Skip to main content
Log in

Action does not drive visual biases in peri-tool space

  • Published:
Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Observers experience visual biases in the area around handheld tools. These biases may occur when active use leads an observer to incorporate a tool into the body schema. However, the visual salience of a handheld tool may instead create an attentional prioritization that is not reliant on body-based representations. We investigated these competing explanations of near-tool visual biases in two experiments during which participants performed a target detection task. Targets could appear near or far from a tool positioned next to a display. In Experiment 1, participants showed facilitation in detecting targets that appeared near a simple handheld rake tool regardless of whether they first used the rake to retrieve objects, but participants who only viewed the tool without holding it were no faster to detect targets appearing near the rake than targets that appeared on the opposite side of the display. In a second experiment, participants who held a novel magnetic tool again showed a near-tool bias even when they refrained from using the tool. Taken together, these results suggest active use is unnecessary, but visual salience is not sufficient, to introduce visual biases in peri-tool space.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. We present analyses collapsed across the factor of cue validity for ease of presentation. The results of a 3 × 2 × 2 × 2 mixed-factors ANOVA including the validity factor are available in the Supplemental Materials section.

  2. The factor of target distance corresponds to the target’s location with respect to the rake when it was held or placed next to the display (near versus far).

  3. We present analyses collapsed across the factor of cue validity for ease of presentation. The results of a 2 × 2 × 2 × 2 mixed-factors ANOVA including the validity factor are available in the Supplemental Materials section.

  4. The small (i.e., 5–20 ms) but statistically significant near-tool effects we observed were consistent with previous findings using this paradigm (McManus & Thomas, 2020; Reed et al., 2010).

References

  • Abrams, R. A., Davoli, C. C., Du, F., Knapp, W. H., III, & Paull, D. (2008). Altered vision near the hands. Cognition, 107(3), 1035–1047.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Agauas, S. J., Jacoby, M., & Thomas, L. E. (2020). Near-hand effects are robust: Three OSF pre-registered replications of visual biases in perihand space. Visual Cognition, 28, 192–204.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baccarini, M., Martel, M., Cardinali, L., Sillan, O., Farnè, A., & Roy, A. C. (2014). Tool use imagery triggers tool incorporation in the body schema. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 492.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Bell, J. D., & Macuga, K. L. (2022). Are tools truly incorporated as an extension of the body representation?: Assessing the evidence for tool embodiment. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 29, 343–368.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Berlucchi, G., & Aglioti, S. (1997). The body in the brain: Neural bases of corporeal awareness. Trends in Neurosciences, 20, 560–564.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Berti, A., & Frassinetti, F. (2000). When far becomes near: Remapping of space by tool use. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 12, 415–420.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Biggio, M., Bisio, A., Avanzino, L., Ruggeri, P., & Bove, M. (2017). This racket is not mine: The influence of the tool-use on peripersonal space. Neuropsychologia, 103, 54–58.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Brozzoli, C., Pavani, F., Urquizar, C., Cardinali, L., & Farnè, A. (2009). Grasping actions remap peripersonal space. NeuroReport, 20(10), 913–917.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bruno, V., Carpinella, I., Rabuffetti, M., De Giuli, L., Sinigaglia, C., Garbarini, F., & Ferrarin, M. (2019). How tool-use shapes body metric representation: Evidence from motor training with and without robotic assistance. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 12.

  • Canzoneri, E., Ubaldi, S., Rastelli, V., Finisguerra, A., Bassolino, M., & Serino, A. (2013). Tool-use reshapes the boundaries of body and peripersonal space representations. Experimental Brain Research, 228(1), 25–42.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cosman, J., & Vecera, S. (2010). Attention affects visual perceptual processing near the hand. Psychological Science, 21, 1254–1258.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Costantini, M., Ambrosini, E., Tieri, G., Sinigaglia, C., & Committeri, G. (2010). Where does an object trigger an action? An investigation about affordances in space. Experimental Brain Research, 207, 95–103.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Farnè, A., & Làdavas, E. (2000). Dynamic size-change of hand peripersonal space following tool use. NeuroReport, 11, 1645–1649.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Farnè, A., Bonifazi, S., & Làdavas, E. (2005a). The role played by tool-use and tool-length on the plastic elongation of peri-hand space: A single case study. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 22, 408–418.

  • Farnè, A., Iriki, A., & Làdavas, E. (2005b). Shaping multisensory action–space with tools: Evidence from patients with cross-modal extinction. Neuropsychologia, 43(2), 238–248.

  • Graziano, M. S., & Cooke, D. F. (2006). Parieto-frontal interactions, personal space, and defensive behavior. Neuropsychologia, 44(6), 845–859.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gozli, D., & Brown, L. (2011). Agency and control for the integration of a virtual tool into the peripersonal space. Perception, 40, 1309–1319.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gozli, D., West, G., & Pratt, J. (2012). Hand position alters vision by biasing processing through different visual pathways. Cognition, 124, 244–250.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Holmes, N. P. (2012). Does tool use extend peripersonal space? A review and re-analysis. Experimental Brain Research, 218(2), 273–282.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Holmes, N. P., Calvert, G. A., & Spence, C. (2007a). Tool use changes multisensory interactions in seconds: Evidence from the crossmodal congruency task. Experimental Brain Research, 183(4), 465–476.

  • Holmes, N. P., Sanabria, D., Calvert, G. A., & Spence, C. (2007b). Tool-use: Capturing multisensory spatial attention or extending multisensory peripersonal space? Cortex, 43(3), 469–489.

  • Humphreys, G., Riddoch, M. J., Forti, S., & Ackroyd, K. (2004). Action influences spatial perception: Neuropsychological evidence. Visual Cognition, 11(2/3), 401–427.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Iriki, A., Tanaka, M., & Iwamura, Y. (1996). Coding of modified body schema during tool use by macaque postcentral neurones. NeuroReport, 7(14), 2325–2330.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Làdavas, E., di Pellegrino, G., Farnè, A. M., & Zeloni, G. (1998). Neuropsychological evidence of an integrated visuotactile representation of peripersonal space in humans. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 10, 581–589.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Maravita, A., Husain, M., Clarke, K., & Driver, J. (2001). Reaching with a tool extends visual–tactile interactions into far space: Evidence from cross-modal extinction. Neuropsychologia, 39(6), 580–585.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Maravita, A., Spence, C., Kennett, S., & Driver, J. (2002). Tool-use changes multimodal spatial interactions between vision and touch in normal humans. Cognition, 83(2), B25–B34.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • McManus, R., & Thomas, L. E. (2020). Vision is biased near hand-held, but not remotely operated tools. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 82, 4038–4057.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Posner, M. I., Walker, J. A., Friedrich, F. A., & Rafal, R. D. (1987). How do the parietal lobes direct covert attention? Neuropsychologia, 25(1), 135–145.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Reed, C. L., Betz, R., Garza, J. P., & Roberts, R. J. (2010). Grab it! Biased attention in functional hand and tool space. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 72(1), 236–245.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Reed, C. L., Grubb, J. D., & Steele, C. (2006). Hands up: Attentional prioritization of space near the hand. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 32(1), 166.

  • Rossetti, A., Romano, D., Bolognini, N., & Maravita, A. (2015). Dynamic expansion of alert responses to incoming painful stimuli following tool use. Neuropsychologia, 70, 486–494.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ruttle, J., ’t Hart, B., & Henriques, D. (2021). Implicit motor learning within three trials. Scientific Reports, 11, 1627.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Serino, A., Canzoneri, E., Marzolla, M., Di Pellegrino, G., & Magosso, E. (2015). Extending peripersonal space representation without tool-use: Evidence from a combined behavioral-computational approach. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience, 9, 4.

  • Spence, C., Pavani, F., & Driver, J. (2004a). Spatial constraints on visual-tactile cross-modal distractor congruency effects. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 4, 148–169.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Spence, C., Pavani, F., Maravita, A., & Holmes, N. (2004b). Multisensory contributions to the 3-D representation of visuotactile peripersonal space in humans: Evidence from the crossmodal congruency task. Journal of Physiology-Paris, 98, 171–189.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Thomas, L. E. (2015). Grasp posture alters visual processing biases near the hands. Psychological Science, 26, 625–632.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Thomas, L. E. (2017). Action experience drives visual-processing biases near the hands. Psychological Science, 28, 124–131.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Yue, Z., Bischof, G. N., Zhou, X., Spence, C., & Röder, B. (2009). Spatial attention affects the processing of tactile and visual stimuli presented at the tip of a tool: An event-related potential study. Experimental Brain Research, 193(1), 119–128.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

This work was supported by a grant from the National Science Foundation (BCS #1556336).

Open practices statement

Raw and trimmed data are available for all experiments are available (https://osf.io/u58q7/?view_only=090cd14fbb954f04a61702fe2d5f9381). Neither of the experiments were preregistered.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Laura E. Thomas.

Additional information

Publisher’s note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Supplementary information

ESM 1

(DOCX 24 kb)

Rights and permissions

Springer Nature or its licensor (e.g. a society or other partner) holds exclusive rights to this article under a publishing agreement with the author(s) or other rightsholder(s); author self-archiving of the accepted manuscript version of this article is solely governed by the terms of such publishing agreement and applicable law.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

McManus, R., Thomas, L.E. Action does not drive visual biases in peri-tool space. Atten Percept Psychophys 86, 525–535 (2024). https://doi.org/10.3758/s13414-023-02826-x

Download citation

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.3758/s13414-023-02826-x

Keywords

Navigation