Faces convey crucial information for social interactions: They signal the emotional state of our interaction partners, and also provide important social information on their age, sex, and race. Recent research has revealed preferential processing of faces belonging to one’s same age group (own-age faces). They are looked at longer (Ebner, He, & Johnson, 2011), are more distracting (Ebner & Johnson, 2010), and are remembered more accurately (Harrison & Hole, 2009; Neumann, End, Luttmann, Schweinberger, & Wiese, 2015; Wiese, Wolff, Steffens, & Schweinberger, 2013; Wolff, Wiese, & Schweinberger, 2012; see Rhodes & Anastasi, 2012, and Wiese, Komes, & Schweinberger, 2013, for reviews), and their emotional expressions may be more accurately decoded (Malatesta, Izard, Culver, & Nicolich, 1987; but see Borod et al., 2004; Ebner et al., 2011; Ebner & Johnson, 2009; Ebner, Johnson, & Fischer, 2012; Ebner et al., 2013; Hühnel, Fölster, Werheid, & Hess, 2014; Murphy, Lehrfeld, & Isaacowitz, 2010) than faces from other age groups (other-age faces).

Neuroscientific research further suggests own-age effects in the neural processing of emotional faces. Specifically, functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) studies have revealed greater activity in the amygdala (Ebner et al., 2013; Wright et al., 2008), insula, and medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC; Ebner et al., 2013) when observers watch own-age versus other-age faces, possibly reflecting a higher interest in, perceived similarity with, and self-relevance of own-age faces. An interaction was reported for the ventral part of the mPFC, in which own-age effects were found only for neutral and happy, but not for angry, faces (Ebner et al., 2013).

Due to their excellent time resolution, event-related potentials (ERPs) are well-suited to investigate the neurofunctional processes underlying the own-age effect. Surprisingly, previous ERP studies exploring own-age effects have exclusively focused on neutral faces, despite evidence that facial emotion may modulate age effects on decoding accuracy (Ebner, He, & Johnson, 2011; Ebner & Johnson, 2009; Ebner, Riediger, & Lindenberger, 2010). Regarding the neural correlates of these processes, it has been suggested that emotion effects may even override age-of-face effects (Ebner et al., 2013).

The aim of the present study was to investigate the combined effects of facial age, observer age, and emotional expression with ERPs. Specifically, we wished to investigate whether these effects are related to earlier perceptual or later evaluative processing stages. In the following discussion, we briefly review ERP evidence regarding age and emotion effects on the early (N170) and late (late positive potential, LPP) face- and emotion-related ERP components. For a more comprehensive review of the ERP literature, also including findings on the repetition- and memory-related N250, which was considered less relevant here, we refer the reader to Wiese, Komes, et al. (2013).

The most widely studied face-sensitive ERP component is the N170 (see Eimer, 2011, for a review), an early negative deflection peaking around 170 ms over occipitotemporal sites that is associated with the structural processing of faces and is larger for faces than for object stimuli (e.g., Bentin, Allison, Puce, Perez, & McCarthy, 1996). Some previous studies have revealed increased N170 amplitudes for other-race than for own-race faces (Caharel et al., 2011; Gajewski, Schlegel, & Stoerig, 2008; Herrmann et al., 2007; Stahl, Wiese, & Schweinberger, 2008; Walker, Silvert, Hewstone, & Nobre, 2007; Wiese, 2012; Wiese, Kaufmann, & Schweinberger, 2014). Since own-race and own-age effects on ERPs are partly comparable (Ebner, He, Fichtenholtz, McCarthy, & Johnson, 2011; Wiese, Schweinberger, & Hansen, 2008), this finding might extend to own-age effects. However, so far, own-age effects on the N170 have not been confirmed for neutral faces (Wiese, Komes, & Schweinberger, 2012; Wiese et al., 2008), suggesting that partly different neural processes may underlie own-age and own-race effects. Instead, both younger and older observers showed higher N170 amplitudes for older than for younger faces (Wiese et al., 2012; Wiese et al., 2008), indicating that structural encoding may be more difficult for older faces, for both older and younger observers. Concerning emotion effects on the N170, the results are mixed: Whereas some studies have shown a modulation by emotion (Batty & Taylor, 2003; Blau, Maurer, Tottenham, & McCandliss, 2007; Krombholz, Schaefer, & Boucsein, 2007; Luo, Feng, He, Wang, & Luo, 2010; Rellecke, Palazova, Sommer, & Schacht, 2011), others have not (Balconi & Lucchiari, 2005; Eimer & Holmes, 2002, 2007; Eimer, Holmes, & McGlone, 2003; Holmes, Vuilleumier, & Eimer, 2003; Schupp et al., 2004). Thus, the question of whether emotional expressions moderate the N170 has not yet been clarified. Rellecke, Sommer, and Schacht (2012) suggested that “low-level” differences between facial expressions may account for the emotion effects reported in some studies.

The LPP is an enhanced positivity after 300 ms over parietal sites that reflects elaborate processing (Schacht & Sommer, 2009) and is more commonly used to study the processing of emotional faces. The LPP amplitude is enhanced for emotional stimuli with high intrinsic relevance (Schupp et al., 2004), and for facial expressions with negative as compared with positive valence (Schupp et al., 2004) and with higher as compared with lower emotional intensity (Duval, Moser, Huppert, & Simons, 2013; Recio, Schacht, & Sommer, 2014). The LPP is assumed to reflect sustained elaborate and affective processing, initiated by a high intrinsic relevance of the emotional stimuli (Cuthbert, Schupp, Bradley, Birbaumer, & Lang, 2000; Kayser et al., 1997; Schacht & Sommer, 2009; Schupp et al., 2000). There is evidence for own-race effects on LPP amplitudes (He, Johnson, Dovidio, & McCarthy, 2009), at least when the race of the face was task-relevant (Stahl, Wiese, & Schweinberger, 2010). Again, this finding might extend to own-age effects. To our knowledge, this was the first study to investigate own-age effects on LPPs. Several previous studies have examined the impact of age correspondence on the old–new recognition effect measured at similar time intervals (300–800 ms) at centroparietal electrode sites, considered to reflect recollection-based recognition. This old–new effect was enhanced for own-age as compared with other-age faces (Wiese et al., 2012; Wiese et al., 2008; but see Wolff et al., 2012). Focusing on amplitude differences, rather than the amplitude itself, these results allow for no direct conclusions concerning own-age effects on LPPs. Furthermore, previous research has not examined whether own-age effects on LPP amplitudes are modulated by the emotional expression of the face. The aim of the present study was to extend these results by analyzing the combined effects of age congruence and emotion on LPP amplitudes.

A further aim of the present study was to explore the possible mechanisms underlying own-age effects. Own-age effects on recognition memory have been explained by higher expertise due to a higher quantity of contact with same-aged individuals (Harrison & Hole, 2009; Rhodes & Anastasi, 2012; Wiese et al., 2012; Wiese, Wolff, et al., 2013; Wolff et al., 2012). As an alternative or additional explanation, the own-age effect may be related to the quality of contact, to motivational processes, and to higher identification with the own-age group (Hugenberg, Young, Bernstein, & Sacco, 2010; Young & Hugenberg, 2010). Thus, our aim was to explore whether own-age effects on ERPs were associated with the quantity and/or quality of contact and identification with the own-age group.

To sum up, the major aims of the present study were to examine whether own-age effects (1) are moderated by the emotional expression of the face, (2) emerge during earlier perceptual or later evaluative processing stages, and (3) are associated with in-group identification and with quantity or quality of contact. To explore these questions, we recorded an electroencephalogram (EEG) while younger and older observers watched pictures of younger and older faces showing sad and happy expressions, and analyzed age and emotion effects on one early (N170) and one later (LPP) ERP.

Method

Participants

One younger male and one older female observer were excluded from the analyses due to an excessive number of artifacts in the EEG. The remaining sample included 19 older (64–85 years old, M = 70.47, SD = 6.15; ten men, nine women) and 19 younger (18–30 years old, M = 24.84, SD = 3.85; nine men, ten women) observers. All had normal or corrected-to-normal vision and reported being free of neurological or psychiatric diseases. They were recruited via an online participant database at the Humboldt-Universität zu Berlin and by flyers distributed during a lecture at the Third Age University at Humboldt-Universität zu Berlin. They received €24 for participating. The study was approved by the ethics committee of Humboldt-Universität zu Berlin.

The younger observers had more years of education (younger observers: M = 12.50, SD = 0.89; older observers: M = 10.75, SD = 1.65), t(30) = 3.72, p = .001, and showed—as expected—higher levels of fluid intelligence, as assessed by the reasoning subtest (Subtest 3) of the LPS (Leistungsprüfsystem; Horn, 1983), in which nonmatching figures have to be identified among lines of logically related figures (younger observers: M = 29.42, SD = 5.49, older observers: M = 21.89, SD = 4.43), t(36) = 4.65, p < .001. However, the older observers were higher in crystallized intelligence, as assessed by the WST (Wortschatztest; Schmidt & Metzler, 1992), a German vocabulary test in which a target word has to be identified among five pseudowords (younger observers: M = 31.42, SD = 3.25, older observers: M = 35.16, SD = 2.81), t(36) = 3.79, p = .001. Relevant cognitive deficits could be excluded because none of the participants were below one standard deviation of the norm in these cognitive tests.

Stimulus material

Pictures of 56 younger (28 men, 28 women, age range: 19–31 years, M = 24.04, SD = 3.38) and 56 older (28 men, 28 women, age range: 69–80 years, M = 73.16, SD = 2.85) models showing sad and happy facial expressions were selected from the FACES database (Ebner et al., 2010). The intensity of the facial expressions was systematically varied, because previous research had shown own-age effects on emotion expression decoding accuracy for ambiguous (Malatesta et al., 1987) but not for intense (e.g., Ebner & Johnson, 2009; Ebner et al., 2012) expressions. Pictures of varying intensities (10%, 50%, and 90%) were created by morphing the chosen pictures to a neutral picture from the same model using Abrosoft FantaMorph 4. Two sets were created to control for idiosyncratic effects of the face models, each consisting of 56 models (28 older, 28 younger) displaying sad facial expressions and 56 different models displaying happy facial expressions, in 10%, 50%, and 90% intensities, respectively. To minimize eye movements, the original face pictures were cut to standardized image sections, with the external crops located close to the face. These images were presented on a computer monitor (33.5 × 27.2 cm) using the Presentation software. The viewing angle was 10° × 7.5°, and the screen resolution was 96.42 pixels per inch. Figure 1 shows examples of two images.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Trial event timing and stimulus examples

Experimental design

The EEG experiment was divided in two blocks, one for each emotion. The block design was chosen to avoid an influence of unequal perceived frequencies of each emotional valence on the LPP amplitudes (Cacioppo, Crites, Gardner, & Berntson, 1994; Donchin & Coles, 1988). Since previous research had pointed to age effects on the perceived valence of neutral expressions (Czerwon, Lüttke, & Werheid, 2011), we wanted to circumvent any interference of these effects with the age effects on LPPs. The order of blocks was counterbalanced across observers. At the beginning of the first block, three training trials were conducted with stimuli that were not used in the experiment. Each trial started with a fixation cross (1,750–2,250 ms), followed by a stimulus (1,000 ms). After the stimulus had disappeared, observers were asked to spontaneously and quickly classify the face as emotional or neutral by pressing one of two keys. The assignments of keys to the response options were counterbalanced across observers. If observers did not respond within 2,000 ms, an instruction asking them to react faster appeared on the screen; if no response was shown within the next 4,000 ms, the next trial started. The intertrial interval was 2,000 ms. In each block, observers were allowed to rest after each quarter of the stimuli had been presented. Figure 1 presents the trial event timing.

Procedure

Prior to the EEG experiment, observers worked on the WST and the reasoning subtest of the LPS, as described above. The EEG experiment was conducted in a light- and sound-attenuated room. After the EEG experiment, observers decoded the emotional facial expressions by rating the stimuli according to the intensity of their displayed sadness and happiness on 5-point scales, anchored with 0 (not at all) and 4 (very intense). Ratings were considered correct if the rating on the target emotion was higher than the rating on the nontarget emotion. Sad faces (10%, 50%, 90%) were rated correctly by 37%, 77%, and 94% of the observers, and happy faces (10%, 50%, 90%) by 20%, 88%, and 99% of the observers. After that, the observers worked on two graphical items measuring the perceived overlap of the self and the groups of younger and older adults (via the Inclusion of Other in the Self Scale [IOS] by Aron, Aron, & Smollan, 1992; adapted by Schubert & Otten, 2002), and were asked to indicate how often they had contact in person with older and with younger adults, respectively, on 8-point scales (1 = very seldom, 2 = once per year, 3 = 2–3 times per year, 4 = once per month, 5 = 2–3 times per month, 6 = once per week, 7 = 2–3 times per week, 8 = every day). Furthermore, observers described their quality of contact with older and younger adults, respectively, on the following 7-point bipolar continua, with higher scores representing higher quality of contact: unpleasant–pleasant, destructive–constructive, involuntary–voluntary, aloof–intimate, unequal–equal status, superficial–intensive, and impersonal–personal (Tzeng & Jackson, 1994). Mean scores were calculated for each target age group. The reliability of the latter questionnaire was good for the younger observers (younger targets: Cronbach’s α = .86; older targets: Cronbach’s α = .84), and acceptable for the older ones (younger targets: Cronbach’s α = .89; older targets: Cronbach’s α = .72).The duration of the experiment was approximately 1.5 h.

Electrophysiological recording

A 64-channel EEG was recorded using a Brain Amp EEG amplifier and the Brain Recorder software (Brain Products, Munich, Germany). Active Ag–AgCl electrodes were mounted in an electrode cap (ActiCap System). An electrooculogram (EOG) was recorded from FP2 and IO below the right eye, to control for eye artifacts. The TP9 electrode served as the initial common reference, and AFz as the ground. Impedances were kept below 5 kΩ. A sampling rate of 250 Hz and an online bandpass filter at 0.01 to 70 Hz were applied during recording. The time constant was set at 10 s.

Data analysis

The EEG data were corrected for eye movements and blink artifacts using the multiple-source eye correction method (surrogate method; Berg & Scherg, 1994), implemented in BESA 5.2 (Brain Electrical Source Analysis; MEGIS Software GmbH, Gräfelfing, Germany) and re-referenced offline to the average reference. A low-pass filter of 30 Hz was used. The continuous EEG data were segmented from –200 to 1,000 ms relative to stimulus onset, with the first 200 ms as the baseline. Artifacts were rejected semi-automatically using a gradient criterion of 50 μV, a difference criterion of 200 μV, and an amplitude criterion of 100 μV. The minimum number of trials per condition was 16, and the mean number of trials per condition was 25.9. The remaining trials were averaged for each experimental condition and recalculated to the average reference. The EOG channels were excluded. Average ERP waveforms were calculated for each experimental condition and filtered with a 10-Hz high cutoff for visual presentation. For the N170, peak analyses were carried out between 140 and 210 ms after stimulus onset. After visual inspection, the LPP amplitude was defined as the mean voltage in the interval of 300–600 ms after stimulus onset. Pooled waveforms were calculated separately for the N170 and LPP at the electrodes where the component showed its maximum, as well as at their respective contralateral homologues (N170: P7, P8, PO7, PO8, TP7, TP8, TP9, TP10; LPP: Pz, P1, P2, POz, PO3, PO4).Footnote 1

Results

N170

The results of the 2 (Observer Age) × 2 (Face Age) × 2 (Emotion) × 3 (Intensity) analysis of variance (ANOVA) on N170 amplitudes are displayed in Table 1. Concordant with previous results, main effects of observer age as well as face age indicated larger N170 amplitudes for older (M = –8.60, SD = 3.03) than for younger (M = –4.23, SD = 2.04) observers, as well as for older (M = –6.65, SD = 3.43) than for younger (M = –6.17, SD = 3.34) faces. Neither the Observer Age × Face Age interaction nor the Observer Age × Face Age × Emotion interaction reached significance. Thus, we found no own-age effects on N170 amplitudes.

Table 1 Results of ANOVAs on N170 and LPP amplitudes, as a function of observer age, face age, emotion, and intensity, as well as their interactions

LPP

The results of the 2 (Observer Age) × 2 (Face Age) × 2 (Emotion) × 3 (Intensity) ANOVA on LPP amplitudes are also displayed in Table 1. A main effect of observer age indicated larger LPP amplitudes for younger (M = 5.93, SD = 4.18) than for older (M = 3.58, SD = 1.94) observers. Furthermore, the Observer Age × Face Age × Emotion interaction was significant. Follow-up analyses revealed no significant Observer Age × Face Age interaction for happiness, F(1, 36) = 1.32, p = .259, η p 2 = .035, but this interaction did emerge for sadness, F(1, 36) = 5.02, p = .031, η p 2 = .122. LPP amplitudes were higher for sad faces in the own-age group, as compared with the other-age group. However, this difference was only significant for older observers (younger faces: M = 3.27, SD = 2.04; older faces: M = 3.61, SD = 1.99), t(18) = 2.92, p = .009, but not for younger observers (younger faces: M = 6.03, SD = 4.30; older faces: M = 5.87, SD = 4.37), t(18) = 0.85, p = .407. Grand-mean ERPs for sad faces as a function of observer age and face age are depicted in Fig. 2.

Fig. 2
figure 2

Grand-mean N170 and LPP amplitudes for pooled electrodes, shown separately for sadness and happiness, as a function of observer age and face age

In addition, the Face Age × Intensity interaction was significant. Follow-up t tests revealed that LPP amplitudes were higher for older (M = 4.96, SD = 3.54) than for younger (M = 4.57, SD = 3.42) faces at 90% intensity, t(37) = 2.88, p = .007, but did not differ at the 50%, t(37) = 1.97, p = .057, and 10%, t(37) = 1.04, p = .306, intensities.

Apart from that, the Emotion × Intensity interaction reached significance. For happiness, follow-up t tests revealed higher LPP amplitudes at 10% (M = 4.97, SD = 3.60) than at 90% (M = 4.68, SD = 3.35) intensity, t(37) = 2.39, p = .022. For sadness, contrariwise, amplitudes were lower at 10% (M = 4.51, SD = 3.48) than at 90% (M = 4.85, SD = 3.65) intensity, t(37) = 2.32, p = .026. LPP amplitudes did not differ between the 50% and 10% or the 50% and 90% intensities, for either happiness or sadness (all ps > .05).Footnote 2

Facial expression decoding

Participants’ facial expression decoding accuracy was analyzed in a 2 (Observer Age) × 2 (Face Age) × 3 (Intensity) × 2 (Emotion) ANOVA on the proportions of accurate answers in the rating task after the EEG experiment (see Table 2). There was no general or emotion-specific own-age effect on facial expression decoding, as indicated by nonsignificant Observer Age × Face Age and Observer Age × Face Age × Emotion interactions. However, we did observe a significant Observer Age × Face Age × Emotion × Intensity interaction. Separate 2 (Observer Age) × 2 (Face Age) × 3 (Intensity) ANOVAs for happiness and sadness revealed no significant Observer Age × Face Age × Intensity interaction for happiness, F(1, 54) = 2.79, p = .085, η p 2 = .072, but this interaction did emerge for sadness, F(2, 62) = 7.38, p = .002, η p 2 = .170. Separate 2 (Face Age) × 3 (Intensity) ANOVAs for younger and older observers for sadness revealed significant interactions for both younger, F(2, 30) = 21.87, p < .001, η p 2 = .549, and older, F(2, 32) = 73.05, p < .001, η p 2 = .802, observers. Both younger, t(18) = 5.55, p < .001, and older, t(18) = 5.50, p < .001, observers decoded 50%-intense sad expressions more accurately in younger (younger observers: M = .91, SD = .08; older observers: M = .79, SD = .15) than in older (younger observers: M = .74, SD = .15; older observers: M = .62, SD = .19) faces. For younger observers, we found no differences between the face age groups for sad expressions of 10%, t(18) = 1.14, p = .269, or 90%, t(18) = 1.54, p = .142, intensity. Older observers decoded 10%-intense sad expressions more accurately in older (M = .43, SD = .23) than in younger (M = .27, SD = .18) faces, t(18) = 4.12, p = .001, but they decoded 90%-intense expressions more accurately in younger (M = .95, SD = .07) than in older (M = .89, SD = .12) faces, t(18) = 2.87, p = .010. Thus, older observers showed an own-age advantage for sad expressions of low intensity. To examine the relationship of this own-age effect to the own-age effect on LPP amplitudes, we first calculated own-age effect measures for decoding accuracy as well as for LPP amplitudes for sadness—by, for example, subtracting the decoding accuracy of sadness for the other-age group from the decoding accuracy for the own-age group—and examined the correlations between these measures. The correlation of this own-age advantage in older observers with the own-age effect in LPPs for sad faces was in the expected positive direction, but not significant, r(18) = .111, p = .226.

Table 2 Results of ANOVAS on hit rates, as a function of observer age, face age, intensity, emotion, and their interactions

A set of 2 (Observer Age) × 2 (Target Age) ANOVAs on mean scores from the IOS and from the questionnaires on the quality and quantity of contact with older and younger targets revealed significant main effects of target age for the IOS, F(1, 36) = 11.29, p = .002, η p 2 = .239, and quality of contact,Footnote 3 F(1, 34) = 4.33, p = .045, η p 2 = .113, but not for the quantity of contact, F(1, 36) = 3.98, p = .054, η p 2 = .100. The main effects were qualified by significant Observer Age × Target Age interactions [IOS: F(1, 36) = 19.21, p < .001, η p 2 = .348; quality of contact: F(1, 34) = 10.12, p = .003, η p 2 = .229; quantity of contact: F(1, 36) = 45.05, p < .001, η p 2 = .556]. The results of follow-up t tests are displayed in Table 3. Observers identified more with the own-age group and reported higher quality of contact with the own-age than with the other-age group, but this difference was only significant for younger observers. Furthermore, both younger and older observers reported higher quantity of contact with the own-age than with the other-age group. No significant main effects of observer age were apparent [IOS: F(1, 36) = 1.80, p = .189, η p 2 = .048; quality of contact: F(1, 34) = 0.11, p = .744, η p 2 = .003; quantity of contact: F(1, 36) = 0.31, p = .583, η p 2 = .008].

Table 3 Means from questionnaires on the quality and quantity of contact and on identification with the two age groups, for younger and older observers

To examine the correlation with the LPP own-age effect, we first calculated own-age effect measures for quality and quantity of contact, for IOS scores, and for LPP amplitudes for sadness, by, for example, subtracting the quantity of contact with the other-age group from the quantity of contact with the own-age group. We observed a significant positive correlation between the LPP own-age effects for sadness and quality of contact, r(34) = .287, p = .045 (one-tailed tests). The correlations of the LPP own-age effect with those related to the IOS, r(36) = .183, p = .135, and quantity of contact, r(36) = .041, p = .400, however, were not significant.

Discussion

The present study was to our knowledge the first experiment investigating the combined effects of observers’ and facial age and facial emotion on ERPs. As the main result of our study, we found an own-age effect on the LPP for sadness, but not for happiness. This own-age effect emerged for older, but not for younger, observers and related to the quality of contact with the own-age group.

Our finding of an own-age effect in older observers is in line with previous reports in older observers of more pronounced own-age effects on amygdala activity when watching neutral (Wright et al., 2008) or emotional (Ebner et al., 2013) faces. On the basis of these findings, Ebner et al. (2013) suggested that age may be a more salient and relevant feature for older adults, who may be more frequently reminded of their age because of age-related declines in various domains of functioning. However, so far, this explanation is only speculative and needs empirical investigation.

Our finding that the own-age effect on the neural processing of faces was moderated by emotional expression confirms and extends recent findings obtained with fMRI (Ebner et al., 2013). Exploiting the higher temporal resolution of the ERP method, we could show that these own-age effects coincide with alterations of the LPP, reflecting late evaluative stages of information processing (Herbert, Kissler, Junghöfer, Peyk, & Rockstroh, 2006; Schacht & Sommer, 2009). Similar LPP effects have been reported for own-race effects in response to neutral faces (He et al., 2009). In contrast, we found no own-age effects on earlier (N170) perceptual processing stages, confirming and extending previous studies on own-age effects in response to neutral faces (see Wiese, Komes, et al., 2013, for a review). Our findings suggest that age congruence influences later stages in the processing of emotional faces, which are commonly associated with the faces’ motivational relevance (Cuthbert et al., 2000; Schacht & Sommer, 2009; Schupp et al., 2007), rather than with their structural encoding. As previous research reported own-race effects on N170 amplitudes (e.g., Caharel et al., 2011; Wiese et al., 2014), our results support the view that the neural processes underlying own-age and own-race effects partly differ (Wiese, Komes, et al., 2013).

The own-age effect for sad faces in older observers suggests preferential processing of sad faces in old-age peers. Motivational sources as well as behavioral consequences of such a preference are conceivable. Their direction, however, is unclear, since this preference might facilitate both social support and avoidance of social interactions with sad peers. The ERP measures obtained in our study leave this point open for future studies that could combine physiological parameters with behavioral and motivational measures suited to measure appetence or valence.

Concerning the possible mechanisms underlying own-age effects, observers identified themselves more with the own-age group and reported a higher quality and quantity of contact with the own-age group. Furthermore, the significant positive correlation between the own-age effects on LPP amplitudes and on quality of contact indicates that individuals with close contact to age peers show ERP evidence of enhanced motivational relevance of sad faces from the same age group. However, the quantity of contact and identification with the own-age-group were not related to the own-age effect on LPPs. This is in contrast to previous reports of the quantity of contact being related to effects of facial age on ERPs and recognition memory (Wolff et al., 2012). In the present study, the quantity of contact was assessed with a one-item questionnaire. Future research should examine whether significant correlations with LPP amplitudes would emerge for more reliable measures. As an alternative explanation, Wolff et al. (2012) did not examine emotional stimuli, but ERP old–new effects, which may be more sensitive to repeated prior exposure, and thus to quantitative aspects of contact with age peers.

Our finding that the own-age effect was specific for sadness opens up several considerations regarding the particular significance of this emotion, as distinct from happiness. Since the LPP has been associated with the intrinsic relevance of stimuli (Schupp et al., 2004), our result may reflect a higher relevance of sad faces when they are from own-aged rather than other-aged individuals. Happiness may differ from sadness in this respect because a smile may strongly signal affiliation, and thereby overrule group boundaries (van der Schalk et al., 2011), rendering happy faces of own-aged and other-aged individuals equally relevant. This is in line with previous reports of attenuated in-group effects for happy faces. Some studies examining facial mimicry—that is, the imitation of emotional expressions—have shown in-group effects for negative, but not for positive, emotions (Bourgeois & Hess, 2008; van der Schalk et al., 2011), and the own-race effect in emotion decoding accuracy was smallest for happiness (see Elfenbein & Ambady, 2002, for a review). Also in line with this assumption, in a recent study of our own group, we found an emotion-specific own-age advantage in decoding accuracy for sadness, but not for happiness (Fölster, Hess, Hühnel, & Werheid, 2015). A possible limitation of the present study is its focus on one positive and one negative emotional category, due to methodological particularities of the ERP method such as the high sensitivity of the LPP to imbalanced frequencies of stimulus categories. Sadness was chosen on the basis of our previous research (Fölster et al., 2015). Opposing other negative emotions to happy faces with respect to the own-age-effect on ERPs will be a valuable topic for future research. Furthermore, future research may examine whether our results may expand to spontaneous or dynamic facial expressions, and to middle-aged adults.

Another interesting question for further research will be whether the own-age effect extends to implicit behavioral measures such as facial mimicry, or explicit measures such as decoding accuracy. A recent study of our group showed no own-age effect on facial mimicry (Hühnel et al., 2014), but results have been mixed for decoding accuracy. As a possible explanation, an own-age effect on decoding accuracy was only found when expressions were difficult to decode. Thus, previous studies with comparatively low decoding accuracy have shown an own-age effect (Fölster et al., 2015; Malatesta et al., 1987), whereas those with higher decoding accuracy did not (Borod et al., 2004; Ebner, He, & Johnson, 2011; Ebner & Johnson, 2009; Ebner et al., 2012; Ebner et al., 2013; Hühnel et al., 2014). Also in line with this assumption, in the present study, we only found an own-age effect on decoding accuracy for sad expressions of low intensity, and only for older observers. As a possible explanation of this pattern, observers may need more cognitive resources to decode difficult emotional stimuli, possibly leading to a higher influence of motivational factors. It is conceivable that the own-age effect might in this way affect decoding accuracy in everyday interactions, in which ambiguous or mixed expressions often occur. However, more research investigating own-age effects on decoding accuracy for expressions of varying intensities will be needed to elucidate this point.

The present study was the first analyzing the combined effects of facial age and emotion on ERPs. Future replications of the study including a middle-aged sample and larger sample sizes are recommended. To overcome a further limiting aspect, the relevance of the frequency of contact should be assessed more precisely in future studies, in order to reliably examine a possible relationship to own-age effects on LPPs.

In summary, we found an own-age effect on ERPs for sad, but not for happy, faces, which may be due either to a higher perceived relevance of sadness shown by own-aged individuals, or to the strong impact of smiling, signaling affiliation across age group boundaries. This own-age effect for sadness was found in later, evaluative (Herbert et al., 2006; Schacht & Sommer, 2009), but not in earlier, perceptual processing stages, and was related to the quality, but not the quantity, of contact or identification with the own-age group. This pattern of results supports the view that higher motivational relevance (Cuthbert et al., 2000; Kayser et al., 1997; Schacht & Sommer, 2009; Schupp et al., 2000) may underlie preferential processing of sad facial expressions in own-age peers.