Background

Medication adherence can be defined as the extent to which a patient’s behaviour corresponds with the prescribed medication dosing regime, including time, dosing and interval of medication intake [1, 2]. Non-adherence is a crucial point for the success and safety of many therapies [3,4,5]. Medication non-adherence is a widespread problem that causes high costs worldwide [5,6,7,8,9,10]. Especially in chronic conditions with long-term therapies, adherence is important to achieve target outcomes but is often low [10].

Adherence is a multifactorial phenomenon that can be influenced by various factors. These factors can be divided into five different dimensions: social and economic factors, therapy-related factors, disease-related factors, patient-related factors and health care system-related factors [10, 11]. Some factors can have an influence on intentional non-adherence (conscious decision not to take the medication; e.g., because of high co-payments), while others can have an influence on non-intentional (forgetting) non-adherence (e.g., forgetfulness because of mental comorbidity).

Insights into the factors that might have a negative influence on adherence are important for several reasons. First, this information can support the identification of patients at high risk for non-adherence. Second, it can support the identification of possible adherence barriers that might be eliminated. Third, it can support the development of individually tailored adherence-enhancing interventions.

The objective of this (updated) overview (systematic review [SR] of systematic reviews) was to identify those factors that influence adherence to oral drugs in patients with physical chronic diseases. Given the considerable amount of literature in this field, this updated overview provides a current and compact overall view on this topic.

Methods

There was no published protocol for this overview. Unless otherwise indicated, all described methods were specified before conducting the overview. This overview was not registered.

Information sources

This overview is a focused updated version of an overview published by our research team in 2014 [12]. This overview is reported according to the Preferred Reporting Items for Overviews of systematic reviews (OoSRs), including the harms checklist [13].

We performed a systematic literature search in MEDLINE (via PubMed) and Embase (via Embase). The complete search strategy, including the applied search limits, is provided in Additional file 1. In contrast to our previous search filter, we included unspecific terms for influencing factors (e.g., factors, predictors) as well as specific terms (e.g., gender, age) because we focused only on certain pre-defined influencing factors (for the reasoning, see the “Study Selection” section). We anticipated that these parameters would lead to a higher sensitivity compared with the search for the previous overview version. In addition, the search was performed without limiting the publication date. We performed the search of the electronic databases on June 13, 2018. In addition to the electronic searches, we crosschecked the references of all included SRs.

Study selection

We selected SRs according to the following predefined inclusion criteria:

  1. 1.

    Patients: Adult patients (≥ 16 years) with physical chronic diseases. We considered every physical chronic illness. We excluded SRs that analysed children (if > 20% of the included studies analysed children), and considered only patients with acute conditions or considered only patients with mental illnesses.

  2. 2.

    Medication: Oral drug intake (at least 50% of patient population)

  3. 3.

    Exposure: Pre-specified (see the text below) potential influencing factors for adherence. We defined a factor as any exposure that is not controlled by the study investigator

  4. 4.

    Outcome: Implementation adherence (correct dose, timing and/or frequency of intake) [2]

  5. 5.

    Study type: SRs (definition: systematic literature search in at least one electronic database and assessment and documentation of risk of bias of included studies) of quantitative studies

  6. 6.

    Publication language: English or German

We aimed to summarize the evidence for factors that are widely applicable across different conditions, therapies and regions/settings. Therefore, we limited our overview to unrelated factors of therapy and disease, i.e., we excluded factors that likely strongly vary depending on disease (e.g., symptoms), therapy (e.g., side effects) or health care system (e.g., insurance type). Compared with the previous version, we narrowed the scope by considering only factors for which there were some indices for an influence in the previous broad overview [12]. We chose the following factors: age, gender, ethnic status, education, employment, financial status/income, marital status/not living alone, social support, measure of intake complexity (e.g., number of tablets, number of medications, frequency of intake), duration of therapy, duration of disease, comorbidity, co-payments, medication costs and insurance status (insured/not insured).

In addition to these pre-defined eligibility criteria, a further criterion was defined post hoc during study selection. Both reviewers agreed to exclude those SRs that reported only the number of statistically significant studies (e.g., 10 studies showed a statistically significant effect of gender) without reporting effect sizes and the total number of studies on a certain comparison (e.g., 12 studies analysed gender). The decision to exclude studies that were reported in this way was made because the results could have been highly biased by selective reporting otherwise.

The study selection (title/abstract screening and full-text screening) was performed by two reviewers independently. Any differences between the reviewers were discussed until consensus.

Data collection

All data were extracted using standardized extraction forms piloted beforehand. Data were extracted by one reviewer, and completeness and accuracy were verified by a second reviewer. Any disagreements were discussed until consensus. For each SR, we extracted the following characteristics: condition/medication, eligibility criteria for primary studies (only other than our applied inclusion criteria), search period and any search limits.

The results were extracted according to the type of evidence synthesis. For all meta-analyses, we extracted pooled effect estimates with 95% confidence intervals, tests and measures for statistical heterogeneity, the number of included studies and the number of patients included in the meta-analyses. In the case that the included SR performed only a narrative synthesis, we used modified vote counting to extract the results. This method has been suggested for presenting results of quantitative synthesis and overcoming problems of simple vote counting [14, 15]. We extracted information on the effect direction, total number of included primary studies showing a certain effect direction, statistical significance of primary studies (p < 0.05) showing the effect direction and total number of primary studies that analysed a certain factor.

All data in the tables were harmonized so that the influence on adherence (not non-adherence) refers to an increase in the factor regardless of whether the factor is positive (e.g., socioeconomic status) or negative (e.g., co-payments).

Risk of bias assessment of individual studies and across studies

We used the Risk of Bias in Systematic Reviews (ROBIS) tool to assess the included SRs [16]. The ROBIS tool is based on three phases. Phase 1 aims to assess the relevance of the SR. For this purpose, the relevance of the research question should be assessed. This optional phase was skipped in this overview because the relevance was already completely covered by the eligibility criteria. Phase 2 comprises four different domains (domain 1: study eligibility criteria, domain 2: identification and selection of studies, domain 3: data collection and study appraisal, and domain 4: synthesis and findings) and aims to identify biased areas in the SRs. In the final phase 3, the assessor judges whether the whole SRs is at risk of bias. In addition to the results of phase 2, three additional signalling questions should be considered in phase 3. These three signalling questions refer to the discussion/interpretation of the SRs. We did not extract any data from the discussion/interpretation; therefore, we did not consider these signalling questions in the overall judgement. Thus, the overall judgement of risk of bias is exclusively based on the results of phase 2 [17]. The ROBIS tool was applied by two independent reviewers (TM, AG). Disagreements were resolved by discussion. TM was also an author of two of the included SRs. To ensure an objective assessment, the risk of bias assessment of these SRs was performed by a reviewer other than TM.

Synthesis of results

For all factors, a summary evaluation of the influence on adherence across SRs was made. The evidence for an impact was rated by considering the following criteria that were inspired by the GRADE [18] criteria.

  • Risk of bias of the included SRs and their included primary studies. In primary studies, we considered in particular adjustment for confounding, missing data and adherence measurements

  • Imprecision (statistical certainty, amount of information on a certain factor [number of primary studies and SRs, effect size)])

  • Inconsistency (within and between SRs, e.g., due to different adherence measures)

Based on these criteria, the effects were rated as robust evidence for an impact, some evidence for an impact, probably no impact or uncertain impact. The impact rating was performed by two reviewers.

Overlaps (multiple included primary studies) were assessed by creating a cross table of all included SRs and their primary studies. In addition, the corrected covered area (CCA) was calculated. The CCA is a value that indicates the proportion of overlapping primary studies. It is calculated as follows: \( \mathrm{CCA}=\frac{\left(N-r\right)}{\left(r\times c-r\right)} \); N = number of primary studies (includes multiple counting); r = number of index studies (defined as first-time primary study); and c = number of included systematic reviews. The CCA can assume a value between 0 and 100%. The smaller the value is, the lower the overlap. Conversely, the higher the value is, the greater the overlap [19].

Results

Study selection

The electronic literature research resulted in 4849 hits after removal of duplicates (including hits from the previous search). After title and abstract screening, 4702 articles were excluded, and 147 were judged to be potentially relevant. The full texts of these articles were screened in detail. Fifteen SRs met all eligibility criteria and were included in this overview. Most SRs were excluded because a methodological quality assessment of the included primary studies was not performed or factors other than our pre-specified influencing factors were investigated. In addition to the 15 newly identified relevant SRs, six SR of the previous overview were included. Finally, 21 SRs were included in this overview [20,21,22,23,24,25,26,27,28,29,30,31,32,33,34,35,36,37,38,39,40]. The process of study selection is illustrated in the PRISMA flowchart [41] (Fig. 1).

Fig. 1
figure 1

Flowchart of the study selection

Characteristics of the included systematic reviews

The following conditions and medications were considered: chronic non-malignant pain [35], cardiovascular diseases (e.g., coronary artery disease, hypertension, diabetes mellitus) [21,22,23,24,25,26, 29, 30, 33, 37], Parkinson disease [36], hepatitis C [27], oral anticancer agents [28, 39], inflammatory arthritis [38], HIV/AIDS [31, 32, 34] and chronic diseases [20]. Sinnott et al. did not restrict the condition or medication but included all studies on publicly insured patients who were exposed to co-payments for medications [40]. Of the 21 included SRs, 14 only synthesized the results narratively, and seven performed a meta-analysis. The characteristics of all included SRs are presented in Table 1. A list of excluded studies is available in Additional file 2.

Table 1 Study characteristics

The 21 SRs included 313 primary studies, and data from these studies were used in this evidence synthesis. The number of index publications was 285 (r = 285), which resulted in a primary study overlap estimated by the CCA of approximately 0.5%. The cross table can be found in Additional file 3.

Risk of bias of the included systematic reviews

Risk of bias across the SRs was lowest in domain 3 (data collection and study appraisal). In this domain, six SRs were judged to be at high risk of bias. Compared with domain 3, the other domains, including 1 (eligibility criteria), 2 (identification and selection of studies) and 4 (synthesis), were at higher risk of bias across studies. In all these domains, more than 50% of the SRs were at high risk of bias. In particular, imprecise eligibility criteria, inadequate restrictions in the eligibility criteria, inappropriate search strategies, simple vote-counting and no protocols available were the most common reasons for the high risk of bias in these domains. Figure 2 shows the results of the phase 2 ROBIS rating according to the four different domains.

Fig. 2
figure 2

Risk of bias in the systematic reviews. orange: high (risk of bias), grey: low (risk of bias), blue-grey: unclear (risk of bias)

A comparison of the individual SRs shows that only three SRs were at low risk of bias in all four domains [25, 27, 28]. In contrast, 2/3 of all included SRs were at high risk of bias in two or three domains [20, 21, 23, 24, 26, 30, 33, 35, 37,38,39]. Three SRs were rated to be at high risk of bias in all domains [22, 32, 36]. The results for each included SRs are illustrated in Table 2. We rated the overall risk of bias for eight SRs as low and for 13 SRs as high.

Table 2 Results of the risk of bias assessment

Impact of influencing factors of adherence

The evidence synthesis of the analysed factors (according to the different diseases/therapies) is presented in Table 3. The results of each individual included SR are presented in the Additional file 4.

Table 3 Evidence synthesis

Social and economic factors

The evidence for an impact of education on adherence was uncertain for most diseases/therapies. Some evidence for a positive impact of education on adherence was exclusively noted for cardiovascular conditions [23, 37]. The impact of employment was mostly uncertain. Some evidence for a positive impact was exclusively noted in HIV-infected patients [32, 34]. The other conditions that were investigated for this influencing factor (hepatitis C, inflammatory arthritis and cardiovascular conditions) showed inconsistent results and thus were judged as uncertain evidence [23, 27, 38]. For the analysis of the influence of ethnic status on adherence, we considered different comparisons because the grouping in primary studies differed widely. Some evidence exist for inflammatory arthritis and robust evidence for cardiovascular conditions (in the USA) that white ethnicity is associated with higher adherence [33, 38]. In HIV-infected patients, there was some evidence that white individuals are more adherent than black individuals [32]. The SRs of cardiovascular conditions showed some evidence that large ethnic groups are more adherent than ethnic minorities [37]. Among patients with chronic diseases and patients taking oral anticancer agents, there was some evidence that a better financial status has a positive influence on adherence [20, 39]. The impact of financial status was uncertain in Parkinson disease, hepatitis C and cardiovascular conditions [21, 23, 27, 36, 37]. The influence of the socioeconomic status was uncertain in inflammatory arthritis and patients taking oral anticancer agents [28, 38]. In cardiovascular conditions, some evidence exists that a higher socioeconomic status has a positive impact on adherence [29]. Marital status was investigated in the SRs on Parkinson disease, inflammatory arthritis, chronic diseases, HIV, patients taking oral anticancer agents and cardiovascular conditions. The results were very inconsistent, and consequently, the impact was judged as uncertain overall [20, 23, 32, 36, 38, 39]. In addition, the impact of social support was uncertain in all SRs [23, 28, 30, 37, 38].

Therapy-related factors

We found some evidence for a negative influence of intake of different medications in cardiovascular conditions. The impact of all other therapy related factors (duration of therapy, number of tablets, intake frequency, intake at meals) was uncertain in all conditions [23, 28, 35,36,37,38,39].

Disease-related factors

Duration of disease was the only disease-related factor considered in this overview. Most of the SRs that analysed this factor showed conflicting effect directions, and the evidence for an impact was thus judged as either uncertain or probably no impact overall [23, 27, 28, 35, 38, 39].

Patient-related factors

In six of eight conditions, positive effect directions for higher age were reported. In two conditions (cardiovascular conditions and Parkinson disease), some evidence of an impact was found, and the impact of the other four conditions/medications was uncertain [20, 23, 24, 28, 35,36,37,38,39]. In contrast, negative effect directions of higher age in chronic diseases, cardiovascular conditions and oral anticancer agents were reported [20, 21, 23, 24, 28, 39]. However, the evidence for an impact was uncertain. More distinct (no linear) age groups were compared in the SRs on adherence in inflammatory arthritis, chronic diseases, HIV-infected patients, patients taking oral anticancer agents and cardiovascular conditions [20, 21, 23, 28, 31, 32, 37,38,39]. In two conditions, there was some evidence for an impact. In HIV-infected patients, persons older than 45 years tend to be more adherent than those under 45 years [32]. In patients taking oral anticancer agents, there was some evidence that middle-aged people (approximately 45–60) are more adherent than very old (> 75 years) and younger people (< 45 years) [28]. General comorbidity or physical comorbidity was assessed in inflammatory arthritis [38], patients taking oral anticancer agents, hepatitis C, chronic diseases and cardiovascular conditions [20, 21, 27, 28, 37, 39]. Overall, positive as well as negative effect directions were reported in all included SRs, and the evidence was therefore judged to be uncertain. General mental comorbidity was considered a potential adherence-influencing factor in the conditions Parkinson disease, hepatitis C, chronic diseases and cardiovascular conditions. Negative effect directions were reported for most conditions, while the results were inconsistent in hepatitis C and cardiovascular conditions [20, 21, 27, 30, 36, 37]. The evidence for an impact was mostly judged as uncertain for this factor. Some evidence for a negative impact of mental comorbidity on medication adherence was exclusively noted in hepatitis C and cardiovascular conditions [21, 27, 30, 37]. Depression was analysed in patients taking oral anticancer agents, HIV infection or cardiovascular conditions. In patients taking oral anticancer agents and HIV-infected patients, some evidence was observed, and robust evidence for a negative impact was noted in cardiovascular conditions [28, 30, 32]. Gender was analysed in the SRs on chronic pain, hepatitis C, inflammatory arthritis, chronic diseases, oral anticancer agents and cardiovascular conditions [20, 21, 23, 27, 28, 33, 35, 37,38,39]. The impact was judged as uncertain in all SRs because the effect directions were conflicting (within and between SRs). Some evidence for higher adherence in women was noted exclusively in cardiovascular conditions [21, 23, 33, 37].

Health care system-related factors

For co-payments (any co-payment and higher co-payments), the effect direction was almost always negative. Some evidence for a negative impact of co-payments on adherence in inflammatory arthritis, chronic diseases and cardiovascular conditions exists [20, 22, 23, 25, 26, 38]. The meta-analysis of Sinnott et al. provides robust evidence for a negative impact of co-payments on adherence across different conditions [40]. The evidence for an impact was uncertain in oral-anticancer agents [39]. In cardiovascular conditions, there was some evidence that patients not paying any co-payments are more adherent than those patients paying (any) co-payments [25, 26]. Medication costs were analysed in patients with inflammatory arthritis and patients taking oral anticancer agents. Only negative effect directions were reported, but the evidence for a negative impact on adherence was uncertain in both conditions [38, 39]. It was uncertain whether health insurance status (insured vs. uninsured) influences adherence in patients with chronic or cardiovascular conditions [23, 25].

Discussion

This overview includes 21 SRs on 313 individual primary studies in a broad spectrum of chronic conditions. Compared with the previous version, this focused update increases the certainty of evidence for some factors (e.g., co-payments or ethnic status) and identifies new evidence on other factors (socioeconomic status, depression and insurance status) [12].

We analysed seven potentially socioeconomic adherence-influencing factors. Although mostly positive effect directions were reported, the overall evidence for an impact is uncertain for employment and education. The evidence synthesis indicates that belonging to an ethnic minority seems to be associated with reduced adherence. In contrast, higher financial status and better socioeconomic position seem to have a positive impact on adherence. None of the therapy-related (but not therapy-specific) factors showed evidence for a strong impact on adherence. The same seems to be true for disease duration. Studies focusing on distinct age groups suggest that age does not have a linear association with adherence but that the association is rather a concave shape with an adherence peak in middle to older ages, i.e., adherence is particularly low in very young and very old persons. Studies that analysed age as a continuous linear variable and studies that dichotomized age showed inconsistent results. The explanation for the inconsistent results of the linear analyses might also be attributed to the fact that the association is indeed non-linear. Gender seems to have no consistent impact on adherence. Considering comorbidities, there was only robust evidence that depression impacts adherence negatively. We also found robust evidence that co-payments reduce adherence. Considering this information together with the socioeconomic factors and age suggests that there is a social gradient in adherence behaviour.

Although the majority of literature on adherence-influencing factors is overwhelming, we could only judge the influence for many factors as uncertain. In addition, from the high risk of bias, the main reason for so many uncertain judgements was imprecision. The main cause for downgrading due to imprecision was insufficient reporting, which prevented us from adequately assessing the results. For example, in many cases, we could not even use modified vote counting satisfactorily. Therefore, unclear impact ratings indicate that the evidence is insufficient to allow a conclusion not that there is the tendency that these factors have no impact.

Moreover, the results for many factors were inconsistent. Overviews of SRs are always at high risk for discordant or heterogeneous results across the included SRs [42]. We tried to prevent strong heterogeneity by focusing on factors for which we assumed homogeneity across different conditions and considering only implementation adherence to oral drugs. Nevertheless, the results of our overview were also partly heterogeneous. This is particularly true for the influencing factors education, employment, different medications, duration of disease and gender. One might argue that this suggests that the influence of these factors dependents on condition or setting. However, if inconsistency was observed, this was mostly true within as well as between SRs. Thus, we believe that positive findings might be caused by spurious findings in primary studies (confounding bias, type one error rate, selective reporting). A condition-related explanation for heterogeneity might be that many SRs seem to include symptomatic as well as asymptomatic patients. Research has shown that symptomatic patients are mostly more adherent than asymptomatic patients [43, 44]. This assumption is supported by the fact that especially therapy- and disease-related influencing factors, which are related to the symptomatic patients, were inconsistent. Moreover, none of the included SRs distinguishes intentional (conscious decision not to take medication) and unintentional adherence (forget to take medication); however, it strongly stands to reason that the influencing factors can depend on the underlying reasons for non-adherence [45]. Additional sources of inconsistency that we could not control for were different definitions and measurements of influencing factors (e.g., socioeconomic status) and even more adherence measures (e.g., self-reported vs. electronic monitoring, > 90% of pills taken vs. > 80% vs. mean intake).

We included SRs on any physical chronic diseases and analysed only factors we assumed were independent of disease/therapy. Therefore, on the one hand, we believe that our results are widely applicable for implementation adherence to oral drugs in physical chronic diseases. On the other hand, it should be considered in the interpretation of the findings that the influence of a factor might vary between region/setting. In particular, the influence of different ethnic groups probably depends on the country/region since an ethnic minority in one region could be an ethnic majority in another region However, although ethnic minorities are different ethnic groups in different countries, we believe that all ethnic minorities likely face similar adherence challenges independent of the country they live in.

The identified risk factors of non-adherence can indicate patients who are at increased risk for non-adherence. For clinical practice, this information can help identify and select patients who require support for being adherent. In studies on adherence, the information can help with the identification of relevant participants [46] or the development of adherence risk prediction models [47]. Moreover, the knowledge of influencing factors of adherence can support the development of tailored health technologies to increase adherence by treating the underlying barriers (e.g., depression treatment, reducing co-payments). In this regard, health policy decision makers should consider that there seems to be a social gradient in adherence.

Our overview has some methodological limitations. First, we limited our literature search to English and German languages because there were no other language skills in our research team and no resources for translating articles. Second, we used modified vote counting; however, we are aware that this type of methodology has strong limitations. Nevertheless, we decided to use modified vote counting because we anticipated that this is the only method to harmonize the results from different types of narrative synthesizes. Third, we only analysed therapy-unrelated factors. Consequently, regarding indications where therapy-related factors play an important role (e.g., adverse events in chemotherapy), our evidence is incomplete per se.

Conclusion

There is sufficient evidence that depression and co-payments have a negative impact on adherence. Evidence suggests that general mental comorbidity and belonging to an ethnic minority might have a negative impact on adherence and that a higher socioeconomic status might have a positive impact on adherence. In addition, the evidence suggests that the influence of age on medication adherence has a concave pattern, i.e., lower adherence in young age groups, increasing adherence with a peak in middle to older age groups and lower adherence in very old age groups. The moderate to high risk of bias in the included SRs and the exclusion of 78 reviews due to missing quality assessment of included primary studies indicate that there is a need for more methodically sound research to provide stronger conclusions. Future primary studies and SRs should use validated adherence measures, adjust the analysis for relevant confounding factors, avoid using arbitrary cut-offs for influencing factors (e.g., age) and report the effect measures with 95% confidence intervals. Furthermore, the studies should analyse intentional and non-intentional adherence distinctly.