Introduction

For first-time mothers, the postpartum period is a time of rapid transformation. New changes can bring about joy and excitement while also leading to significant levels of stress and heightened risk for postpartum depression. Research demonstrates that stressors during the postpartum period are numerous, including not only stressors pertaining to infant care, but also physical (i.e., fatigue, pain, sexual concerns), intrapersonal (i.e., adjustment to maternal identity, concerns about weight, self-care), and interpersonal stressors (i.e., caring for other family members, work, relationship with partner) (Groer et al., 2002). Although increases in levels of stress to some degree are normative during the postpartum period (Abidin, 1995; Kristensen et al., 2018), excessive stress constitutes a serious threat to maternal, infant, and family wellbeing (Booth et al., 2018; Crnic et al., 1983; Leppert et al., 2018; Razurel et al., 2013; Rodgers, 1998). This is likely by challenging the allostatic load as shown in other work on populations with higher levels of stress (Juster et al., 2010). Researchers have called for greater investigation into the psychological experience of stress as a distinct construct, as maternal stress has important implications for but differs from the experience of depression and anxiety (Rallis et al., 2014). Many researchers also have become interested in how stress reduction can curtail the development of mood disorders and impaired maternal-infant bonding, particularly among disadvantaged communities (Groer et al., 2002; Kudo et al., 2014). Understanding how mothers’ emotion regulatory capacities might buffer against stress is one way to address this question. The current study examined how postpartum perceived stress, depressive symptoms, and emotion regulation strategies are related to mothers’ neural regulation of negative emotion and parenting adaptation.

A putative mechanism underlying the development of maternal stress and depression during the postpartum period is individual differences in emotion regulation. Emotion regulation is the process of monitoring, evaluating, and modulating one’s emotional reactions to accomplish one’s goals (Eisenberg & Spinrad, 2004; Thompson, 1994). Different emotion regulation strategies can be employed to modulate one’s emotion, such as cognitive reappraisal, expressive suppression, and distraction (Gross, 1998; McRae & Gross, 2020). Cognitive reappraisal involves reinterpreting the meaning of an emotional stimulus as less negative; by contrast, expressive suppression involves inhibiting one’s own emotional responses. Cognitive reappraisal and distraction are considered antecedent-focused strategies (i.e., changing one’s emotional response or attention before or as the emotion develops), whereas suppression is a response-focused strategy (i.e., changing one’s emotional response only after the emotion has developed) (Ortner et al., 2016). Cognitive reappraisal often is considered an adaptive emotion regulation strategy, although its effectiveness can depend on the individual’s skill and the context of the stressor (Ford & Troy, 2019). For example, cognitive reappraisal is more effective than distraction for decreasing negative emotions over the long-term (Hermann et al., 2017); however, distraction is very effective at decreasing emotion rapidly in the moment (Denson et al., 2012). Cognitive reappraisal is not without cognitive costs and requires the use of self-control resources (Ortner et al., 2016; Sheppes et al., 2009; Sheppes & Meiran, 2008). Suppression has been shown to deplete physiological and cognitive resources (Friese et al., 2013; Wang et al., 2014), whereas this is less seen with distraction (Ochsner & Gross, 2005; Sheppes et al., 2009). Overall, a large body of research suggests that the habitual use of cognitive reappraisal is associated with healthier outcomes compared with expressive suppression, such as greater experience and expression of positive emotions, fewer depressive symptoms, resilience to stressors, and better interpersonal relationships (Carlson et al., 2012; Cutuli, 2014). These capacities are likely important for caregiving as well.

The capacity of mothers to regulate their emotions facilitates more sensitive caregiving and may have important implications for both maternal and child emotional health (Morris et al., 2017). Early parenting relies heavily on emotion regulation capabilities, as the parent is tasked with regulating both their emotional state and that of their infant (Morelen et al., 2016; Rutherford et al., 2015). Mothers of toddlers who report less everyday use of cognitive reappraisal describe experiencing greater negative emotion when their child misbehaves; this is further associated with mothers’ use of overreactive and harsh disciplinary strategies (Lorber, 2012). Although it can be difficult to determine the causality of findings, research suggests that postpartum distress and mothers’ everyday use of emotion regulation strategies are highly linked. Mothers with higher postpartum depressive symptoms are more likely to use maladaptive emotion regulation strategies, such as self-blame and rumination (Haga et al., 2009). Further, the use of more effective regulation strategies, such as cognitive reappraisal, are associated with lower postpartum depressive symptoms (Haga et al., 2009). Among samples of first-time mothers, higher perceived stress is associated with less use of cognitive reappraisal, higher depressive symptoms, and decreased life satisfaction (Watcharakitippong et al., 2017). Maternal distress and emotion regulation strategy likely influence one another and may be reflected in mothers’ neural regulation of emotion.

We are interested in how postpartum perceived stress and depressive symptoms are reflected in emotion regulation processes and associated brain circuitry, particularly in the prefrontal cortex. Previous work highlights the importance of frontal cortical regions in modulating amygdala activity and facilitating effective emotion regulation (Banks et al., 2007; Ochsner et al., 2002). Cognitive reappraisal is typically associated with increased activity in the ventromedial, dorsolateral, and medial prefrontal cortex (PFC) (Wager et al., 2008). This increased activity often is associated with decreased amygdala reactivity and reduced perceived negative emotion (Wager et al., 2008). Some studies have found modulation of other emotion-related regions, such as the ventral striatum and insula (Ochsner et al., 2012). Individuals who use cognitive reappraisal more frequently exhibit increased dorsolateral PFC and dorsal anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) activation, along with reduced activation in limbic regions, such as the amygdala and hippocampus, while viewing aversive images (Cutuli, 2014; Drabant et al., 2009; Vanderhasselt et al., 2013). Cognitive reappraisal is a multistep and effortful process, and brain regions involved are thought to support different functions. Individuals must maintain their reappraisal goal in working memory and direct their attention to relevant stimulus features (e.g., dorsolateral and posterior PFC, inferior parietal regions), select appropriate information from semantic memory to support reappraisal (e.g., ventrolateral PFC), and engage in performance monitoring to assess success at modifying their emotional response (e.g., dorsal region of ACC) (Ochsner et al., 2012). This process is likely disrupted in the context of stress and adversity.

Individuals with mood disorders, as well as those living under stressful conditions, such as poverty, generally show reduced prefrontal cortex recruitment while attempting to regulate negative emotion in an effortful way (Erk et al., 2010; Kim et al., 2013; Liberzon et al., 2015; Picó-Pérez et al., 2017; Zilverstand et al., 2016), suggesting difficulties in top-down control of negative emotion. However, contradictory findings suggest that distressed individuals exhibit hyperactivation in some areas, including greater, but counterproductive or compensatory recruitment of cortical regions (Johnstone et al., 2007), as well as greater activation in regions important for inhibitory control, such as the superior parietal lobule and supramarginal gyri (Picó-Pérez et al., 2017).

Some studies have investigated how distressed mothers’ brains respond when simply viewing negative noninfant-related stimuli, although not within the context of instructed reappraisal. Studies suggest that postpartum women respond differently than nonpregnant controls when viewing negative noninfant-related stimuli, such as enhanced reactivity in the insula and inferior frontal gyrus (IFG) (Gingnell et al., 2015). These differences may place women at increased risk for postpartum mood problems. Increased reactivity in the insula and IFG when viewing angry and fearful adult expressions was associated with subclinical elevations in postpartum anxiety and depressive symptoms (Gingnell et al., 2015). In the amygdala, women who experience postpartum depression and anxiety exhibit reduced activation to negative noninfant cues, such as International Affective Picture System (IAPS) pictures or adult face images (Moses-Kolko et al., 2010; Silverman et al., 2007, 2011). However, to our knowledge, no prior study has examined how postpartum stress and depressive symptoms are reflected in the neural correlates of effortful emotional regulation capacities, such as cognitive reappraisal.

The current study employed a functional neuroimaging (fMRI) approach to test the association between perceived stress and depressive symptoms on the neural regulation of emotion in first-time mothers. We utilized a validated and widely used Emotion Regulation Task (ERT), involving cognitive reappraisal of distressing images (i.e., noninfant stimuli from IAPS) to assess effortful attempts to regulate emotion (Banks et al., 2007; Phan et al., 2005). We were interested in the association between maternal perceived stress and depressive symptoms on neural regions, including the PFC and amygdala activation during emotion regulation. We hypothesized that elevated maternal perceived stress and depressive symptoms would be associated with differing activation levels in prefrontal control regions (e.g., dorsolateral, dorsomedial, and ventrolateral PFC) and in the amygdala during reappraisal compared with maintain conditions. However, based on the literature that suggests both directionalities, we did not have a hypothesis that includes specific directions of the associations in both the PFC and amygdala regions. Furthermore, we predicted that the differences in neural activation associated with perceived stress and depressive symptoms would be associated with lower reappraisal success in the scanner, reduced everyday use of reappraisal strategies, and higher levels of parenting stress.

Materials and methods

Participants

Participants were English-speaking, first-time mothers and their biological infants, at an average of 4.4 months postpartum. Ninety-one percent of participants were recruited within the first 6 months postpartum. Mothers were recruited through flyers and brochures in Denver metro areas, such as midwifery clinics, Women, Infants, and Children (WIC) centers, and Colorado state Prenatal Plus programs to ensure a socioeconomically diverse sample. Exclusion criteria included: a current/historical self-reported psychiatric/neurological illness other than depression or anxiety (to achieve a controlled, but ecologically valid community sample approach), psychoactive drug use (except antidepressants), birthing complications or infant medical illnesses involving more than a one-night stay in the neonatal intensive care unit (NICU), and maternal magnetic metal in the body. The original study focused on recruiting low or middle-income mothers; thus, participants had an income-to-needs ratio (INR) below 8.0 (Kim et al., 2017).

Sixty-five participants completed MRI scanning. Five participants were excluded due to excessive motion (>20% TRs removed; motion cutoff was framewise displacement in any direction exceeding 0.5 mm) and one was excluded because she completed only one run of the emotion regulation task (ERT), resulting in 59 participants in the final analyses. Demographic characteristics are described in Table 1. Of 59 participants who were included in the analysis, 35 participants overlap with Kim et al. (2017), 26 overlap with Kim et al. (2016), 49 overlap with Olsavsky et al. (2019), and 46 overlap with Dufford et al. (2019), all of which used different tasks from the ERT or performed a resting state functional connectivity analysis. Therefore, there is no direct overlap of neuroimaging data among these manuscripts and the current manuscript, although the cohort is similar and has some overlap in terms of participants.

Table 1 Sample characteristics

Procedures

The Infant Development, Environment, and Attachment (IDEA) Project study protocol was approved by the university institutional review board. Researchers contacted mothers by phone and assessed eligibility for the study. If eligible, mothers completed two sessions. First, mothers completed a home visit protocol, with interviews and questionnaires. Subsequently, mothers visited the Intermountain Neuroimaging Center at the University of Colorado – Boulder to complete the fMRI portion of the study (median of 23 days following home visit). Mothers received financial compensation for all visits, and childcare and transportation assistance were provided if needed.

Measures

Perceived Stress Scale

Generalized, nonspecific perceptions of stress were assessed by using the 4-item version of Cohen’s Perceived Stress Scale (PSS-4; Cohen & Williamson, 1988; Cohen et al., 1983). This measure evaluated participants’ perceived ability to handle their personal problems and control the important things in their life, and how often they felt things were going well during the past month. An example item includes, “In the last month, how often have you felt difficulties were piling up so high that you could not overcome them?” The PSS-4 has been validated and used in many studies of pregnant and postpartum populations (Karam et al., 2012; Murphey et al., 2017; Park et al., 2009). The PSS had acceptable internal consistency in the current study with a Cronbach’s α coefficient of 0.72, consistent with prior research (Warttig et al., 2013). Mean and standard deviation of total perceived stress scores were similar to those found in a community sample of postpartum women (Murphey et al., 2017).

Beck Depression Inventory

Depressive symptoms were assessed using the Beck Depression Inventory (BDI; Beck et al., 1961). This measure consists of 21 items scored on a 4-point scale. Items assess depressive symptoms in the past week, including low mood, anhedonia, feelings of guilt, irritability, fatigue, and poor appetite. The BDI has been used in other studies of postpartum women (O’Hara et al., 2019; Poyatos-León et al., 2017; Roomruangwong et al., 2017). In the current study, the BDI had good internal consistency with a Cronbach’s α coefficient of 0.81. BDI scores of 10-18 are described as mild to moderate depression, scores of 19-29 are classified as moderate to severe depression, and scores of 30-63 are considered severe depression. Eighteen participants within our sample (30.5%) met criteria for mild to moderate depression, and two participants (3.4%) met criteria for moderate to severe depression.

Parenting Stress Index

Maternal parenting stress was assessed using the Parenting Stress Index – Short Form (PSI-SF; Abidin, 1995). This self-report measure consists of 36 items scored on a 5-point scale. The Total Stress Index is a sum of Parental Distress, Parent-Child Dysfunctional Interaction, and Difficult Child subscales. The Parental Distress subscale assesses stress related to personal factors, such as depression, conflict with partner, and life restrictions due to caregiving. The Parent-Child Dysfunctional Interaction subscale assesses the extent to which the child is meeting the parent’s expectations and the parent-child relationship is stable. The Difficult Child subscale measures parents’ perceptions of the child’s self-regulatory abilities and whether they view the child as disruptive. The PSI-SF demonstrates good internal consistency, retest reliability, and correlates strongly with the full PSI (Abidin, 1995; Reitman et al., 2002; Roggman et al., 1994). The PSI-SF has acceptable to good internal consistency in the current study with a Cronbach’s α coefficient of 0.89 for the Total Stress Index.

Emotion Regulation Questionnaire

Mothers completed a questionnaire assessing emotion regulation style, the Emotion Regulation Questionnaire (ERQ; Gross & John, 2003). This is a 10-item scale assessing the tendency to regulate emotions via cognitive reappraisal and expressive suppression. Mothers responded on a 7-point Likert-type scale ranging from 1 (strongly disagree) to 7 (strongly agree). Six items were averaged to assess reappraisal (e.g., “When I want to feel less negative emotion (such as sadness or anger), I change what I'm thinking about”), and four items were averaged to assess suppression (e.g. “When I am feeling negative emotions, I make sure not to express them”). The ERQ has been used previously in postpartum samples of women (Martini et al., 2017; Rutherford et al., 2015). Consistent with prior validation studies (Gross & John, 2003), the ERQ had good internal consistency with a Cronbach’s α coefficient of 0.88 for Cognitive Reappraisal and 0.78 for Expressive Suppression. Means and standard deviation of reappraisal (M = 5.40, SD = 1.12; range: 2.83–7.00) and expressive suppression (M = 3.08, SD = 1.27; range: 1.00–6.00) were similar to those found in larger samples (Gross & John, 2003; Haga et al., 2009).

fMRI paradigm

We utilized a validated and widely used Emotion Regulation Task (ERT), involving cognitive reappraisal of negative images, to assess effortful attempts to regulate emotion (Banks et al., 2007; Ochsner et al., 2002; Phan et al., 2005). This task has been used in many studies with community and clinical samples (McRae et al., 2008; Perlman et al., 2012; Wager et al., 2008). Negative and neutral images were selected from the International Affective Picture System (IAPS; Lang et al., 1997) based on normative ratings of valence and arousal. Participants were instructed to view neutral images (Look; “Simply look at the images that appear”), experience their natural emotional state while viewing aversive images (Maintain; “Look at the images and be aware of and experience how the image makes you feel”), or to decrease the intensity of their negative affect by using cognitive reappraisal while viewing aversive images (Reappraise; “Use the techniques we’ve told you to try to reevaluate the image in a less negative context”).

Before entering the scanner, participants received instruction in cognitive reappraisal and practiced with a research assistant. The research assistant gave explicit instructions and a number of examples of how to transform a depicted scenario into a less negative or more positive situation (e.g., the people are crying tears of joy because it is a wedding) or to rationalize or objectify the content of the picture (e.g., the people are actors in a movie). To confirm that participants fully understood the reappraisal process, participants were presented with negative images and were instructed to narrate their reappraisal out loud to the research assistant. Feedback and training continued until it was clear the participant understood the process. The fMRI sessions were conducted only when participants fully understood each condition.

The fMRI task utilized a block design, with 20 second blocks, with instructions before each block to “look,” “maintain,” or “reappraise” displayed for 5 seconds. Then, participants viewed 20 seconds of distressing or neutral pictures. Each picture appeared for 5 seconds before being replaced by another picture, without an interstimulus interval. After each block, a rating scale immediately appeared for 5 seconds. Participants were asked to rate the intensity of their negative emotion via a button box (“How negative do you feel?”), with responses ranging from 1 (not at all) to 4 (very much). Interspersed between look, maintain, and reappraise blocks were baseline blocks consisting of a fixation cross and lasting 20 seconds. During this time, participants were instructed to simply relax and wait for the next group of pictures. Total task length was 15.5 minutes and consisted of three runs of 5 min 15 seconds each. Each run contained six blocks, including two look, two maintain, and two reappraise blocks which were pseudo-randomized. The behavioral indicator of the success of regulating negative emotion was calculated by subtracting average ratings following reappraise blocks from average ratings following maintain blocks.

Data acquisition and analysis

fMRI acquisition

Scanning was conducted using two different scanners due to a scanner update; scanners were Siemens Trio and Siemens Prisma. 36 participants were scanned on the Siemens Trio and 23 on the Siemens Prisma. Both were 3.0 T Siemens magnet scanners using a standard 32-channel head coil, acquiring 540 T2*-weighted echo-planar-imaging (EPI) volumes. The parameters of T2* functional sequences were matched across the scanners (TR = 2,300 ms; TE = 27 ms; flip angle = 73; field of view = 192 mm; matrix size, 64 × 64; 36 axial slices; voxels = 3 mm3). In addition to functional data, high-resolution anatomical T1-weighted images were acquired using 3D magnetisation-prepared rapid gradient-echo (MPRAGE) protocol. For the Siemens Trio, high resolution T1-weighted magnetization prepared rapid gradient-echo (MPRAGE) images were acquired with the following parameters: 192 sagittal slices, TR = 2,530 ms, TE = 1.64 ms, flip angle = 7°, FOV = 256 mm2 and voxel size 1 x 1 x 1 mm. For the Siemens Prisma, T1 sequence parameters were 224 sagittal slices, TR = 2,400 ms, TE = 2.07 ms, flip angle = 8°, FOV = 256 mm2, and voxel size 0.8 x 0.8 x 0.8 mm.

Analyses were conducted to examine potential systematic effects of scanner type. First, the noise variance across scanners was examined. The mean average temporal signal-to-noise ratio (TSNR) did not significantly differ by scanner type, t(57) = 1.35, p = .182 (Siemens Trio: M = 224.47, SD = 33.07; Siemens Prisma: M = 211.78, SD = 38.92). Second, the main variable of interest, perceived stress, did not significantly differ by scanner type t(57) = 0.78, p = .938 (Siemens Trio: M = 5.06, SD = 2.89; Siemens Prisma: M = 5.00, SD = 2.59). Third, when only scanner type was included in the whole-brain model, there were no main effects of scanner type and no significant two-way interactions of scanner type x condition. While it is not possible to completely rule out, the results of these analyses do not provide strong evidence for systematic scanner effect. However, to be on the conservative side, scanner type was included as a covariate in the whole-brain analysis.

Covariate selection

Scanner type and postpartum months were included in all fMRI models. Postpartum months was included as a covariate, because the current sample had considerable variability in postpartum age (M = 4.58 months, SD = 1.98, range: 0–10). The following sociodemographic variables were tested and included in post-hoc analyses if they were associated p < .10 with the independent variable (perceived stress, depressive symptoms): maternal age, education, race and ethnicity, handedness, breastfeeding, income-to-need ratio in the past year, maternal psychiatric medication use, maternal self-reported history of depression, anxiety, or other psychiatric disorder, and infant sex. Post-hoc perceived stress models included history of self-reported psychiatric disorder and current psychiatric medication use as covariates as they were associated p < .10 with perceived stress.

Behavioral data

Participants’ ratings of negative affective state were compared in Look, Maintain, and Reappraise conditions using a repeated measures ANOVA. Additionally, associations between perceived stress and depressive symptoms with reappraisal success in the scanner were examined.

Image preprocessing

Preprocessing and statistical analysis were conducted in Analysis of Functional Neuroimages software (AFNI) (Cox, 1996). The first four pre-steady-state volumes (2 dummy TRs and 2 additional TRs) for each run were discarded. Preprocessing steps included slice timing correction, motion correction, affine alignment, normalization, smoothing, and scaling. Slice timing correction temporally interpolated slices such that the resulting data were as close as possible to if the brain image was acquired at a single time point. Participants’ sequential functional volumes were realigned to the last volume of the last run to correct for head movement. Images with motion greater than 0.5 mm in any direction were censored. Participants with excessive motion (>20% of TRs removed) were excluded from the analysis (see Participants section). Within the data included in the analysis, the range of number of volumes censored was 0–80 (M = 13.56 ± 20.14; median = 6; ≤20% of the total volumes). Thus, at least 90 volumes for each condition were included in the present analysis. After motion correction, the functional, echo-planar image (EPI) was aligned with the structural image via coregistration. Each participant’s brain was then normalized to map onto the Talairach template (Talairach & Tournoux, 1988). Lastly, spatial smoothing was applied using 6-mm full width at half maximum blur estimates and intensity scaling was applied to compare data on a standardized unit.

fMRI Single-Subject Models

At the individual level, a general linear model (GLM) was used to analyze BOLD signal changes in response to three task conditions. Linear regression modeling was performed per voxel with the following regressors: three condition regressors, six motion parameter regressors (3 translational, 3 rotational), and a third-order polynomial modeling baseline drift.

Whole-brain models

Group level analyses examined single-subject images for Look, Maintain, and Reappraise conditions, utilizing a 3dLME model in AFNI (Cox, 1996), controlling for relevant covariates and scanner type. A whole brain mask was created based on 90% EPI coverage. Perceived stress and depressive symptoms were modeled separately to avoid collinearity as the continuous between-subject factor, because these two scores were correlated with one another (r = 0.640, p < .001). Within subject factors were task condition: Look vs. Maintain and Maintain vs. Reappraise. We ran two separate models, Look vs. Maintain and Maintain vs. Reappraise, as Look vs. Reappraise was not the contrast of interest. We examined main effects and interaction effects of perceived stress and depressive symptoms in the whole-brain analysis by entering each of these covariates separately within the 3dLME models. Correction was performed for multiple comparisons within the whole brain using the cluster extent threshold of k ≥ 33 with a height threshold of p < .001, which was determined via 3dClustSim with spatial autocorrelation function (ACF) option. To decompose significant interactions, post-hoc analyses were performed using average percent signal change values that were extracted from significant clusters and analyzed further using SPSS (Statistical Package for the Social Sciences version 22, Chicago, IL).

Exploratory regression analyses—emotional coping and parenting stress

We examined exploratory associations between neural activation extracted from the results of the 3dLME whole-brain analysis in suprathreshold clusters (subtraction data of Reappraise minus Maintain), reappraisal success in the scanner, everyday reappraisal and suppression, and parenting stress indices using bivariate Pearson correlations in SPSS.

Results

Sample characteristics

Mothers were 26 years old on average (M = 25.98, SD = 5.52), primarily Hispanic (44.1%) and non-Hispanic White (42.4%), and almost half of the participants had 13 or fewer years of education. The sample was socioeconomically diverse; almost half of families were low-income (47.5% with an income-to-needs ratio ≤2). See Table 1 for detailed participant demographic information. Elevated perceived stress was significantly associated with elevated depressive symptoms (r = 0.640, p < .001). Some data were not approximately normally distributed, so Mann-Whitney U tests were conducted when appropriate. Perceived stress scores (t(56) = −3.462, p = .001) and depressive symptoms (Mann-Whitney U = 217.00, p = .002) were higher among women with a self-reported history of depression, anxiety, posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD), or obsessive compulsive disorder (OCD). Perceived stress also was higher among mothers currently taking psychiatric medications (Mann-Whitney U = 58.50, p = .039). However, perceived stress and depressive symptoms were not associated with any other demographic variables, including maternal age, education, income-to-needs ratio (INR) in the past year, race, ethnicity, breastfeeding, and postpartum months (ps > .10). Parenting stress was associated with history of self-reported psychiatric disorder (Mann-Whitney U = 256.50, p = .010) but was not significantly associated with psychotropic use (Mann-Whitney U = 132.50, p = .506).

Behavioral data

Participant ratings of negative emotional state differed between the Look, Maintain, and Reappraise conditions (F(2) = 52.91, p < .001). Post-hoc testing using the Bonferroni correction revealed that ratings increased from the Look (M = 1.09 ± 0.15) to Maintain condition (M = 2.94 ± 0.58), increased from the Look (M = 1.09 ± 0.15) to Reappraise condition (M = 2.38 ± 0.55), and decreased from the Maintain (M = 2.94 ± 0.58) to Reappraise condition (M = 2.38 ± 0.55) (all ps < 0.001). Perceived stress and depressive symptoms were not significantly correlated with reappraisal success in the scanner (ps > .10).

Whole-brain analysis

Perceived Stress

Look vs. Maintain

There were no main effects of perceived stress symptoms and no significant two-way interactions of perceived stress x condition (Look, Maintain).

Maintain vs. Reappraise

A significant two-way interaction of perceived stress x condition (Maintain, Reappraise) was identified in the right dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC; Brodmann Area 9 & 10) at p < .05 corrected (41 voxels, x,y,z = 26,56,26; Fig. 1a). Surface visualization was conducted using Caret version 6.65 (Van Essen et al., 2001). Post-hoc correlation analysis revealed that elevated perceived stress was associated with increased neural activity in this cluster during Reappraise compared to Maintain (Fig. 2a). Perceived stress was positively associated with neural activation during Reappraise activation (r = .287, p = .027). Perceived stress was not associated with Maintain activation (r = −.132, p = .319).

Fig. 1
figure 1

(a) Right dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC) activation (BA9 & 10; x,y,z = 26,56,26, k = 41) showing perceived stress condition interaction, p < .05 corrected; (b) Left caudate nucleus (x,y,z = −13,−4,20, k = 52) showing perceived stress main effect, p < .05 whole-brain cluster-wise corrected

Fig. 2
figure 2

(a) Positive association between perceived stress and neural response in the right DLPFC in the contrast of Reappraise vs. Maintain. (b) Negative association between perceived stress and neural response in the left caudate nucleus during Maintain and Reappraise

Main effects of perceived stress in the whole-brain analysis revealed a significant cluster in the left caudate nucleus at p < .05, corrected (52 voxels, x,y,z = −13,−4,20; Fig. 1b). Post-hoc correlation analysis revealed that elevated perceived stress was associated with less neural activation in the caudate nucleus across Maintain and Reappraise conditions (Fig. 2b).

Given the significant correlations between variables of interest and maternal self-reported history of psychiatric disorder and current psychiatric medication use, we examined the effect of those potential confounding factors. When self-reported psychiatric history and current medication use were included in the whole-brain model, the association between perceived stress and right DLPFC during Reappraise compared to Maintain remained, p < .05 corrected. The main effect of perceived stress on left caudate activity also remained, p < .05 corrected.

Depression

Look vs. Maintain

There were no main effects of depressive symptoms and no significant two-way interactions of depressive symptoms x condition (Look, Maintain).

Maintain vs. Reappraise

There were no main effects of depressive symptoms and no significant two-way interactions of depressive symptoms x condition (Maintain, Reappraise).

Associations with emotional coping and parenting stress

Elevated perceived stress was associated with greater reported use of suppressive expression (r = .283, p = .031), less use of cognitive reappraisal (r = −.290, p = .027), and increased parenting stress (r = .564, p < .001) (Fig. 3). Everyday use of cognitive reappraisal was further associated with reduced parenting stress (r = −.281, p = .032); however, suppression was not significantly associated with parenting stress.

Fig. 3
figure 3

Associations between perceived stress and everyday use of (a) reappraisal (r = −.290, p = .027) and (b) suppression (r = .283, p = .031)

Depressive symptoms were not associated with everyday suppressive expression or cognitive reappraisal (ps > .10) but were associated with greater parenting stress (r = .528, p < .001).

Exploratory analyses revealed that right DLPFC activation extracted from the main analysis (subtraction data of Reappraise minus Maintain) was associated with increased parenting stress (r = .564, p < .001) (Fig. 4). However, caudate activation was not significantly associated with parenting stress (ps > .10). Right DLPFC activation during Reappraise compared with Maintain and left caudate activation during Maintain and Reappraise were not significantly associated with reappraisal success in the scanner or everyday reported use of reappraisal or suppression (all ps > .10).

Fig. 4
figure 4

Association between dorsolateral PFC activation in the contrast of Reappraise vs. Maintain and parenting stress

Discussion

The present study examined whether maternal distress levels were associated with behavioral and neural activation during emotion regulation among a socioeconomically diverse group of first-time mothers. Mothers experiencing higher levels of perceived stress during the postpartum period reported being less likely to use reappraisal strategies and more likely to use suppression. We found that women reporting higher levels of perceived stress also showed enhanced right dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC) activity when engaging in cognitive reappraisal (Reappraise condition), compared to when naturally responding to negative stimuli (Maintain condition). No significant differences were found among mothers reporting higher levels of depressive symptoms. Additionally, higher levels of perceived stress were associated with reduced left caudate nucleus activity across Maintain and Reappraise conditions. Exploratory analyses revealed that enhanced right DLPFC activity during Reappraise compared to Maintain conditions was associated with elevated parenting stress. Findings suggest that stress and reappraisal use are reflected in mothers’ neural regulation of emotion and may have implications for their transition to parenthood.

Maternal perceived stress was associated both with self-reported emotion regulation strategy use, as well as brain response during volitional emotion regulation. Mothers with higher perceived stress reported using reappraisal strategies less during their everyday life. This may be because they find these strategies more effortful and must expend greater energy to effectively reappraise negative emotional stimuli. This explanation is consistent with research demonstrating that higher perceived stress is associated with increased avoidant coping and reduced approach coping (Soderstrom et al., 2000; Watcharakitippong et al., 2017). Higher perceived stress also was associated with increased right dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC) activation during Reappraise compared with Maintain conditions. The DLPFC is involved in the processing of emotion-related meanings and is activated following presentation of both negative and positive emotional stimuli (Keightley et al., 2003; Lane et al., 1997; León-Carrión et al., 2007; Teasdale et al., 1999). Furthermore, the DLPFC plays a central role in the prefrontal emotion regulation network and supports the cognitive modulation of emotion and strategies, such as reappraisal (Buhle et al., 2014; Morawetz et al., 2016).

Despite significant findings for Reappraise compared to Maintain conditions, perceived stress was not associated with differential brain activation during Maintain versus Look conditions. Maintain (i.e., naturally responding to a negative picture) and Look (i.e., naturally responding to a neutral picture) conditions can be interpreted as tapping into emotional reactivity. Perhaps perceived stress did not have a significant impact on brain activation due to overall dampened stress reactivity during the perinatal period (de Weerth & Buitelaar, 2005).

Surprisingly, depressive symptoms were not significantly associated with use of emotion regulation strategies or brain activation during the emotion regulation task. Nonsignificant findings may be due to the recruitment of a community sample and limited range in depression scores. Although perceived stress and depressive symptoms were highly correlated in our sample, only 3.4% of participants met criteria for moderate to severe depression based on the clinical cutoff for the Beck Depression Inventory. The majority of the sample exhibited minimal depressive symptoms (84.7%, BDI ≤ 13); study findings should be interpreted within the context of this largely healthy population. Future research should investigate whether emotion regulation strategies and brain activation differ among women diagnosed with postpartum depression.

The association between perceived stress and heightened DLPFC activation during reappraisal may indicate greater effort required to down-regulate emotional responses. Although the majority of prior studies describe decreased DLPFC activity during reappraisal among distressed populations (Zilverstand et al., 2016), there are some studies that find enhanced activations. Johnstone et al. (2007) found that depressed individuals showed greater recruitment of the right lateral and ventrolateral PFC during efforts to reappraise negative emotional stimuli. In a right lateral PFC region (BA10), overlapping with the right dorsolateral cluster in the current study, depressed individuals showed increased activation during Reappraise compared with Maintain. By contrast, healthy controls showed less activation during Reappraise compared with Maintain conditions. Reappraisal while viewing violent scenes was also associated with greater right DLPFC activation in individuals with social anxiety compared with healthy controls (Goldin et al., 2009). Hyperactivation of the DLPFC may be a signal of inefficient regulation. The DLPFC also shows higher activation among depressed individuals on cognitive tasks, which helps to facilitate cognitive performance (Fitzgerald et al., 2017; Matsuo et al., 2007).

Greater right DLPFC activation among stressed mothers also may be related to heightened anxiety during reappraisal. Individuals experiencing psychological distress often show enhanced right DLPFC activation when viewing negative stimuli (Anand et al., 2005; Keedwell et al., 2005; Ritchey et al., 2011). In fact, there is a pattern of relative hyperactivity in the right DLPFC and hypoactivity in the left DLPFC among depressed and phobic individuals during emotional activation (Grimm et al., 2008; Liotti et al., 2002; Schienle et al., 2013). Distressed individuals can benefit from treatments, such as repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation (rTMS) that decrease right DLPFC activation (Bermpohl et al., 2006; Fitzgerald et al., 2003; Gershon et al., 2003; Mantovani et al., 2013); reported outcomes include improvements in emotion regulation (Diefenbach et al., 2016). Among community populations, decreasing right DLPFC activity is associated with improved inhibitory control of emotion (Cho et al., 2010; Ironside et al., 2016), whereas increasing it is associated with negative emotional responding (Kelley et al., 2013). In the current study, mothers experiencing elevated perceived stress may exhibit salience biases and differences in their interpretation of stimuli during reappraisal.

Exploratory analyses showed that neural activation during cognitive reappraisal was linked to concerns surrounding parenting. Specifically, elevated right DLPFC activation was associated with elevated total parenting stress, including worries about child misbehavior, maternal-child bonding, and maternal depressive symptoms and role changes. It is important to note that parenting-specific stress and general perceived stress are distinct, but overlapping constructs (Creasey & Reese, 1996). There is likely some spillover from parenting stress to general stress, and vice versa (Barnett et al., 1992). However, research suggests that parenting-specific stress is a unique and sometimes stronger predictor of maternal psychological distress and child outcomes compared with general life stress (Creasey & Reese, 1996; Quittner et al., 1990). Maternal stress surrounding her new parenting role may have important implications for her health and wellbeing during the postpartum period, as well as the health and wellbeing of her child. Overall, maternal neural response during emotion regulation is associated with indicators of her adaptation to her new parenting role.

Perceived stress also was associated with reduced caudate nucleus activation, both when viewing and reappraising aversive stimuli, which may reflect altered processing of negative emotion. The caudate, part of the striatum, is highly activated by emotionally arousing content (Carretié et al., 2009; Gerdes et al., 2010). It is involved in implicit and reward-based learning (Haruno et al., 2004; Seger & Cincotta, 2005) and may be particularly activated during automatic emotional processing (Bogert et al., 2016). Caudate activation is reduced among distressed individuals; patients with depression and PTSD show decreased activations of the caudate when viewing negative and positive images (Elman et al., 2009; Felmingham et al., 2014; Lee et al., 2008; Ritchey et al., 2011). After receiving cognitive behavioral therapy, depressed patients demonstrated an increase in caudate activation to viewing emotional images (Ritchey et al., 2011). Individuals exposed to a stressor (e.g., cold pressor, negative performance feedback), also show dampened caudate activation (Kumar et al., 2014; Porcelli et al., 2012). Dampened activation of subcortical structures, such as the caudate may reflect altered emotional arousal and dysfunctional prefrontal-subcortical circuitry following stress. Research suggests structural and functional connectivity between the caudate nucleus and DLPFC (Draganski et al., 2008; Leh et al., 2007; Robinson et al., 2012). In the present study, reduced caudate activation among stressed mothers may indicate less implicit emotion processing and regulation. This could contribute to greater effortful and conscious attempts at cognitive reappraisal and heightened right DLPFC activation (Rive et al., 2013).

Study Limitations & Future Directions

This study should be considered in light of several limitations. First, all measures were assessed concurrently, such that it is impossible to infer causality of findings or to examine mediation. General perceived stress and parenting stress were highly correlated in the present sample, and both were related to reduced use of cognitive reappraisal during everyday life. High levels of stress may contribute to the use of more suppression and fewer reappraisal strategies; conversely, more frequent use of suppression and lower utilization of reappraisal strategies may be associated with higher stress levels in mothers. Longitudinal studies can help to disentangle the impact of emotion regulation strategy selection and stress on parenting success and challenges. For example, longitudinal training in reappraisal techniques such as distancing has been shown to reduce participants’ level of perceived stress in their daily lives (Denny & Ochsner, 2014). Parenting interventions also are associated with reductions in parenting stress and altered neural responses to infant cry, namely increases in child versus self-focused activity in social brain areas (Swain et al., 2017).

Second, brain activation, stress, and everyday reappraisal were not associated with reports of reappraisal success in the scanner. This result is consistent with findings that only 16% of studies find significant group differences in self-reported reappraisal success between clinical and control populations while in the scanner (Dillon & Pizzagalli, 2013; Zilverstand et al., 2016). Highly stressed mothers may be able to regulate their emotions effectively when given appropriate instructions, but this process may be more effortful and not as well integrated into their everyday life.

It is important to note that the relationship of right DLPFC during reappraisal and parenting stress was an exploratory analysis. Future studies are needed to test the relationship between parenting stress and the neural regulation of emotion, particularly identifying how parenting stress is associated with observed caregiving behaviors and neural response during parenting-relevant tasks. Although the use of a well-validated task for emotion regulation was a strength of the current study, it is important to acknowledge that we assessed mothers’ neural regulation of emotion in the context of noninfant-related stimuli. Assessing emotion regulation processes more directly related to parenting may yield important findings. Future research should examine mothers’ neural regulation of emotion in response to negative infant stimuli, such as infant cry sounds, sad baby pictures, or challenging parenting situations. Importantly, studies also should assess how maternal emotion regulation capabilities influence observed parenting behaviors, such as sensitivity and hostility (Crandall et al., 2015; Firk et al., 2018; Lorber, 2012; Schultheis et al., 2019). Research is needed to examine whether neural activity during emotion regulation is related to parenting perceptions, as well as parenting behaviors and child outcomes (Firk et al., 2018; Riem & Karreman, 2019).

Lastly, future studies should assess mothers’ prior experiences in therapy and mothers’ brain activity and physiological response while engaging in regulation strategies such as suppression and distraction. The current study did not assess if mothers had prior experiences with therapy. Prior training in cognitive reappraisal could have influenced mothers’ brain response and skill in implementing reappraisal strategies (Troy et al., 2013). It also is important to acknowledge that expressive suppression was assessed via maternal self-report in the current study. Future research should examine maternal brain response while using regulation strategies, such as expressive suppression, acceptance, and distraction (Firk et al., 2018). Self-reports of suppression were associated with elevated perceived stress in the current study. Thus, in a future study, suppression may be associated with heightened amygdala activation during the down-regulation of negative emotion (Abler et al., 2010; Vanderhasselt et al., 2013). Pupillometry data, skin conductance, and facial expressivity also could be used to assess emotional reactivity in conjunction with fMRI scanning in future studies (Mckinnon et al., 2020; Riem & Karreman, 2019). It is currently unknown if some subjects may have averted their eyes when viewing negatively valenced images, which requires future investigation. Although the current study assessed for potential systematic effects of scanner type (which are described in the methods section) and included scanner type as a covariate in the analyses, future studies should seek to replicate these findings using a single scanner.

Study Implications

Although cognitive reappraisal is generally considered an adaptive emotion regulation strategy, there may be other more effective coping strategies for parenting, particularly parenting under stress. Distraction has been shown to be an effective regulation strategy for decreasing mothers’ emotional intensity and bilateral amygdala activation while listening to infant cry sounds (Firk et al., 2018). Acceptance and commitment therapy (ACT) approaches, such as mindfulness and cognitive defusion may be particularly helpful among postpartum samples (Bonacquisti et al., 2017). Teaching mothers mindfulness skills can promote adaptive coping in stressful contexts (e.g., poverty, relationship conflict) and parenting challenges (e.g., nursing, sleep deprivation) to facilitate sensitive and responsive parenting (Duncan & Bardacke, 2010; Gershy et al., 2017). Additionally, individuals report that acceptance and mindfulness strategies are easier to use than cognitive reappraisal, and participants exhibit less depletion of cognitive resources after engaging in these strategies (Keng et al., 2013; Troy et al., 2018). Future research should evaluate how distraction and mindfulness emotion regulation strategies support maternal mental health during the postpartum period, particularly among mothers experiencing high levels of contextual stressors and psychiatric symptoms.

It also is important to note that expressive suppression may be an appropriate emotion regulation strategy for some mothers and in some cultural contexts. Studies find that individuals with Western-European values experience more negative consequences of suppression (e.g., increased negative emotion). By contrast, suppression of negative emotion was associatied with fewer or no adverse effects among individuals endorsing more Asian values, as well as in Mexican American and Chinese American samples (Butler et al., 2007; Su et al., 2015). Research recruiting diverse postpartum samples, including diversity in culture, race and ethnicity, and socioeconomic status, can help us to identify adaptive emotion regulation strategies and brain response and develop appropriate interventions for new mothers.

The postpartum period is a time of elevated maternal stress and increased vulnerability. Although effective maternal emotion regulation is important for sensitive caregiving and the adaptation to parenthood, few studies have investigated how to best support maternal emotion regulation during this stressful life transition. The current study finds that highly stressed first-time mothers exhibit differences in their habitual emotion regulation strategies, favoring suppression to reappraisal strategies. Stressed mothers also demonstrate altered brain activation in the DLPFC during cognitive reappraisal, which is further associated with elevated concerns about parenting. Future research should assess levels of perceived stress among diverse samples of postpartum mothers, including characterizing longitudinal changes between postpartum stress, brain response during volitional emotion regulation, and parenting behaviors. Elevated perceived stress may interfere with mothers’ ability to engage in cognitive reappraisal, especially when stressors are chronic and outside of mothers’ control (Crandall et al., 2015; Kim et al., 2013). Investigation of contextual risk factors, such as economic and social stressors, and multiple emotion regulation strategies, such as acceptance and distraction, are critical to support the most vulnerable mothers and infants. High perceived stress could be used as a risk factor to identify postpartum mothers who would benefit from additional support, such as enhanced training in cognitive reappraisal or alternative emotion regulation strategies. Research is needed to understand the neural mechanisms underlying adaptive emotion regulation during the postpartum period, due to the unique emotional demands during this time and the long-term implications for maternal and infant wellbeing.