Introduction

Post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD) is a mental disorder that can occur in response to a traumatic or life-threatening event [1]. The prevalence rates of PTSD are between 1.9% and 8.8% [2]. PTSD results in significant social and economic burden and puts individuals at increased risk for physical and mental health problems [3]. Symptoms of PTSD include intrusive memories or recollections (e.g., flashbacks or nightmares), persistent avoidance of stimuli reminiscent of the traumatic event, negative alterations in cognition and mood, and hyperarousal or changes in physical and emotional reactions (e.g., restricted range of affect, feelings of detachment and disinterest) [1, 4, 5]. Patients with PTSD often describe difficulties with concentration, attention, and memory [6,7,8]. Many studies have documented poorer performance on tests of attention, working memory (WM), and other cognitive domains including mental manipulation and retroactive interference [6, 8, 9].

Clinical practice guidelines recommend several evidence-based treatments for PTSD, including the use of eye movement desensitization and reprocessing (EMDR) [10]. The efficacy of EMDR is now well established [11,12,13,14]. However, the mechanisms underlying its efficiency remain unclear.

Since its introduction, EMDR has been subject to debate [15]. In particular, the necessity of bilateral stimulation like eye movement (EM) during EMDR is controversial. Recent meta-analysis by [16] suggests that bilateral stimulation can be considered as a distraction inducing an attention-demanding task, which is (at least partially) necessary for an efficient treatment by EMDR. Other type of stimulations apart from EM that have been found to be beneficial include drawing a Figure [17], subtraction arithmetic [18], and playing the computer game Tetris [19].

A central component of EMDR treatment is the dual focus of attention, also called dual taxation [15]. Dual taxation takes place when an individual recalls a distressing memory while also performing a bilateral stimulation or secondary task, such as EM, mental arithmetic, or drawing complex Figure [20]. During the session, the therapist works on memory retrieval while getting the patient to engage in a secondary task [21, 22]. It can be suggested that the secondary task distracts attention away from the retrieval of memory. This reduction in attention would then impact the mechanisms of retrieval, disturbing it and resulting in an incomplete retrieval of memory traces. In the case of PTSD, memory traces of the traumatic event are characterized by the vividness of the emotion. The partial retrieval of these memory traces would blur their content, resulting in the reduction of vividness or the level of emotion linked to the memory traces. This partial retrieval thus leads to the reprocessing of the memory traces. Considering the functioning of the cognitive system, the present work specifically examined WM as one potential mechanism of EMDR action, with the hypothesis that this reprocessing takes place in WM. Other putative mechanisms of action of EMDR, such as mimicking of rapid eye movement (REM) states and the orientating response hypothesis[23] as well as the integration of the traumatic event into semantic networks[24] for instance will not be investigated in this article.

WM is the cognitive structure in charge of the short-term storage of information in view of its processing. Its dual function (storage and processing) has been extensively examined since the earlier study by [23]. Recently, a consensus emerged in the WM literature that this dual function depends on the allocation of attention (see 24 for review). Although the different WM models diverge on how exactly attention is shared between storage and processing, ample evidence was gathered to support the idea that any distraction of attention by a secondary task has a detrimental effect on storage (see 25 for review). More significant to the topic of the present study, the competition in attention allocation created by the dual task impairs memory retrieval; as such, the disturbing images would become less emotional and vivid [15, 17, 26,27,28,29,30]. This could be a potential mechanism of action of EMDR treatment for PTSD, and this hypothesis has been supported by several studies [17, 27, 31, 32]. However, there is still no systematic review examining this hypothesis – the WM hypothesis – as a mechanism supporting EMDR treatment.

Therefore, the purpose of this study is to summarize the state of research about the role of dual tasking during EMDR treatment for PTSD. We propose an interdisciplinary perspective integrating cognitive science and clinical psychology to improve our comprehension of the mechanisms underlying the effects of EMDR with a focus on working memory. Our specific objective is to examine whether the WM hypothesis – that competition for limited WM resources (i.e., attention) induced by the dual task attenuates the vividness and emotionality of the traumatic memory – would provide an explanation for the beneficial effect induced by eye movement or other bilateral stimulation during EMDR treatment for PTSD.

Method

Search strategy

The searching and reporting of results followed the Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses (Prisma) guidelines [33]. A systematic search was conducted in PubMed and Web of Science on the role of WM in EMDR from 1987 (the starting point of Shapiro’s work on EMDR) up to 21 March 2021. The search term was ‘EMDR and working memory’. We chose keywords with large meanings in order to reach the maximum number of results. The screening, with respect to PTSD, was done in a second step during the screening and selection of studies, during which all articles not related to PTSD were excluded. The automatic search was completed with a manual search from the references list of previous systematic reviews, meta-analysis, and the retrieved articles. Our search yielded 165 records: 114 in Web of Science, 51 in PubMed, and 6 from additional relevant records (see Fig. 1).

Fig. 1
figure 1

PRISMA flowchart of the identification and selection of studies

PTSD: post-traumatic stress disorder

Inclusion and exclusion criteria

The inclusion criteria were as follows: (i) studies must report empirical evidence published in peer-reviewed journals; (ii) they must be written in English; (iii) they must focus on the case of PTSD or PTSD-induced conditioning; (iv) they must examine the mechanism of action of EMDR; (v) they must focus on dual taxation of the working memory; and, finally, (vi) they must focus on eye movement and other forms of bilateral stimulation (e.g., tactile, sound).

We excluded: (i) book reviews, presentations, theses, literature reviews, meta-analyses, and magazines; (ii) papers published in a language other than English; (iii) papers that were not on trauma, PTSD, or PTSD-induced conditioning.

Screening and eligibility

After removing the duplicates, two reviewers (D. W. and V. C.) independently screened the studies at two different stages: title/abstract and full text. This was to ensure that no potential articles were left out and that the articles included in the review fulfilled the inclusion criteria. The percentage of consensus between the two reviewers was about 80%. Disagreements were resolved by discussion and consensus based on inclusion and exclusion criteria.

Data processing, extraction, and synthesis

One author (D. W.) extracted the main information from the included studies, and this information was independently reviewed and cross-checked for accuracy and completion by the other author (V. C.). Finally, we also reported the statistical results in terms of test value or effect size, as well as the results of the statistical tests performed.

Results

Descriptive summary

We identified a total of 171 studies, of which 111 remained after filtering out duplicates. Titles and abstracts were then screened; of these, 94 were excluded for the following reasons: was not empirical (e.g., reviews, n = 17), was a thesis (n = 2), was not on post-traumatic stress disorder (n = 45), was not on EMDR (n = 5), did not examine WM (n = 23), was a case study (n = 1), or was written in a language other than English (n = 1). We read the full text of 17 studies that appeared to meet the inclusion criteria to assess them for eligibility. Six papers were removed at this stage due to the following reasons: was not on trauma or PTSD (n = 2), was not on eye movements (n = 2), was a description of the protocol without results (n = 1), and was a meta-analysis (n = 1). A total of 11 original reports published between 2011 and 2021 were included in the systematic review. The full details of these studies are provided in Table 1.

Table 1 Summary of articles included in the systematic review (11 studies)

Characteristics of included studies

All 11 studies used a design comparing different conditions (i.e., with vs. without dual task) and/or groups (i.e., control and experimental group). Among the 11 studies, we identified two different types of designs: studies involving participants with current PTSD symptoms (N = 5: [31, 34,35,36,37] and studies testing healthy participants (N = 6) – that is, without a PTSD diagnosis or current psychiatric disorder – but who were exposed to potential traumatic stimuli by means of trauma films [38] or pictures or images [32, 39], or who were called upon to remember a traumatic event [40,41,42]. The results are presented and organized according these two categories. We provide an addition paragraph on the long-term effect of the dual task intervention.

Studies with participants having current PTSD symptoms

We found a total of five studies with participants having current PTSD symptoms, recruited mostly from specialized mental health care centres [31, 34, 36, 37]. One study recruited university students suffering from intrusive images determined by a therapist to be suitable for EMDR processing [35]. Two studies included participants having symptoms documented in patients with post-traumatic stress disorder, like intrusive images about potential future catastrophes – i.e., flashforwards [35] and auditory hallucinations [37], while three other studies included PTSD patients diagnosed by a trained clinician (e.g., clinical psychologist/psychiatrist) and based on Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM) IV-TR criteria [31, 34, 36].

PTSD symptoms were assessed with instruments like the Yale-Brown Obsessive Compulsive Scale, with items 2 and 3 pertaining to the occurrence of intrusions and the degree of annoyance with them [35]; the Clinician-Administered PTSD Scale interview, based on the DSM-IV [31, 34, 36]; and the Psychotic Symptoms Rating Scale–Auditory Hallucinations questionnaires [37].

After the experiment, most studies indicated a better desensitization of the traumatic memories or images in terms of reduced vividness and emotionality under the dual-task conditions compared to the control condition. For example, 35 showed that dual task (i.e., recall with EM) was associated with reduction in the vividness and emotionality of intrusive images, compared to recall without EM. In the same vein, 31 reported reduced activity and reduced connectivity in emotional processing in brain regions using functional MRI measures. They found that right amygdala and rostral anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) activity was significantly lower after recall with EM than after recall only. Similarly, functional connectivity from amygdala to rostral ACC was decreased after recall with EM compared to recall only. 34 performed a randomized clinical trial and observed better desensitization for the dual-task conditions (i.e., exposure with eyes moving while fixating on therapist’s moving or non-moving hand), with a larger decrease of symptoms and a high remission rate of PTSD diagnosis compared to exposure alone. Finally, 37 showed that making eye movements or counting out loud resulted in a stronger reduction in the emotionality of auditory hallucinations compared to the control condition.

However, the study by36 showed that the emotionality of auditory memories was reduced in the three conditions: making EM (visual taxation), counting down (auditory taxation) or staring at a non-moving dot (control condition), with a stronger reduction under the auditory and visual taxation conditions than under the control condition. Nevertheless, they found no significant difference between the three conditions, indicating no modality-specific effect and no support for the efficacy of dual taxation.

It is important to note that most studies were based on self-reported behavioural tasks as an outcome measure – in particular, the visual analog scale (VAS) score on vividness and emotionality of disturbing memories or images [34,35,36,37]. Such a measure can be prone to bias. Only one study, by 31, used functional MRI measures. Furthermore, the sample size varied considerably, from 8 participants to 139 participants, and most studies did not include a control group.

Nevertheless, the overall consistency in findings is notable, supporting the beneficial effect of dual taxation. The results point to a reduction in the vividness of intrusive images and in their emotionality under dual-task conditions compared to the control condition. In addition, two studies [34, 43] also reported evidence for a larger decrease in symptoms and a high remission rate of PTSD diagnosis under the dual-task conditions.

Studies with healthy participants without PTSD diagnosis but who were exposed to potential traumatic stimuli

Out of the 11 included studies, six studies examined healthy participants without PTSD symptoms but who were exposed to potential traumatic stimuli. Four studies reported a significant difference between the experimental conditions, supporting the hypothesis that there is a reduction in vividness and emotionality under dual-task conditions compared to the control condition. For instance, 32 showed how low- and high-cognitive load tasks reduced responses to threat and enhanced extinction learning compared to the control condition. Moreover, higher cognitive load had a bigger impact on the reduction in the conditioned response compared to the two other conditions (low load and control). Finally, the participants with the lower accuracy rate in the high-load task had the stronger reduction in conditioned response. 39 investigated the effect of induced EM on memory accuracy on a visual discrimination task with two conditions of recall (with vs. without EM) in a sample of 68 undergraduates. Their results showed that false positive rates increased in recall with EM the day after the conditioning phase. 40 examined whether recall with EM would result in larger reductions in memory vividness, negative valence, and distress than recall alone in a sample of 45 participants. The participants recalled and rated negative autobiographical memories in eight successive blocks of four sessions (lasting 24 s each). Memory was deflated after the first block of four sessions in a recall test with EM, but it was inflated under the condition with recall alone. Lastly, 42 investigated whether mental imagery of the 9/11 terrorist attacks following media exposure was dampened by taxing WM in 45 young healthy adults. They compared three conditions of recall – with EM, with a concurrent task (playing Tetris), or with no additional task (recall only). Compared to the recall-only control condition, dual-task conditions reduced ratings of vividness and emotionality.

However, two studies reported that dual-task conditions did not result in greater decreases in hotspot vividness/unpleasantness compared to a control condition. For example, 41 tested a non-clinical sample of 30 undergraduates and examined the effects of EM on positive verbal imagery after an EMDR session using two conditions of recall: with 15–20 s of horizontal EM or with 15 s of eyes stationary control condition. Their results showed no significant differences between the two conditions. 38 investigated 76 healthy students on the effects of dual tasks on intrusive memories after watching a trauma film using three conditions: recall with EM, recall with a concurrent task, or recall without any additional task (control condition). Even after prolonging the duration of the intervention (from 6 × 24s in the first experiment to 16 × 24s in the second experiment) and adding additional measures for intrusion characteristics, they found no effect for the dual task, because recall with EM or with a concurrent task did not lead to greater drops in vividness and unpleasantness compared to the absence of a task in the control condition. As such, both studies showed possible methodological limitations related to the use of subjective measures, as well as to issues related to whether the intervention should be short or long.

Overall, except for two studies that failed to report a significant effect of dual task, the results obtained from healthy participants exposed to potential traumatic stimuli support a reduction in vividness and emotionality during a short period.

Long-term effect of the dual task intervention

Two studies with healthy participants without a PTSD diagnosis but who were exposed to potential traumatic stimuli also included information on follow-up tests. 40 showed that after the 4 × 24s intervention, recall under EM resulted in memory deflation. Meanwhile, recall only (i.e., without EM) caused memory inflation. However, after the full intervention (32 × 24s), both conditions resulted in immediate and 24-hour reductions on all outcome measures. 42 also showed that, compared to the recall-only control condition, dual-task conditions reduced ratings of vividness and emotionality; however, this effect vanished in a follow-up test that took place 24 h later.

In conclusion, there is a reduction in the ratings of vividness and emotionality after a short intervention, but this effect of dual task disappears with time, as evidenced by the follow-up tests.

Discussion

The purpose of this systematic review was to examine whether the WM taxation model provides an explanation for the beneficial effect induced by EM in EMDR treatment. We identified 11 studies that met our inclusion criteria and categorized them into two sets: studies with participants having current PTSD symptoms and studies with healthy participants without a PTSD diagnosis who were exposed to potential traumatic stimuli. The most remarkable finding of this systematic review is that, overall, regardless of the type of studies, the results showed a reduction of vividness and emotionality in the recall of traumatic stimuli under a dual-task condition compared to a control condition such as recall alone (see Fig. 2). Our results showed (a) that a secondary eye movement task during recall weakens emotional memory retrieval, (b) that secondary tasks that do not involve eye movements (e.g., counting, playing Tetris) weaken retrieval to a similar degree, and (c) that competing demands on working memory are the cause of these effects. However, two studies using a follow-up test showed that this effect does not seem to last long, as it disappeared over time.

Fig. 2
figure 2

Graphically summary of the results

Note: Post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD), eye movements (EM), working memory (WM)

In line with our hypothesis, we found a reduction in vividness and emotionality in the recall of traumatic stimuli under dual-task conditions compared to a control condition. Interestingly, this result is fairly similar for participants having current PTSD symptoms and for healthy participants who were experimentally exposed to traumatic stimuli. Notably, the more complex the secondary task during EMDR is, the greater the reduction in vividness and distress associated with negative memories. Most of the studies showed that the variation in the difficulty level of the secondary task plays a role in the efficient desensitizing of the traumatic memory. For example, doing EM, counting, and playing Tetris during the recall of a disturbing memory or image showed stronger effects than the control condition – i.e., without any secondary tasks. Our results provide support for the WM hypothesis as an account for the functioning of EMDR. The recall of memory or stimuli competes with the secondary task (e.g., EM, counting, or playing Tetris) for limited WM capacity. The resource taxation by the secondary task would deflate some aspects of the memory. As a consequence, the memory would be blurred during the dual task, and the disturbing images would become less emotional and vivid [15, 26, 40].

With regard to trauma symptoms, we also found that, in studies with participants having current PTSD symptoms, there was a larger decrease in symptoms under the dual-task conditions compared to the control condition. Although no information was reported on whether the treatment outcome was stable over time, a randomized clinical trial study by 34 showed that remission of PTSD diagnosis after EMDR session and larger treatment effects were observed with dual taxation. This suggests that this diminution of symptoms could be related to the decrease in the vividness and emotionality of the traumatic stimuli.

Our results also suggest that the effects of dual taxation disappear with time, as shown by two studies that provided follow-up tests. However, it is important to take into account that these studies were conducted on healthy participants and not on participants with PTSD symptoms. More studies with follow-up tests are definitely warranted.

Most of the studies used self-reported evaluation scales to rate the vividness and emotional intensity of the disturbing memory, which may be prone to bias [44]. The most common behavioural outcomes measures used was the VAS score on vividness and emotionality, followed by the SUD score. However, only one study provides insight into the neurobiological mechanisms underlying the effects of dual taxation. 31 showed that EM during recall was associated with reduced activity in certain brain regions, such as less activation of the right amygdala and rostral anterior cingulate cortex (ACC), which serve as a central hub for cognitive and emotional networks [45, 46]. This study suggests a neurobiological pathway – i.e., the reduced activity of the amygdala and ACC – which may help to understand how dual taxation during EMDR leads to less vivid and less emotional traumatic stimuli. This would fit with previous studies showing a relationship between EM and attention regulation [47, 48]. Nevertheless, because of the small sample size in the EMDR studies, replication in a larger and independent sample is warranted.

Although these studies provide valuable information on the taxation of WM involved in EMDR, they also have a number of limitations. First, there are very few studies with participants having current PTSD symptoms (n = 5). In addition, measures used to examine PTSD were quite different and included only partially symptoms related to PTSD and / or were not always aimed at diagnosing PTSD. This statistically limits the power of the conclusion. Second, most studies did not control for or determine reaction time, described as an objective index of the extent to which different dual attention tasks tax WM [49]. Knowing the reaction time could help understand the processing of information and appreciate the duration of mental processes of various conditions, as suggested by [50]. The fact that most studies did not control for reaction time does not allow for conclusions about the causal nature of cognitive variables. Third, the use of truly experimental design is limited in studies of PTSD patients. For ethical reasons, it is very difficult to experimentally manipulate how traumatic events are processed. Fourth, in most cases, the assessment of vividness and emotion was conducted using self-reports of behavioural measures, which may be prone to bias and self-representation strategies. Fifth, only two studies provided a follow-up test, thus making it difficult to draw conclusions on the efficiency of dual taxation over time.

Clinical implications

From a clinical perspective, the conclusion of this systematic review may help advance the understanding of mechanisms involved in EMDR. Our results provide evidence for the WM hypothesis that EMDR efficacy is related to the distraction of attention during the retrieval of memory traces in WM. Recalling a traumatic memory while performing a secondary task would shift the individual’s attention away from the retrieval process and result in a reduction in vividness and emotionality, which is also associated with the reduction of symptoms. Thus, any task requiring attentional resources, whether visual or auditory, could allow for the desensitization of a traumatic memory, as well as its reprocessing in memory with a less intense and less vivid emotion. However, one must be very careful about the level of difficulty of the distractive task used. Caution should also be taken regarding the duration of these changes, since two studies showed that these effects disappeared over time.

Conclusion

Although EMDR has been shown to be effective in the treatment of PTSD for years, it remains controversial due to the lack of understanding of its mechanisms of action. We propose an interdisciplinary perspective integrating cognitive science and clinical psychology to improve our comprehension of the mechanisms underlying the effects of EMDR with a focus on working memory. We examined whether the WM hypothesis provides an explanation for the beneficial effect induced by bilateral stimulation during EMDR therapy for PTSD. A notable finding of this review is that most studies indicated a reduction in the evaluation of the vividness and emotionality of the traumatic stimuli after EMDR in the healthy sample as well as in the PTSD sample. Overall, the studies examined provide support for the WM hypothesis. Our research paves the way for future studies to investigate this mechanism in a large sample and in a clinical setting using follow-up tests.