Introduction

Obesity has doubled across 70 countries over the last 40 years [1], and physical inactivity is one of the most common behavioral factors [2]. Among different age groups, college students are a high-risk group for obesity and physical inactivity, which are, in turn, susceptible to developing non-communicable diseases (NCDs) later in life [3]. A high proportion of college students are characterized by sedentary behavior [4]. They tend to spend most of their time on activities such as studying, sitting in lectures, using computers or cell phones, surfing the Internet, or playing video games, which may expose them to physical inactivity and weight gain. In addition, entering a new challenging chapter of life immediately after adolescence, college students undergo physiological and psychological stressors influencing many aspects of their health behaviors and lifestyles, including nutritional/exercise habits, which could lead to adverse health outcomes such as overweight and obesity [5].

According to a secular trend study investigated across two waves, the Nutrition and Health Survey in Taiwan (NAHSIT 1993–1996 and NAHSIT 2005–2008), the prevalence of overweight and obesity increased in both men and women [6]. Subsequently, another study [7] combined three sets of data from NAHSIT 1993–1996, 2005–2008, and 2013–2016 to examine the prevalence of overweight and obesity. The results indicated an increase in the prevalence of obesity among men but not in women, which was especially significant among young and middle-aged populations. However, the long-term relationship between the escalating prevalence of obesity and its potential risk factors, such as weight stigma, has not been examined in Taiwan.

Many adverse health outcomes due to being overweight or obese may result from personal experience, perception, anticipation, and internalization of weight stigma [8]. Weight stigma delineates negative beliefs, attitudes, denigration, or social castigation directed toward individuals because of their weight [9]. Weight stigma usually manifests through stereotypes, prejudicial attitudes, and discriminatory actions [8, 9]. Two weight stigma constructs are distinctly defined: perceived and internalized weight stigma. Perceived and experienced weight stigma are often applied interchangeably to describe how others think about the individual’s weight. On the other hand, internalized weight stigma refers to accepting societal weight bias as true of oneself [10]. Just as the obesity rate has been increasing in recent decades, weight stigma has also escalated [11] and is increasingly recognized as a global health issue [12].

Perceived weight stigma indicates perceived prejudicial attitudes toward and social devaluation of individuals identified as having excess body weight or not complying with the prevailing thin body ideal [13, 14]. These unjust attitudes are rooted in negative stereotypes, such as judging obese people as clumsy, incompetent, or lacking in determination [15]. For mistreated individuals, perceived weight stigma may elicit physiological and behavioral consequences, intensify declining metabolic health, and thus augment weight gain [9, 16]. High perceived weight stigma is significantly related to impairments and disturbances in mental health [17].

Furthermore, stigmatizing portrayals of obese individuals are pervasively present in the mass media. Characters with larger body sizes are usually depicted with prejudice and devaluation and are seen as associated with stereotypical behaviors such as sedentary behavior or overeating [18]. These images could reinforce arbitrary perceptions that obesity stems from personal choices, which may result in a lower level of public interest in obesity-related preventions or interventions as well as more significant weight stigma among the general public [19].

Perceived stigma may be converted to internalized weight stigma when perpetrators take stigmatizing actions toward their victims [20]. Subsequently, internalized weight stigma would lead to negative health outcomes and mediate the association between perceived weight stigma and health outcomes [21]. Internalized weight stigma is defined as acceptance of negative stereotypes about weight, applying negative stereotypes personally, and devaluing oneself due to self-defining as overweight [22]. Also, internalized weight stigma could be transmitted negatively, such as through societal discrimination or devaluation, and subsequently causes psychosocial problems, including aggressive behavior, lower self-esteem, depression, suicide ideation, and anxiety [23]; on the other hand, internalized weight stigma could be conveyed in seemingly positive ways. For instance, medical advice for weight loss strategies emphasizes that individuals should be responsible for weight management. Thus, medical advice encourages individuals to exert their willpower to exercise more or eat less.

Such encouragements that may be perceived as motivations for weight loss may provoke feelings of shame [24, 25] and self-blame [26], intensifying internalized weight stigma. In addition, given that body image is greatly influenced by social desirability and experiences [27], individuals with larger body shapes tend to have higher levels of body dissatisfaction [28] and body image disturbance [29], which may heighten internalized weight stigma as well. Internalized weight stigma increases vulnerability to psychological distress, and the harm could intensify internalized weight stigma in turn. The long-term consequences of internalized weight stigma include psychological problems and the risk of future weight gain and obesity [30].

Hence, internalized weight stigma may be an unintended consequence of anti-obesity efforts and hinder the intended effect [14]. Also, literature has indicated a gradient relationship between internalized weight bias and body weight [13]. A large body of literature has articulated that perceived weight stigma is related to high weight bias internalization [31] and that perceived and internalized weight stigma influences health outcomes [8, 10, 32]. Different cross-sectional studies have investigated the impacts of weight stigma in general, perceived weight stigma, and internalized weight stigma on various health outcomes [33,34,35,36].

Given the link between perceived and internalized weight stigma and health problems mentioned above, further exploration of how weight stigma gradually penetrates individuals’ perceptions and impacts different health outcomes is warranted. However, longitudinal research still needs to prospectively examine the effect of perceived and internalized weight stigma on health outcomes. Furthermore, more is required to know about the progress of individuals perceiving weight stigma, internalizing it, and the potentially sequent impact on health outcomes in a trajectory model. In addition, the scope of this research should be widened and explored in contexts other than just Western culture.

Several research studies have explored the role of internalized weight stigma as a mediator of the association between perceived weight stigma and biopsychosocial outcomes such as psychological well-being and physical health [37], body shame [38], and exercise, but not changes in body mass index BMI, an objective biomarker that plays a decisive role in health [39]. Considering the growing prevalence of obesity worldwide, it is critical to further assess weight stigma. Weight stigma has been found to be associated with increased BMI, such that people with obesity often perceive and internalize discrimination because of their weight. In other words, weight stigma is the consequence of increased BMI [40]. Compared to prior studies in which BMI was controlled to avoid its biological correlate of weight stigma [10, 29, 41], the present study defined body mass index (BMI) changes as a critical health outcome.

In a longitudinal research design, data on changes in BMI were examined to explore the proposed model. Also, most previous weight stigma-related literature was conducted in Western contexts [13, 42, 43]. Thus, exploring the model within Eastern societies is essential. An important strategy in fighting the obesity epidemic involves identifying and addressing predisposing factors.

Aims

This study aimed to underscore the negative consequences of weight stigma on weight-related health outcomes. In a longitudinal study, this investigation aimed to examine the role of internalized weight stigma as a mediator of the relationship between perceived weight stigma and changes in BMI, controlling for psychological distress.

Methods

Participants, data collection, and ethical considerations

The inclusion criteria of the eligibility were (i) studying in a college program; (ii) having the ability to read written Chinese; and (iii) being aged 20 years or above (i.e., a legal age to provide informed consent without a guardian in Taiwan). All participants provided an e-form informed consent to confirm their willingness to participate in the present study. No specific exclusion criteria were used for the eligibility. Moreover, the Institutional Review Board in the Chi Mei Medical Center (IRB Serial No.: 11007-006) and the Human Research Ethics Committee in the National Cheng Kung University (Approval No.: 109-551-2) approved the study.

The data collection was conducted using Google Forms. More specifically, a link and a QR code connected to Google Forms were distributed to the target participants with the help of the authors’ colleagues teaching or studying at a university in Taiwan. The link was then shared among the participants interested in the present study; the participants were informed that they could forward the link to their university student friends. Due to the nature of the snowballing sampling method, the current authors could not trace all the distributed links and were unable to calculate the response rate. Therefore, the authors only set a required sample size for the present study at baseline (i.e., 1000 participants).

After logging in to Google Forms, each participant could read the study purpose, participant eligibility, incentive information (about 3.3 USD were provided for each time of participation), and participant rights in detail. Then, they were asked to click the ‘agree’ button to represent their e-form informed consent if they wanted to participate. If the participant did not want to provide e-form informed consent, they were advised to click the ‘disagree’ button, and the survey ended. Those who clicked the ‘agree’ button could read the survey questions and responses. Also, the participants could discontinue the online survey at any time. The participants were asked to provide their email addresses and mobile phone numbers at baseline for the authors to follow up with them three months later. The baseline survey was conducted between August and September 2021, and the follow-up was conducted between November and December 2021. Although the data collection periods (including both baseline and follow-up surveys) overlapped with the COVID-19 pandemic, there were no severe impacts from COVID-19 because the entire survey period only had a relatively minor outbreak in Taiwan [44,45,46,47].

Measures

Body mass index (BMI) and demographic characteristics

Participants’ BMI was calculated using their self-reported weight (in kg) and height (in cm). BMI was calculated for baseline and follow-up separately. Then, change in BMI was calculated using the follow-up BMI value minus baseline BMI value. Moreover, the participants answered their demographics using multiple choice questions for their degree level (undergraduate or postgraduate), major (see Table 1 for details), gender (male or female), age (in year), monthly income (see Table 1 for more information), current cigarette user (yes or no), current alcohol user (yes or no), and diagnosis of any chronic diseases (yes or no).

Table 1 Participants’ characteristics (N = 974)

Internalized weight stigma

The Weight Self-Stigma Questionnaire (WSSQ) was used to assess participants’ weight-related self-stigma, referring to internalized weight stigma in this study. The WSSQ contains 12 items with a five-point Likert scale response (1 = completely disagree; 5 = completely agree), and a higher score indicates a higher level of internalized weight stigma. A sample item is “I’ll always go back to being overweight”. The original WSSQ has good psychometric properties [48], and the WSSQ has been translated into different language versions with satisfactory psychometric properties [49,50,51,52] including the Chinese version [53]. The present study’s data indicated that the WSSQ used in the present sample had excellent internal consistency (α = 0.93 at baseline; 0.95 at follow-up).

Perceived weight stigma

The Perceived Weight Stigma Scale (PWSS) was used to assess participants’ perceived weight stigma. The PWSS contains 10 items with a dichotomous scale response (0 = yes; 1 = no), and a higher score indicates a higher level of perceived weight stigma [54]. A sample item is “Because of your weight, people act as if you are inferior”. The Chinese version of PWSS has good psychometric properties [33, 35, 36, 53]. The present study’s data indicated that the PWSS used in the present sample had excellent internal consistency (Kuder–Richardson Formula 20 [KR-20] = 0.87 at baseline; 0.93 at follow-up).

Psychological distress

The Depression, Anxiety, Stress Scale − 21 (DASS-21) was used to assess participants’ psychological distress in three forms: depression, anxiety, and stress. The DASS-21 contains 21 items with a four-point Likert scale response (0 = did not apply to me at all; 3 = applied to me very much), and a higher score indicates a higher level of psychological distress. A sample item is “I found it hard to wind down”. The original DASS-21 has good psychometric properties [55], and the DASS-21 has been translated into different language versions with satisfactory psychometric properties, including the Chinese version [56,57,58]. The present study’s data indicated that the DASS-21 used in the present sample had excellent internal consistency (α = 0.96 at baseline; 0.98 at follow-up).

Data analysis

Before the main analyses were performed, missing values were evaluated for whether they were missing completely at random (MCAR) via Little’s MCAR test [59]. If MCAR is not supported, the missing values were evaluated if they were missing at random (MAR) via independent t-tests on the studied variables at baseline. More specifically, if significant differences in the studied variables at baseline were identified between the participants who were retained in the study or lost to follow-up, the MAR is not supported. Findings of the main analyses are considered not having serious biases when the loss to follow-up is less than 60% under either MCAR or MAR condition [60]. Additionally, multiple imputation was used to impute the missing values, given that it provides more robust results than the dataset without imputation when MCAR or MAR is supported [61].

Most demographics of the participants were analyzed using frequency and percentage with age using mean and SD. Afterward, changes in BMI, internalized weight stigma, perceived weight stigma, and psychological distress were calculated using the follow-up scores (or values) minus the baseline scores (or values). Therefore, a positive change score indicated greater scores in follow-up, whereas a negative change score indicated smaller scores in follow-up. The change scores were then used to examine the magnitude of their associations via Pearson correlation coefficients.

Hierarchical regressions were then used to examine how the change in BMI scores could be explained by the changes in weight stigma and psychological distress. More specifically, Model 1 in the hierarchical regression involved two demographic variables (i.e., age and gender) as independent variables with the change in BMI as the dependent variable; Model 2 was based on Model 1 to add changes in perceived weight stigma, internalized weight stigma, and psychological distress as independent variables. Finally, a mediation model was conducted using Hayes’ Process Macro (Model 4) with 5000 bootstrapping samples. In the mediation model, change in perceived weight stigma was treated as the independent variable; change in internalized weight stigma as the mediator; age, gender, and change in psychological distress as covariates; change in BMI as a dependent variable. A significant mediated effect of internalized weight stigma would be supported when the 95% confidence interval in the 5000 bootstrapping samples does not cover 0 [62]. Moreover, given that males and females are likely to have different experiences of weight stigma and weight status, both hierarchical regressions and mediation model were conducted separately for males and females as further subgroup analyses.

The hierarchical regression and mediation models were constructed using change scores instead of using baseline and follow-up scores together because of the following reasons. First, using both baseline and follow-up scores to construct the model may cause the issue of collinearity (e.g., baseline perceived weight stigma and follow-up perceived weight stigma are two highly correlated independent variables in the regression model). Second, using change score could minimize the number of independent variables in the model, and a simpler model is better to satisfy the principal of parsimony in statistical analysis. However, change scores are likely to be attributable to the standard error of measurement. Therefore, significant tests were carried out to ensure the changes are true (i.e., baseline and follow-up scores are significantly different) before constructing the models. In this regard, baseline BMI, percentage overweight (i.e., % of participants with BMI > 24 kg/m2), perceived weight stigma, internalized weight stigma, and psychological distress were compared with follow-up BMI, percentage of overweight, perceived weight stigma, internalized weight stigma, and psychological distress using McNamer test (for percentage of overweight) or paired t-tests (for other variables).

Results

Among the 974 participants (396 males [40.7%]; mean [SD] age = 23.70 [4.31] years), nearly three-fourths were undergraduates (n = 686 [70.4%]). In addition, the top three majors of the present sample were management (n = 262 [26.9%]), liberal arts (n = 130 [13.3%]), and engineering (n = 119 [12.2%]). Most of the participants were not current cigarette users (n = 705 [72.4%]) or current alcohol users (n = 662 [68.0%]). Very few participants self-reported that they had a chronic disease (n = 36 [3.7%]) (Table 1).

Little’s MCAR test was significant (χ2 = 103.79; df = 8; p-value < 0.001); therefore, the missing values were not MCAR. The following independent t-tests on the missing values supported that the missing values were MAR (p-values = 0.32 to 0.99). Moreover, the loss to follow-up was 38.6% (376 of 974). Therefore, multiple imputations were used for the following analyses.

Significant differences were found between baseline and follow-up measures in BMI (22.20 vs. 22.54; p-value < 0.001), overweight% (27.9% vs. 30.1%; p-value = 0.003), perceived weight stigma (1.27 vs. 1.71; p-value = 0.002), internalized weight stigma (30.88 vs. 29.46; p-value = 0.001), and psychological distress (13.12 vs. 15.25; p-value = 0.001) (Table 2). The significant differences found between baseline and follow-up scores indicate that the change scores might not be severely influenced by the standard error of measurement.

Table 2 Descriptive statistics of studied variables in baseline and follow-up

Pearson correlations (Table 3) showed that changes in perceived weight stigma (r = 0.12; p-value < 0.001) and internalized weight stigma (r = 0.27; p-value < 0.001) were significantly and positively associated with the change in BMI; changes in perceived weight stigma, internalized weight stigma, and psychological distress were significantly and positively associated with each other (r = 0.26 to 0.36; p-values < 0.001).

Table 3 Correlations between change scores of studied variables (N = 974)

Hierarchical regression models showed that changes in perceived weight stigma (standardized coefficient [β] = 0.09; p-value = 0.01) and internalized weight stigma (β = 0.30; p-value < 0.001) were associated with the change in BMI in a positive direction; change in psychological distress was associated in a negative direction (β = -0.15; p-value < 0.001) (Table 4). In the subgroup analysis of the female subgroup, changes in perceived weight stigma (β = 0.10; p-value = 0.01) and internalized weight stigma (β = 0.32; p-value < 0.001) were associated with the change in BMI in a positive direction; change in psychological distress was associated in a negative direction (β = -0.11; p-value < 0.001). However, in the male subgroup, changes in internalized weight stigma (β = 0.26; p-value < 0.001) and psychological distress (β = -0.20; p-value = 0.001) were associated with change in BMI, while perceived weight stigma was not associated with change in BMI (β = 0.05; p-value < 0.21) (Table 5).

Table 4 Changes in body mass index explained by changes in weight stigma and psychological distress
Table 5 Changes in body mass index explained by changes in weight stigma and psychological distress stratified by gender

The mediation model additionally showed that change in internalized weight stigma was a significant mediator (unstandardized coefficient = 0.04; 95% bootstrapping CI = 0.02, 0.06) in the association between change in perceived weight stigma and change in BMI after controlling age, gender, and change in psychological distress (Fig. 1). Moreover, change in perceived weight stigma had a significantly direct effect on the change in BMI (unstandardized coefficient = 0.07; 95% bootstrapping CI = 0.02, 0.13). Subgroup analysis in the mediation model showed that change in internalized weight stigma remained a significant mediator in both female and male subgroups (Figs. 2 and 3). However, a slight difference was found in the subgroup analysis: change in perceived weight stigma had a significant effect on change in BMI for female participants (unstandardized coefficient = 0.09; 95% bootstrapping CI = 0.01, 0.16) (Fig. 2) but not for male participants (unstandardized coefficient = 0.06; 95% bootstrapping CI = -0.03, 0.75) (Fig. 3).

Fig. 1
figure 1

Mediated effects of internalized weight stigma in the association between perceived weight stigma and body mass index. Dashed line indicates indirect effects; solid lines indicate direct effect. Total effect of change in perceived weight stigma to change in body mass index was 0.11. Coefficients reported using unstandardized coefficients with 95% confidence interval in parentheses. Hayes’ Process Model 4 was used with 5000 bootstrapping samples

Fig. 2
figure 2

Mediated effects of internalized weight stigma in the association between perceived weight stigma and body mass index among female participants. Dashed line indicates indirect effects; solid lines indicate direct effect. Total effect of change in perceived weight stigma to change in body mass index was 0.13. Coefficients reported using unstandardized coefficients with 95% confidence interval in parentheses. Hayes’ Process Model 4 was used with 5000 bootstrapping samples

Fig. 3
figure 3

Mediated effects of internalized weight stigma in the association between perceived weight stigma and body mass index among male participants. Dashed line indicates indirect effects; solid lines indicate direct effect. Total effect of change in perceived weight stigma to change in body mass index was 0.09. Coefficients reported using unstandardized coefficients with 95% confidence interval in parentheses. Hayes’ Process Model 4 was used with 5000 bootstrapping samples

Discussion

College students are a high-risk group for physical inactivity and obesity, especially during the process of experiencing independence, developing behaviors, and forming an independent lifestyle right after leaving the prior structured and disciplined environment of secondary schools [3, 63]. Consequently, weight stigma may emerge to play a crucial role in causing adverse biopsychosocial health outcomes, which might in turn, contribute to the correlates of obesity [10]. However, the mechanisms underlying the impact of weight stigma on changes in BMI have been quite underexplored. More specifically, research is needed on the trajectory of weight stigma, from perceived stigma to internalized stigma, and subsequently to stigma’s impact on BMI change. The present study aimed to expand this weight stigma theoretical model by examining the directional association of weight stigma and changes in BMI within a longitudinal study. This study demonstrated that the associations between perceived weight stigma, internalized weight stigma, and changes in BMI were in line with prior findings. Specifically, the results indicated a positive association and temporal relationship between perceived weight stigma and internalized weight stigma, which were consistent with previous research [8, 32, 35]. A positive gradient was identified between perceived weight stigma and internalized weight stigma [38]. As perceived weight stigma contributes to a higher intensity of internalized weight stigma, this dynamic may, in turn, lead to poorer health behavior and health outcomes [43].

Prior studies have identified that weight-related stigma has been found to have negative consequences for health outcomes [13], including aspects of psychological well-being and physical health [36, 38, 64] and biopsychosocial health [16]. Also, weight stigma may cause detrimental harm to health behaviors and health outcomes that could increase the risk of weight gain [65]. Individuals with high perceived weight stigma were prone to engage in obesity-promoting behaviors [66], such as problematic eating [67], avoidance of dieting [17, 68], and avoidance of physical activity [69]. Internalized weight stigma is also a significant predictor of weight-related health behaviors, including reduced participation in physical activity [70], poorer weight loss maintenance [71], and noncompliance with nutritional recommendations [72]. As a result, internalized weight stigma ultimately predicts poorer weight loss maintenance. However, there is very little evidence of the association between weight stigma and actual changes in BMI. In an effort to fill this research gap, the present study demonstrated that both perceived and internalized weight stigma positively related to the prospective change in BMI. Specifically, through the subgroup analysis, there gender difference was shown on the impact of weight stigma on the changes of BMI. The changes in internalized weight stigma remained a significant mediator in both female and male subgroups; however, change in perceived weight stigma had a significant effect on change in BMI for female participants only. The gender difference may be explained by the Objectification Theory [73, 74]. That is, society teaches self-objectification to women, such that how they are perceived as viewed by society becomes internalized, which makes perceived and internalized weight stigma more closely linked, especially for women during the mate-choice period.

Greater weight stigma corresponded with greater change in BMI. The result is consistent with prior studies indicating that problematic eating [67], avoidance of dieting [17, 68], and avoidance of physical activity [69] could be the consequential behavior of weight stigma that causes a change in BMI. In addition, the present study found that perceived weight stigma and internalized weight stigma were both positively related to changes in psychological distress, which indicated that weight stigma had a crucial impact on the quality of psychological health. Such findings were consistent with prior literature that identified higher perceived weight stigma [75] and internalized weight stigma [17] as both associated with poorer mental health outcomes, with internalized weight stigma having a greater impact on personal psychological health than perceived weight stigma [76]. More specifically, internalized weight stigma was significantly and positively associated with depressive symptoms [31, 77]. Also, perceived weight stigma accounted for 27% of the expected association between greater physiological dysregulation and overweight/obesity [78]. Other studies specified the positive associations between weight stigma and psychological distress, including depression and anxiety [16, 32, 79]; moreover, one study depicted a significant indirect effect of internalized weight stigma in the relationship between perceived weight stigma and depression and anxiety [80], potentially increasing body weight [81].

Psychological distress could provoke weight gain through maladaptive coping strategies, up-regulation of perceived appetite, and emotional eating and binge eating, leading to a vicious circle of further weight gain and continued subsequent distress [82]. The present study supported the mechanism whereby weight stigma may lead to changes in BMI, a possibly obesogenic process, suggesting an ultimately vicious cycle of weight stigma, psychological distress, changes in BMI, and risk of long-term obesity.

Despite several strengths of the current study, including sufficient sample size, a longitudinal approach to trace changes over time, and controlling confounding factors that may affect mediation analysis, there are some limitations in the present study. First, the studied sample was assessed through a web survey, limiting generalizability. Second, responses to the self-report web survey containing questions about weight stigma and BMI could be influenced by social desirability or social approval. Moreover, the follow-up study only lasted for three months; longitudinal effects should be examined over longer time frames in future studies.

In sum, after carefully reviewing the literature for a better understanding of the impact of weight stigma on various health outcomes across physical, psychological, biopsychosocial, and behavioral domains, the present study investigated a theoretical trajectory illustrating how weight stigma is perceived, internalized, and contributes to changes in BMI, the essential cause of obesity, within the context of Asian culture. The findings in this study may help victims of weight stigma better recognize these perceiving-internalizing processes of weight-related perceptions and the consequences on their health. As more data are assembled to replicate these associations, more consistent findings across diverse samples may be of help in devising means of mitigating the harm of weight stigma on health. While Western countries (like the US, Canada, the UK, and ones in Europe) have awarded and called for initiatives to address the issue of weight stigma [38], the same actions should be advocated and recognized in other countries as well. A clearer understanding of weight stigma and its consequences is essential to solving challenges in the field of weight-related health.