Perfectionism is characterized by an individual setting excessively high performance standards, worrying about making mistakes, and doubting about actions. Numerous behavioral studies confirmed that perfectionism, especially maladaptive perfectionism, has a close relationship with some negative outcomes and mental vulnerabilities, such as suicidal behaviors, stress, obsessive-compulsive disorder, eating disorders (Bachner-Melman et al., 2007; Chang, Ivezaj, Downey, Kashima, & Morady, 2008; Enns et al., 2002; Frost, Heimberg, Holt, Mattia, & Neubauer, 1993; Frost, Marten, Lahart, & Rosenblate, 1990; Lee et al., 2009; Lindner, Fichter, & Quadflieg, 2013; O’Connor & Nock, 2014; O’Connor, Rasmussen, & Hawton, 2010; Sassaroli, et al., 2008; Uzun Ozer, O’Callaghan, Bokszczanin, Ederer, & Essau, 2014; Wheeler, Blankstein, Antony, McCabe, & Bieling, 2011; Wu & Cortesi, 2009), and so forth. Furthermore, previous studies suggested that maladaptive perfectionism might be an underlying risk factor for the vulnerability of anxiety and depression (Besharat, Issazadegan, Etemadinia, Golssanamlou, & Abdolmanafi, 2014; Bieling, Summerfeldt, Israeli, & Antony, 2004; Enns et al., 2002; Frost et al., 1990; Frost et al., 1993; Wheeler et al., 2011; Uzun Ozer et al., 2014; Wu & Cortesi, 2009).

Various attempts have been made to define the construct of perfectionism. Early theories and research viewed perfectionism as a unidimensional construct and developed scales with a limited number of items. For example, Burns (1980) considered perfectionism as “those whose standards are high beyond reach or reason, people who set unremittingly impossible goals” (p. 34) and developed a perfectionism scale with only 10 items. Garner and his colleagues also designed a unidimensional perfectionism subscale in their Eating Disorder Inventory (Garner, Olmstead, & Polivy, 1983). However, subsequent research indicated that the perfectionism should be a multidimensional cognitive-personality construct, including both personal and social components. In addition, some of dimensions should be deemed to be adaptive, linking with positive outcomes, such as organization and self-oriented perfectionism, and others should be maladaptive, linking with distress or negative outcomes, including concern over mistakes, doubts about actions, and socially prescribed perfectionism (Chang et al., 2008; Enns et al., 2002; Frost et al., 1993; Frost et al., 1990; Hewitt, Flett, Turnbull-Donovan, & Mikail, 1991; Wheeler et al., 2011). According to these views, Frost and his colleagues developed the Multidimensional Perfectionism Scale (FMPS), which is one of the most commonly used and well-validated measures. It employed a six-component model, comprising concern over mistakes (CM), personal standards (PS), parental expectations (PE), doubts about actions (DA), parental criticism (PC), and organization (OR) (Frost et al., 1993; Frost et al., 1990).

Some behavioral studies revealed that there were close association between CM, DA, and anxious or depressive symptoms (Enns et al., 2002; Frost et al., 1993; Frost et al., 1990; Hewitt et al., 1991; Saboonchi & Lundh, 1997; Shafran & Mansell, 2001; Stöber, 1998; Uzun Ozer et al., 2014; Wheeler et al., 2011). Individuals with high perfectionism often misinterpreted mistakes as failure and worry about losing others’ respect following failure, or excessively doubt their own ability to accomplish tasks and thus show no confidence in their own performance. Subsequently, such worry and doubt would trigger and maintain their anxious or depressive symptoms (Frost et al., 1990). In both healthy and clinical samples (Besharat et al., 2014; Bieling et al., 2004; Cheng et al., 2015; Egan, Wade, & Shafran, 2011), perfectionism can be confirmed as a predictive cognitive-personality risk factor of anxious and depressive symptoms. Studies on treatment for mood disorders, including cognitive-behavioral therapy and some other treatments in major depressive disorder, suggested perfectionism could be considered a crucial factor and would affect therapeutic outcomes to some extent (Blatt, Quinlan, Pilkonis, & Shea, 1995; Blatt, Zuroff, Bondi, Sanislow, & Pilkonis, 1998; Zuroff et al., 2000). Thus, understanding the brain neural correlates underlying perfectionism would be particularly essential to the generation and cognitive process of negative affect.

To date, there have been few studies about the brain structure bases of perfectionism and its relation to negative emotion, whereas much of the literature has discussed the brain regions and networks of negative emotion, such as anxiety (Sylvester et al., 2012). Maladaptive perfectionism would be commonly accompanied by high trait anxiety (Kawamura, Hunt, Frost, & DiBartolo, 2001). Research on individuals with high trait anxiety uncovered their impoverished prefrontal recruitment in response to processing a competition task (Bishop, 2009). By using other tasks, like the two-choice prediction task, individuals with high trait anxiety showed abnormal activation in the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC), mainly the dorsal ACC (dACC) and medial prefrontal cortex (PFC). Results also revealed that there was a positive correlation between activation in dACC, medial PFC, and trait anxiety (Paulus, Feinstein, Simmons, & Stein, 2004). Diffusion tensor imaging studies, from another perspective, showed that fibers between the PFC and the limbic system could predict trait anxiety as well (Eden et al., 2015; Kim & Whalen, 2009).

Moreover, previous studies agreed with the viewpoint that maladaptive perfectionists would have emotions associated with higher order cognitive dysfunctions, such as rumination (Blankstein & Lumley, 2008; Flett, Madorsky, Hewitt, & Heisel, 2002; Harris, Pepper, & Maack, 2008), and suffer from deficits in emotion regulation (Aldea & Rice, 2006; Rudolph, Flett, & Hewitt, 2007). Rumination, a coping style with unintentionally repetitive thinking about one’s negative emotions, was associated with medial, dorsolateral PFC (DLPFC), and ACC in both normal and depressive populations (Cooney, Joormann, Eugene, Dennis, & Gotlib, 2010; Wang et al., 2015). For emotion regulation, it has been proved as a consequence from multiple brain regions, recruiting the crucial engagement of the PFC, ACC, and connectivity with regions from the limbic system (Eden et al., 2015; Kohn et al., 2014; Morawetz, Bode, Baudewig, Kirilina, & Heekeren, 2016). In essence, ACC is believed to play an important role in control, avoidance, error-related activity, affective state, and regulation (Bush, Luu, & Posner, 2000; Giuliani, Drabant, & Gross, 2011; Kohn et al., 2014; Mohanty et al., 2007; Shackman et al., 2011; Stevens, Hurley, & Taber, 2011). A comprehensive meta-analytical study demonstrated emotion involved in other brain regions, including the inferior frontal gyrus (IFG), presupplementary motor area (pSMA), superior temporal gyrus (STG), and angular gyrus (Kohn et al., 2014), which could be supported by other empirical evidence as well (Morawetz, et al., 2016).

Besides, clinical research on patients with maladaptive perfectionism also uncovered abnormalities in PFC and ACC (Egan et al., 2011; Rhéaume, Freeston, Dugas, Letarte, & Ladouceur, 1995; Shafran & Mansell, 2001). For instance, anorexia nervosa patients with a relentless perfectionism showed the structural abnormality and dysfunction of the ACC, cingulate gyrus, and superior frontal gyrus (Mühlau et al., 2007; Rothemund et al., 2011). Obsessive-compulsive disorder patients also exhibited the brain morphological and functional abnormalities in PFC and ACC, regions engaged in conflict monitor error processing (Ducharme, Dougherty, & Drevets, 2016; Fitzgerald et al., 2005; Graybiel & Rauch, 2000; Van Veen & Carter, 2002). These results might suggest the underlying roles of ACC and PFC in perfectionism as well.

In summary, the relationship among individual differences in perfectionism, brain structure, and negative affect in nonclinical samples remains unclear. Therefore, the aim of this study was to investigate the association of the brain regions and perfectionism with anxious and depressive emotion in a large, nonclinical sample of Chinese undergraduate students (N = 161).We first supposed that some subdimensions of perfectionism had significant correlations with the scores of the Self-Rating Anxiety Scale (SAS) and the Self-Rating Depression Scale (SDS) in our large nonclinical sample, respectively. Second, we hypothesized that the brain’s structural variations for perfectionism were widespread in regions related to emotion-related cognitive processes, mainly the PFC (e.g., DLPFC, IFG), ACC, insula, SMA, STG, and angular gyrus (Kohn et al., 2014; Morawetz et al., 2016). Third, given the association between maladaptive perfectionism and negative affect (Enns et al., 2002; Frost et al., 1990; Uzun Ozer et al., 2014; Wheeler et al., 2011), the crucial role of the PFC and ACC in cognitive control, emotional processing, and regulation (Buhle et al., 2014; Bush et al., 2000; Giuliani, Drabant, & Gross, 2011; Kohn et al., 2014; Morawetz et al., 2016; Shackman et al., 2011; Stevens et al., 2011), we hypothesized that perfectionism would be able to mediate the relationship between the PFC and negative affect. By using imaging data in vivo, we are able to reveal the structural correlates of individual differences in perfectionism as well as the association among perfectionism, brain structures, and negative affect. Furthermore, it would provide fundamental evidence for emotional processing and regulation.

Method

Participants

A total of 161 right-handed healthy volunteers (92 women and 69 men; ages 17–26 years, mean age = 19.98 years, SD = 1.13 years) participated in this study. It was a part of our ongoing project exploring the associations among brain structures and function, creativity, and mental health (http://www.qiujlab.com/resource). All subjects were university students who were recruited from the local community in the Southwest University, China. To confirm typical development, all participants had normal or corrected-to-normal vision and were screened by self-reported questionnaires before scanning. Thus, participants who had a history of psychiatric or neurological disorders, received mental health treatment, or took psychiatric medications were excluded.

The Ethics Committee of Southwest University formally approved the research protocol. All participants gave written informed consent, and we also obtained written informed consent from the youngest participant’s (age 17 years) guardian, who was her college instructor. The Brain Imaging Center Institutional Review Board of Southwest University approved this study, and the experiment procedure was carried out in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki principles. All participants were paid for their participation.

Measuring individual anxiety and depression

The SAS was selected to assess the level of anxiety. This 20-item self-rating instrument included 15 somatic and five affective symptoms (Zung, 1971). Participants rated each item on a 4-point Likert scale, ranging from 1 (none or a little of the time) to 4 (most of the time). Examples of SAS items are as follows: “I feel more nervous and anxious than usual” (Anxiousness); “My arms and legs shake and tremble” (Tremors); “I feel weak and get tired easily” (Easy fatigability, weakness); “I can feel my heart beating fast” (Palpitation); “I have nightmares” (Nightmares). What’s more, SAS demonstrated good reliability, with the initial split-half reliability being 0.71. It has been employed in both clinical and nonclinical participants (Olatunji, Deacon, Abramowitz, & Tolin, 2006; Wei et al., 2015; Zung, 1971). It also was used in previous studies on anxiety and perfectionism (Balazs et al., 2013; Julal, 2013; Pravettoni, Cropley, Leotta, & Bagnara, 2007; Sevlever & Rice, 2010; Shahid et al., 2016; Wei et al., 2015; Wu & Wei, 2008; Zi & Zhou, 2007). In this study, the Cronbach’s alpha coefficient for internal reliability in our sample was 0.79.

We used 20 items in the SDS to assess depression in each participant. The SDS is a self-report assessment tool, with each response using a 4-point scale, ranging from 1 (none or a little of the time) to 4 (most of the time). It divided those common characteristics of depression into three parts: pervasive affect, physiological equivalents or concomitants, and psychological concomitants (Zung, 1965). Furthermore, it contained items as follows: “I feel down-hearted and blue” (Depressed, sad, and blue in Pervasive affect), “I still enjoy sex” (Sex: decreased libido in Rhythmic Disturbances of Physiological equivalents), “I get tired for no reason” (Musculoskeletal: fatigue in Other Disturbances of Physiological equivalents), “I find myself restless and can’t keep still” (Agitstion in Psychomotor activities in Psychological concomitants), “I am more irritable than usual” (Irritability in Ideational in Psychological concomitants). Ten items were worded symptomatically positive and another 10 symptomatically negative. The SDS also demonstrated adequate reliability and validity (Thurber, Snow, & Honts, 2002). The psychometric evaluation revealed that the SDS correlated well with the Hamilton Rating Scale for Depression and the Beck Depression Inventory (Biggs, Wylie, & Ziegler, 1978; Dobson, 1985; Davies, Burrows, & Poynton, 1975; Meites, Lovallo, & Pishkin, 1980). Recent research also reported that this scale had an adequate internal reliability and was converged with other self-report assessments of depression, such as the Depression Adjective Check List, the Brief Symptom Inventory, and the Major Depression Scale (Thurber et al., 2002). Therefore, the SDS has also been used in research about depression and perfectionism (Di Schiena, Luminet, Philippot, & Douilliez, 2012; Julal, 2013; Xia, Ding, Hollon, & Fan, 2012; Zi & Zhou, 2007). In our sample, the Cronbach’s alpha coefficient for internal reliability was 0.85.

Measuring individual perfectionism

Perfectionism was assessed by using a self-report scale, the Chinese Frost Multidimensional Perfectionism Scale (CFMPS; Zi & Zhou, 2007), which was revised from one of the most widely used multidimensional perfectionism measures—FMPS (Frost et al., 1990). Compared to Hewitt and Flett’s Multidimensional Perfectionism Scale (HMPS), FMPS reflected a more global characteristic that contained elements of both self-oriented and socially prescribed perfectionism (Frost et al., 1993). Besides, the scale described more specific characteristics, which consisted of six dimensions (Frost et al., 1990). Finally, a study, which compared the two generally used scales, suggested that components from FMPS were more closely related to negative affect compared with items in HMPS (Frost et al., 1993). Thus, FMPS was selected in this study to investigate the neural correlates of perfectionism and the association with negative affect.

In the Chinese version, the dimension of parental criticism (PC) was combined into parental expectations (PE). Thus, CFMPS consisted of five dimensions: CM, PS, PE, DA, and OR. Example items from the questionnaire were as follows: “If I fail at work/school, I am a failure as a person” (CM); “If I do not set the highest standards for myself, I am likely to end up a second-rate person” (PS); “My parents set very high standards for me” (PE); “I usually have doubts about the simple everyday things I do” (DA); and “Organization is very important to me” (OR). For each item, participants were asked to indicate to what extent they agreed or disagreed with the given items on a 5-point Likert scale, ranging from 1 (strongly disagree) to 5 (strongly agree), with the higher scores indicating the greater level of perfectionism (Chang et al., 2008). Previous studies have shown that the scale possessed adequate reliability, construct validity, and criterion-related validity with other constructs such as the SAS, SDS, Symptom Checklist 90, the Fear of Negative Evaluation Scale, and the Self-Esteem Scale in Chinese populations (Zi & Zhou, 2007). In this study, the scale demonstrated an adequate internal reliability (Cronbach’s α = 0.74 to 0.82).

Magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) data acquisition

Whole-brain anatomical images were acquired on a 3.0-T Siemens Trio MRI scanner (Siemens Medical, Erlangen, Germany). High-resolution T1-weighted anatomical images were acquired using a magnetization-prepared rapid gradient echo sequence (repetition time = 1,900 ms; echo time = 2.52 ms; inversion time = 900 ms; flip angle = 9 degrees; resolution matrix = 256 × 256; slices = 176; thickness = 1.0 mm; voxel size = 1.0 × 1.0 × 1.0 mm3).

Voxel-based morphometry (VBM) analysis

All structural data images were preprocessed using the SPM8 software package (Wellcome Department of Cognitive Neurology, London, UK; www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm) implemented in MATLAB 2009b (MathWorks Inc., Natick, MA, USA). First, each subject’s MR image was displayed in SPM8 to screen for monitor artifacts or gross anatomical abnormalities. The reorientation of each structural image was further set to the anterior and posterior commissure manually for better registration. Then, via the new segmentation in SPM8, the images were segmented into gray matter (GM) and white matter (WM). Subsequently for registration, normalization and modulation, we performed Diffeomorphic Anatomical Registration Through Exponentiated Lie (DARTEL) algebra (Ashburner, 2007). After that, the image intensity of each voxel was modulated by the Jacobian determinants to ensure that regional differences in the absolute amount of GM were conserved. Moreover, registered images were transformed to Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) space. Ultimately, the modulated images (GM images) were smoothed with an 8-mm full-width at half-maximum Gaussian kernel to increase the signal-to-noise ratio.

Statistical analysis

A multiple linear regression analysis was conducted to identify regions where gray matter volume (GMV) was associated with individual differences in the subdimension scores of perfectionism, respectively, at the whole-brain level. To control for possible confounding variables, age, gender, and global volumes of GM were entered as covariates of no interest into the regression model. Meanwhile, we also applied the explicit mask achieved by the Masking Toolbox in SPM8 to restrict the result within gray and white matter (http://www0.cs.ucl.ac.uk/staff/g.ridgway/masking/; Ridgway et al., 2009; Zhu, Zhang, & Qiu, 2013). For the whole-brain analyses, the cluster-level statistical threshold was set at p < .05, and corrected at the nonstationary cluster correction (http://www.fmri.wfubmc.edu/cms/software#NS; Hayasaka, Phan, Liberzon, Worsley, & Nichols, 2004) with an underlying voxel level of p < .005, which has been safely applied to VBM data in some research (Hayasaka et al., 2004; Lewis, Kanai, Rees, & Bates, 2014; Li, et al., 2014; Qiao et al., 2013; Wei et al., 2015; Zhu et al., 2013). Given brain regions with a strongly priori hypothesis, the statistical significance level was set at p < .05 with small volume correction (SVC) in regions of interests (ROIs), corrected at family-wise error (FWE), and all ROIs were constructed from WFU Pick Atlas software (http://www.fmri.wfubmc.edu/cms/software#PickAtlas; Maldjian, Laurienti, & Burdette, 2004; Maldjian, Laurienti, Kraft, & Burdette, 2003).

Furthermore, we also defined the significant region (ACC, see VBM results) as a ROI through the xjview toolbox (http://www.alivelearn.net/xjview/). Then, the REX Toolbox (http://web.mit.edu/swg/software.htm) was used to extract the regional GMV (rGMV) of the ACC from each participant. We finally calculated the relationship between the ACC and DA score by using SPSS 16.0 software. Results with p < .05 (uncorrected) were considered statistically significant in these analyses.

Results

Behavioral results

Table 1 depicts the demographics and the characteristics of all questionnaires. As indicated in Table 1, CM was significantly positively correlated with SAS (r = .513, p < .001) and with SDS (r = .507, p < .001). Meanwhile, DA also had a significant positive relationship with SAS (r = .468, p < .001) and SDS (r = .442; p < .001). For the other three subdimensions, OR was both negatively correlated with SAS (r = -.197, p = .013) and with SDS (r = -.275, p < .001). PS, however, was only positively correlated with SAS significantly (r = .206, p = .009). All of the correlation analyses were conducted controlling for gender and age.

Table 1 Descriptive statistics (n = 161)

VBM results

We identified positive correlations between the CM score and rGMV in the right ACC (peak voxel of MNI: x = 14, y = 25, z = 24; t = 3.78), MCC (peak voxel of MNI: x = 10, y = −5, z = 38; t = 3.74), and insula (peak voxel of MNI: x = 46, y = −3, z = 5; t =3.23), and the left angular gyrus (peak voxel of MNI: x = -44, y = −62, z = 39; t = 3.46), the left STG (peak voxel of MNI: x = -48, y = 0, z = 0; t = 3.32) surviving with SVC in the ROIs (FWE; see Table 2).

Table 2 Brain regions with significant correlations between regional gray matter volume (rGMV) and the CM, DA, OR scores, respectively

Multiple regression analysis, with the GMV as dependent variables and DA score as independent variable, revealed a positive correlation of DA score with rGMV in the right cluster mainly located in the ACC (peak voxel of MNI: x = 2, y = 39, z = 26; t = 3.78) corrected at the non-stationary cluster correction with an underlying voxel level of p < .005 (see Fig. 1 and Table 2), the left cluster spread in the triangular part of inferior frontal gyrus (peak voxel of MNI: x = -38, y = 33, z = 21; t = 3.32) and SMA (peak voxel of MNI: x = -10, y = 3, z = 48; t = 3.88). In addition, DA had a negative association with left insula (peak voxel of MNI: x = -28, y = 24, z = 11; t = 3.78) and the right IFG (peak voxel of MNI: x = 49, y = 32, z = -3; t = 3.76; peak voxel of MNI: x = 49, y = 30, z = -6; t = 4.05), with SVC in the ROIs (FWE; see Table 2).

Fig. 1
figure 1

Brain regions show a correlation with the doubts about actions (DA) scores. a The rGMV of the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) is positively correlated with DA. The left figure was displayed by using the MRIcron software (http://www.mccauslandcenter.sc.edu/mricro/mricron/index.html), and the right figure was displayed using the BrainNet Viewer software (Xia, Wang, & He, 2013). b Scatter plots depict a correlation between the rGMV in the ACC and individual differences in DA (r = .249, p = .001)

Besides, with the SVC in the ROI (FWE), we found that the score of OR was positively correlated with the left IFG (peak voxel of MNI: x = -45, y = 31, z = -3; t = 4.02) (see Table 2).

Mediation analysis

At the whole-brain level, partial correlation analyses revealed not only a positive association between the DA score and SAS (r = .468, p < .001), and SDS scores (r = .442, p < .001), respectively, but also a positive relationship between the DA score and the rGMV of ACC (r = .292, p < .001) after controlling for gender, age, and global GMV. It suggested that DA might act as a mediator in the relationships among these variables. Then, we tested whether DA mediated the correlation of the rGMV of ACC and individual self-reported anxiety or depression or not by using the scripts written by Andrew F. Hayes in SPSS 16.0 (http://www.afhayes.com/spss-sas-and-mplus-macros-and-code.html; Preacher & Hayes, 2004, 2008). In mediation analysis, we chose the GMV of ACC as the independent variable (IV), SAS or SDS scores as the dependent variable (DV), respectively, and DA score as the proposed mediator. We also included gender, age, and whole-brain GMV as the control variables for all mediation analyses.

As expected, mediation analyses revealed that the rGMV of the ACC had a significant indirect effect on both SAS and SDS, respectively, with DA as a mediator. The effect of the ACC on SAS decreased and did not reach significance (β = 0.11, ns) after including DA as a mediator in the model. In contrast, the direct relationship namely the total effect was significant (β = 0.31, p < .05). A bootstrap simulation (n = 10,000) further confirmed that this reduction was statistically significant (95 % CI [0.11, 0.33], p < .05) (see Fig. 2a). Similarly, the effect of the ACC on SDS also decreased and did not achieve significance (β = 0.07, ns), after setting DA as a mediator in the model. However, the direct relationship, namely the total effect, was significant (β = 0.26, p < .05). A bootstrap simulation (n = 10,000) further confirmed that this reduction was statistically significant (95 % CI [0.09, 0.33], p < .05; see Fig. 2b). In summary, the two mediation model suggested that DA played a mediating role in the relationship between the GMV of ACC and negative emotion.

Fig. 2
figure 2

The doubts about actions (DA) scores mediate the association between the brain structure, the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC), and SAS, SDS scores (a, b, c), and the SAS, SDS, NA score as mediator, respectively, in the association between DA score and the gray matter volume (GMV) of ACC for addition analyses (d, e, f). a Path diagram (including coefficients) of the mediation analysis demonstrating that the rGMV in the ACC affected individuals’ SAS through DA, and the ab effect is 0.20 (without gender, age and global GMV as covariates, the effect of ab paths is 0.11, p = .0038). b Path diagram (including coefficients) of the mediation analysis demonstrating that the rGMV in the ACC affected individuals’ SDS via DA, and the ab effect is 0.19 (without gender, age and global GMV as covariates, the effect of ab paths is 0.11, p = .0043). c Path diagram (including coefficients) of the mediation analysis demonstrating that the rGMV in the ACC affected individuals’ negative affect via DA, and the ab effect is 0.21 (without gender, age and global GMV as covariates, the effect of ab paths is 0.12, p = .0036). (d, e, f) Path diagram (including coefficients) of the mediation analysis that DA as the IV, the GMV of ACC as the DV and SAS, SDS or NA as the proposed mediator respectively, and none of the b paths in the three models were significant, but the c’ paths were still remain significant. The ab effect is 0.02 (without gender, age and global GMV as covariates, the effect of ab paths is 0.01, p = 0.86) in d. The ab effect is 0.02 (without gender, age, and global GMV as covariates, the effect of ab paths is 0.0002, p = 0.9948) in e. The ab effect is 0.02 (without gender, age, and global GMV as covariates, the effect of ab paths is 0.02, p = 0.65) in f. Note SAS = scores of the Self-Rating Anxiety Scale; SDS = scores of the Self-Rating Depression Scale; NA = the scores of the negative affect, combining the scores of the Self-Rating Anxiety Scale with the scores of the Self-Rating Depression Scale *p < .05. **p < .01. ***p < .001

Depression and anxiety are two common factors of negative affect, and a combining score between depressive and anxious score seems more convincing. Thus, we averaged these two scores as the negative affect score in this study. For mediation analysis, we chose the GMV of ACC as the IV, the negative affect score as the DV and DA score as the proposed mediator respectively. In addition, we controlled for gender, age, and whole-brain GMV in this mediation analysis. Similarly, the effect of the ACC on the negative affect also decreased and did not reach significant (β = 0.09, ns) after setting DA as a mediator. However, the direct relationship namely the total effect was significant (β = 0.30, p < .05). A bootstrap simulation (n = 10,000) further confirmed this reduction was statistically significant (95 % CI [0.10, 0.35], p < .05; see Fig. 2c).

To verify that DA was playing a central role in mediating the relationship between the GMV of ACC and the individual differences of negative emotion (SAS, SDS, NA), we also conducted additional analyses as follows. We set DA as the IV, the GMV of ACC as the DV, and SAS, SDS, or NA as the proposed mediator, respectively. We found none of the b paths in the three models were significant, but the c’ paths were still remain significant. With SAS as a mediator in the model, mediation analyses revealed that the effect of the DA on ACC was still significant (β = 0.16, p = .0039), and the direct relationship, namely the total effect, was significant (β = 0.19, p = .0002), but the b paths were not significant (β = 0.05, p = .3531), and the ab effect is 0.02 (without gender, age, and global GMV as covariates, the effect of ab paths is 0.01; p = .86). A bootstrap simulation (n = 10,000) further confirmed this reduction was statistically significant (95 % CI [-0.04, 0.10], p < .05; see Fig. 2d). After including SDS as a mediator in the model, mediation analyses revealed that the effect of the DA on ACC was still significant (β = 0.17, p = .002), and the direct relationship, namely the total effect, was significant (β = 0.19, p = .0002), but the b paths were not significant (β = 0.03, p = .5379), and the ab effect is 0.02) without gender, age, and global GMV as covariates, the effect of ab paths is 0.0002, p = .9948). A bootstrap simulation (n = 10,000) further confirmed this reduction was statistically significant (95 % CI [-0.05, 0.08], p < .05; see Fig. 2e). Similarly, with NA as a mediator in the model, mediation analyses revealed that the effect of the DA on ACC was still significant (β = 0.17, p = .0036), and the direct relationship, namely the total effect, was significant (β = 0.19, p = .0002), but the b paths were not significant (β = 0.05, p = .4185), and the ab effect is 0.02 (without gender, age and global GMV as covariates, the effect of ab paths is 0.02, p = .65). A bootstrap simulation (n = 10,000) further confirmed this reduction was statistically significant (95 % CI = [-0.05, 0.10], p < .05; see Fig. 2f). These results might more highly suggest that indeed DA is the critical variable in mediating the relationship between the GMV of ACC and the individual differences of negative emotion.

Discussion

In this study, we aimed at exploring the relationship among individual differences in perfectionism, brain structure, and negative emotion in a large, nonclinical sample of Chinese undergraduate students. As a result, both CM and DA, the two subdimensions of perfectionism, were positively correlated with individual difference in self-reported anxiety and depression as well as the GMV in the ACC, a crucial brain region supporting cognitive control and emotional regulation. Furthermore, the score of DA acted as a mediator in the relationship between the GMV of ACC and self-rating anxious, depressive emotion. As far as we know, our research might be the first to reveal the brain structural correlates of individual difference in perfectionism. Taken together, these results suggested perfectionism might play a significant role in the generation of negative affect.

Relationship between perfectionism and negative emotion

According to the behavioral results shown previously, the subdimensions of perfectionism, CM, DA, and OR were related to anxious and depressive emotion, while PS correlated only with anxiety, in our large, Chinese, nonclinical sample. The results were in line with previous findings (Enns et al., 2002; Frost et al., 1993; Frost, et al., 1990; Harris et al., 2008; Saboonchi & Lundh, 1997; Wheeler et al., 2011; Zi & Zhou, 2007). A factor analysis reported that CM and DA could be clustered together to compose the main negative aspect of perfectionism, maladaptive evaluation concerns, and the central feature was evaluative concerns (Frost et al., 1993). Previous studies also found that CM and DA could be labeled as one factor with the characteristic of concerns, fear, and doubt (Purdon, Antony, & Swinson, 1999; Stöber, 1998). Therefore, individuals with a high level of the CM, DA, and PS might fear errors, imperfect performance, and failure. Then, it would make them fail to meet their own standards or others’ expectations, and lose others’ respect. As a consequence, these individuals would have both intrapersonal and interpersonal problems, such as pressure, endless attempts to be perfect, and no time for fun and relaxation. Eventually, these experiences would elicit and maintain negative affects, such as anxiety and depression, and other health risks, including chronic fatigue and burnout (Arpin-Cribbie & Cribbie, 2007; Shafran, Cooper, & Fairburn, 2002; Shafran & Mansell, 2001; Wirtz et al., 2007). Therefore, it is people who are more maladaptively perfectionistic that are more vulnerable to negative emotion.

Our results also pointed out that OR score was negatively correlated with SAS and SDS scores. The dimension of OR was generally viewed as a kind of conscientious, normal, adaptive, and positive feature in the achievement striving factor of perfectionism because this trait could help individuals reduce the possibility of erring, assure the quality of performance and reach their own excessive criterions. At last, it would help lower undesirable emotions, such as anxiety or depression (DiBartolo & Rendón, 2012).

Relationship between brain structure variation and perfectionism

We found that the CM and DA scores, which reflected the level of perfectionism, were related to brain regions mainly located in the right ACC. These results might suggest that ACC might be the neurological correlate of perfectionism, particularly maladaptive evaluative concerns (Bieling et al., 2004; Frost et al., 1993). A prior electroencephalogram study reported that DA and CM might be the significant predictors for the ACC activity in the standard Eriksen flankers task, because the DA subscale, linked to an “obsessional-like” thinking, reflects the extent to which people doubt their ability to accomplish tasks. Thus, individuals might show no confidence in their own performance and try to reach optimal results. With higher scores in the CM subscale, individuals will be more likely to react negatively to mistakes, present a tendency to interpret mistakes as equivalent to failure, and believe that they would lose others’ respect due to failure (Schrijvers, De Bruijn, Destoop, Hulstijn, & Sabbe, 2010). Given that the shared psychologically central feature in CM and DA (Bieling et al., 2004; Frost et al., 1993; Stöber, 1998), the relationship between the GMV of ACC with both CM and DA would be quite comprehensible. A higher CM score reflects more failure fear that might arouse individuals’ negative affect. Then, they would show more tendencies to employ cognitive control to avoid action. As a representation of avoidance, DA reflected individuals’ worries about the denial or rejection from others. Taken together, high association of ACC and two dimensions of perfectionism indicated individuals with high maladaptive perfectionism scores hesitated to take action because of fear of failure (Shafran et al., 2002).

More specifically, the clusters in our VBM results were comprised of a part of both dACC and vACC as the boundaries defined previously in a meta-analysis (Rotge et al., 2014). Evidence demonstrated that the dACC division mainly supported cognitive activities like cognitive control, error detection and conflict processing, while the vACC connected to the insula and orbitofrontal cortex primarily engaged in emotional processes such as affect state experience and regulation (Bush et al., 2000). However, dACC might involve in processing of individual emotion as well (Etkin, Egner, & Kalisch, 2011). Previously, in a two-choice prediction task and at low error rates, individuals with high trait anxiety showed increased activation in ACC, mainly the dACC, and medial PFC. These regions also had a positive correlation with anxiety score (Paulus et al., 2004). During a virtual ball-tossing game, the dACC was more active in exclusion condition than in inclusion one and correlated positively with individuals’ self-reported distress (Eisenberger, Lieberman, & Williams, 2003). In this study, individuals with higher CM and DA scores might be concerned and have doubt about denial and social exclusion from others. Then, all these could make the ACC act analogously in the social rejection experiment along with negative moods such as anxiety and depression. Hence, it would be reasonable to speculate that the relationship between higher CM and DA scores and larger GMV in the ACC could be from the result of a use-dependent brain volume alteration (Giuliani, Drabant, & Gross, 2011; May, 2011; Shackman et al., 2011).

In addition to the association with the ACC, the CM and DA scores were correlated with multiple distributed brain regions, and these regions act a significant role in some cognitive and emotional processes. Overall, increased rGMV in the MCC, insula, STG, and AG were also observed in participants with higher CM scores. And the higher DA scores both correlated with the larger GMV in the left triangular part of the IFG and SMA, and with the smaller GMV in the right triangular and orbital parts of the IFG and insula. OR was also positively correlated with the rGMV in the left triangular part of the IFG. Previous research has demonstrated the important engagement of all these brain regions in multiple cognitive and emotional processes. For instance, the insula has been known to be involved in somatic and emotional awareness (Giuliani, Drabant, Bhatnagar, & Gross, 2011), and the MCC, namely dACC (Stevens et al., 2011), has a strong association with cognitive control, action inhibition, punishment avoidance (Kohn et al., 2014), and negative emotion appraisal and expression (Etkin et al., 2011). But generally, all these distributed brain regions are involved in the processing of emotion (Giuliani, Drabant, Bhatnagar, & Gross, 2011; Giuliani, Drabant, & Gross, 2011; Kohn et al., 2014), implicating the latent neural substrates of emotion dysregulation in the maladaptive perfectionism individuals.

Relationship among brain structure variation, perfectionism, and negative emotion

Further mediation analysis revealed that the score of the DA acted a mediational mechanism in the correlation between the ACC and negative affect, including self-rating anxious and depressive symptoms. As depicted above, that the DA score was associated with the rGMV of the ACC might suggest that individuals with greater ACC would be more inclined to show concern about their deed and evaluate what they did so extremely critically that they would be more sensitive to negative emotion. Thus, the relationship between the rGMV of the ACC and individual difference in anxiety and depression could be induced by the indirect effect of DA. Inferred from the role of the ACC in the processing of cognitive control and negative affect (Shackman et al., 2011) and the main characteristic of DA, excessively severe self-evaluation combined with evaluative concerns (Frost et al., 1990), the positive relationship between the rGMV of the ACC and the score of DA seems to be reasonable. Notwithstanding that the mechanisms underlying the positive correlation between the larger GMV in the ACC and the higher level of anxiety and depression in this study remain unclear, prior findings of structural differences in adolescent mood and anxiety disorders might provide clues. For example, a recent study in adolescent anxiety reported that there was a larger dACC GMV in anxious adolescents modulated by the brain-derived neurotropic factor Val66Met Polymorphism (Mueller et al., 2013). However, there was disagreement in the relationship between the GMV in the ACC and the level of anxiety and depression. For example, a study reported that the dACC volume was positively related to cognitive reappraisal but not negative affect (Giuliani, Drabant, & Gross, 2011). Regardless of the inconclusive and controversial relationship between brain structure size and function intensity (Kanai, Dong, Bahrami, & Rees, 2011; Kanai & Rees, 2011; Takeuchi et al., 2013), there might be some possible inference for this result. First of all, two ACC subdivisions are included in our results, vACC and dACC, which are involved in the integration of cognitive and emotional information, especially cognitive control and negative affect, error-related activity as well as self-reported distress rather than the emotional regulation solely (Bush et al., 2000; Mohanty et al., 2007; Rotge et al., 2014). More important, there are also differences in the analytical method and materials. Indeed, we got our results from the correlation between perfectionism and whole-brain GMV in a larger sample instead of the ROIs drawn manually in previous work (Giuliani, Drabant, & Gross, 2011). There might be a use-dependent volume alteration in GMV of ACC in our sample due to the interaction among perfectionism, the processing of cognitive control and personal distress (Giuliani, Drabant, & Gross, 2011; May, 2011; Shackman et al., 2011). After all, our results substantiated that perfectionism could serve as an underlying mechanism explaining the association between the rGMV in the ACC and individual differences in anxious and depressive affect.

Limitations of current study

Several limitations to this study pertain to the methods for evaluation, the interpretation of current results, as well as the usage of MRI data. The first set of limitations concerns the assessment of perfectionism and self-reported negative emotion. Using multiple methods for evaluation might be better to confirm the results about neural mechanism of perfectionism (Bieling et al., 2004; Chang et al., 2008; Shafran & Mansell, 2001). For example, HMPS, including scales of self-oriented perfectionism, socially prescribed perfectionism, and other-oriented perfectionism, mainly focused on the interpersonal factor of perfectionism; it focused less on personal characteristics and described features of perfectionism in more detail (Zi & Zhou, 2007; Zi et al., 2010). Besides, we relied entirely on self-reported measures, which could be biased by depressed or anxious mood in the respondents’ temporal status, and it would be better to incorporate more objective measures in future research (Enns et al., 2002). Another possible limitation was the interpretation of current results. Caution should be used when interpreting the present results because our cross-sectional design does not allow us to draw conclusions about the directions of causality among brain structure variation, perfectionism, and anxiety and depression. It is particularly necessary to investigate this relationship in longitudinal research. Because no difference of perfectionism between gender was observed in our study, in accordance with previous findings (Besharat et al., 2014), we did not take the gender difference factor into account in this research. All data came from a large, nonclinical sample of Chinese undergraduate students, making these results not easily generalized to other populations. Therefore, it would be helpful to explore relationships in other general samples, such as clinical patients. A final set of limitations concerns the deficient usage of MRI data and analysis methods. In this study, we only used the VBM analysis to investigate the relationship among brain GMV changes, perfectionism, and negative emotion, which might obscure crucial results and influence on our interpretation of the current results and further understanding (Giuliani, Drabant, Bhatnagar, & Gross, 2011). Hence, based on existing work, an investigation from structural or functional networks of perfectionism should be taken into consideration. All limitations mentioned above should be addressed in future works—or instance, more comprehensive measures conducting longitudinal research and more MRI data analyses.

Conclusions

In conclusion, this preliminary study employing the VBM approach to explore the association among perfectionism, brain structure, and negative emotion offers considerable promise. First, we replicated the result of the significant correlation between perfectionism and negative emotion, which demonstrated that the association between maladaptive perfectionism and negative mood did exist in a large Chinese undergraduate population. Besides, based on the employment of the VBM approach, perfectionism showed an association with some brain regions in the GMV, specifically the ACC, revealing a potential neural mechanism for emotional dysregulation in individuals with maladaptive perfectionism. Finally, a follow-up mediation analysis revealed a mediational mechanism of the DA in the relation between the GMV of ACC and self-rating anxious, depressive affect. In sum, these findings not only provided evidence for the relationship between perfectionism and negative affect but also revealed the association among brain structure variation, perfectionism, and negative affect. Our finding indicated the potential role of perfectionism in the processing of negative emotion.