Skip to main content
Log in

Multiple-choice tests stabilize access to marginal knowledge

  • Published:
Memory & Cognition Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Marginal knowledge refers to knowledge that is stored in memory, but is not accessible at a given moment. For example, one might struggle to remember who wrote The Call of the Wild, even if that knowledge is stored in memory. Knowing how best to stabilize access to marginal knowledge is important, given that new learning often requires accessing and building on prior knowledge. While even a single opportunity to restudy marginal knowledge boosts its later accessibility (Berger, Hall, & Bahrick, 1999), in many situations explicit relearning opportunities are not available. Our question is whether multiple-choice tests (which by definition expose the learner to the correct answers) can also serve this function and, if so, how testing compares to restudying given that tests can be particularly powerful learning devices (Roediger & Karpicke, 2006). In four experiments, we found that multiple-choice testing had the power to stabilize access to marginal knowledge, and to do so for at least up to a week. Importantly, such tests did not need to be paired with feedback, although testing was no more powerful than studying. Overall, the results support the idea that one’s knowledge base is unstable, with individual pieces of information coming in and out of reach. The present findings have implications for a key educational challenge: ensuring that students have continuing access to information they have learned.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Levene’s Test for Equality of Variances was violated so we report corrected values.

References

  • Agarwal, P. K., Bain, P. M., & Chamberlain, R. W. (2012). The value of applied research: Retrieval practice improves learning and recommendations from a teacher, a principal, and a scientist. Educational Psychology Review, 24, 437–448.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bahrick, H. P. (1984). Semantic memory content in permastore: Fifty years of memory for Spanish learning in school. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 113, 1–29.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bahrick, H. P., & Hall, L. K. (1991). Preventative and corrective maintenance of access to knowledge. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 5, 1–18.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bahrick, H. P., & Phelps, E. (1988). The maintenance of marginal knowledge. In U. Neisser & E. Winograd (Eds.), Remembering Reconsidered: Ecological and traditional approaches to the study of memory (pp. 178–192). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Bahrick, H. P., Bahrick, P. O., & Wittlinger, R. P. (1975). Fifty years of memory for names and faces: A cross-sectional approach. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 104, 54–75.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Berger, S. A., Hall, L. K., & Bahrick, H. P. (1999). Stabilizing access to marginal and submarginal knowledge. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 5, 438–447.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bjork, R. A., & Bjork, E. L. (1992). A new theory of disuse and an old theory of stimulus fluctuation. In A. Healy, S. Kosslyn, & R. Shiffrin (Eds.), From learning processes to cognitive processes: Essays in honor of William K. Estes (Vol. 2, pp. 35–67). Hillsdale: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brown, A. S. (1991). A review of the tip-of-the-tongue experience. Psychological Bulletin, 109, 204–223.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Brown, R., & McNeil, D. (1966). The “tip of the tongue” phenomenon. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 5, 325–337.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Buhrmester, M., Kwang, T., & Gosling, S. D. (2011). Amazon’s mechanical turk: A new source of inexpensive, yet high-quality, data? Perspectives on Psychological Science, 6, 3–5.

  • Butler, A. C., & Roediger, H. L., III. (2007). Testing improves long-term retention in a simulation classroom setting. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 19, 514–527.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Butler, A. C., & Roediger, H. L., III. (2008). Feedback enhances the positive effects and reduces the negative effects of multiple-choice testing. Memory & Cognition, 36, 604–616.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, G., & Faulkner, D. (1986). Memory for proper names: Age differences in retrieval. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 4, 187–197.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gruneberg, M. M., Smith, R. L., & Winfrow, P. (1973). An investigation into response blockaging. Acta Psychological, 37, 187–196.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hart, J. T. (1965). Memory and the feeling-of-knowing experience. Journal of Educational Psychology, 56, 208–215.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kang, S. H. K., McDermott, K. B., & Roediger, H. L., III. (2007). Test format and corrective feedback modulate the effect of testing on long-term retention. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 19, 528–558.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Landauer, T. K. (1986). How much do people remember? Cognitive Science, 10, 477–493.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Little, J. L., Bjork, E. L., Bjork, R. A., & Angello, G. (2012). Multiple-choice tests exonerated, at least of some charges: Fostering test-induced learning and avoiding test-induced forgetting. Psychological Science, 23, 1337–1344.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Marsh, E. J., & Cantor, A. D. (2014). Learning from the Test: Do’s and Don’ts for Using Multiple-Choice Tests. Chapter to appear. In M.A. McDaniel & R.F. Frey, S.M. Fitzpatrick, & H.L. Roediger (Eds.), Integrating Cognitive Science with Innovative Teaching in STEM Disciplines. (in press)

  • Marsh, E. J., Roediger, H. L., Bjork, R. A., & Bjork, E. L. (2007). The memorial consequences of multiple-choice testing. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 6, 194–199.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Marsh, E. J., Fazio, L. K., & Goswick, A. E. (2012). Memorial consequences of testing school-aged children. Memory, 20, 899–906.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • McDaniel, M. A., Anderson, J. L., Derbish, M. H., & Morrisette, N. (2007). Testing the testing effect in the classroom. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 19, 494–513.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nelson, T. O., Gerler, D., & Narens, L. (1984). Accuracy of feeling-of-knowing judgments for predicting perceptual identification and relearning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 113, 282–300.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rawson, K. A., Dunlosky, J., & Sciartelli, S. M. (2013). The power of successive relearning: Improving performance on course exams and long-term retention. Educational Psychology Review, 25, 523–548.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Read, J. D., & Bruce, D. (1982). Longitudinal tracking of difficult memory retrievals. Cognitive Psychology, 14, 280–300.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Roediger, H. L., III, & Karpicke, J. D. (2006). The power of testing memory: Basic research and implications for educational practice. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 1, 181–210.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Roediger, H. L., III, & Marsh, E. J. (2005). The positive and negative consequences of multiple choice testing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 31, 1155–1159.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Roediger, H. L., III, Agarwal, P. K., McDaniel, M. A., & McDermott, K. B. (2011). Test-enhanced learning in the classroom: Long-term improvements from quizzing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 17, 382–395.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tulving, E., & Pearlstone, Z. (1966). Availability versus accessibility of information in memory for words. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 5, 381–391.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wixted, J. T. (2004). On Common Ground: Jost’s (1897) Law of Forgetting and Ribot’s (1881) Law of Retrograde Amnesia. Psychological Review, 11, 864–879.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Yarmey, A. D. (1973). I recognize your face but I can’t remember your name: Further evidence on the tip-of-the-tongue phenomenon. Memory & Cognition, 1, 287–290.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author Note

This research was supported by a Collaborative Activity Award from the James S. McDonnell Foundation’s 21st Century Science Initiative in Bridging Brain, Mind and Behavior (EJM). We would like to thank Sarah Cox for her help with coding. We also thank the members of the Marsh Lab for their helpful comments and suggestions on earlier drafts of the manuscript.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Allison D. Cantor.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Cantor, A.D., Eslick, A.N., Marsh, E.J. et al. Multiple-choice tests stabilize access to marginal knowledge. Mem Cogn 43, 193–205 (2015). https://doi.org/10.3758/s13421-014-0462-6

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.3758/s13421-014-0462-6

Keywords

Navigation