type, trial, judgment type) analyses of variance

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Mueller, M.L., Dunlosky, J. & Tauber, S.K. Why is knowledge updating after task experience incomplete? Contributions of encoding experience, scaling artifact, and inferential deficit. Mem Cogn 43, 180–192 (2015). https://doi.org/10.3758/s13421-014-0474-2

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.3758/s13421-014-0474-2

Keywords

Navigation