Background

Overtesting has become a growing concern in contemporary healthcare, [1,2,3] with worldwide movements such as the Choosing Wisely, Less Is More, and Too Much Medicine campaigns bringing the issue to the fore [1, 4,5,6,7]. Despite this, awareness and understanding of the causes and consequences of overtesting amongst the public, patients and clinicians remains limited [3, 8, 9].

Medical tests are an important first step in the clinical management pathway, whether they are routine blood pressure measurements, blood tests, imaging studies or more specialised investigations. With the overall aim of healthcare being to prevent premature morbidity and mortality [10], it follows then that the value and rationale for performing tests lies in their ability to improve patient health outcomes [11] through detection and/or monitoring of disease and subsequent treatment. These health outcomes may be clinical, emotional, social, cognitive or behavioural [10].

Tests that are done without improving health outcomes (as defined above), may be examples of overtesting. Overtesting includes unnecessary medical tests in both asymptomatic and symptomatic people, [2] where testing does not improve clinical decision making (clinical utility), or health outcomes (clinical effectiveness) [12]. Overtesting of screening and diagnostic tests may occur where there is an unfavourable balance of benefits to harms [13] and where there would be little or there would be no consequences to the patient from not performing the test [12]. Overtesting of monitoring tests may occur where the tests have poor measurement properties, and/or are done overly frequently (low signal: noise ratio) [14].

Potential harms from overtesting arise through misdiagnosis, false positive results, false negative results and overdiagnosis, where people are labelled as having a “disease” for a condition that would not have caused them harm if it were left undetected and untreated [1, 5, 6, 8, 9, 15,16,17,18]. More often than not, this initiates a further cascade of unnecessary investigations and treatment [1, 15, 16, 19,20,21,22]. Individuals may be affected physically, psychologically and financially [8, 9, 15, 16, 23] and the use of finite healthcare resources prevents their redeployment to others who would have benefited from tests, treatment and other interventions [8, 15, 16, 23]. These unintended consequences increase proportionately with the degree of overtesting [12].

Clinicians are the “gatekeepers” [24] to accessing further healthcare, and are ultimately responsible for requesting medical tests, making diagnoses and offering treatment and/or further tests. Their stewardship of medical testing is key to limiting overtesting and preventing the cascade of harms that may result.

To date, there has been no systematic synthesis of published evidence exploring factors that influence clinicians to overtest. In this narrative literature review and workshop discussion with experts at the Preventing Overdiagnosis Conference 2019, we aimed to identify and create a thematic framework of important factors that may influence clinicians to overtest. The results of this study will inform further planned research to develop a scale assessing clinician knowledge, attitudes and practices around overtesting. The new scale will be used to guide the development of clinician-targeted interventions to prevent overtesting [25], and to measure their impact on testing behaviour.

Methods

We conducted a narrative review of peer-reviewed literature using Boell’s [26] hermeneutic approach characterised by a systematic but flexible and iterative search strategy. A narrative review aims to summarise, interpret and critique available literature based on the authors’ own understanding, or pre-existing theories and models available [27,28,29]. Through an initial non-systematic search of the literature, we identified recurrent themes and developed inclusion and exclusion criteria and the search strategy.

We searched MedLine from inception to April 2019 for published articles using MeSH terms and keywords related to the concepts of “overtesting”, “physician” and “attitude to health” [Supplementary File 1]. We included articles that focussed on overtesting or overscreening and explored factors influencing clinicians’ test requesting behaviour. We did not limit our search to specific conditions, tests or specialties. To supplement the MedLine search, we identified further articles through forward and backward citation and consultation with content experts in overdiagnosis. We excluded articles that focussed on overtreatment, unnecessary treatment or factors influencing clinicians’ treatment decisions.

Two authors (JL and KB) independently screened titles and abstracts. Where either author considered the paper potentially relevant, the full text was retrieved. Two authors screened the full text for inclusion in the review (JL screened all full text, and KB, FS and KP each screened one third). One author (JL) undertook inductive thematic analysis [30] of the included studies to identify distinct factors influencing clinicians’ decisions to order tests. These were initially categorised in a thematic framework of “direct/intrinsic” factors and “indirect/extrinsic” factors. A second author reviewed included studies and added additional factors to the framework (KB, FS and KP each reviewed one third of included studies) [Supplementary File 2].

The resulting framework of “direct/intrinsic” and “indirect/extrinsic” factors was presented at a workshop at the Preventing Overdiagnosis Conference in Sydney, December 2019. The workshop used small group discussion to receive feedback on the initial framework, and to elicit further factors that may influence clinician test ordering behaviour not identified through the narrative review. Basic demographic information was collected from workshop participants with their consent. There were five-six participants in each small group, with discussion facilitated by one of the authors. Discussions were audio-recorded and transcribed for further analysis by the authors to refine the thematic framework developed from the narrative review.

Results

Results of narrative review

A total of 542 articles were retrieved following the search of MedLine in April 2019. 55 articles were included after screening by title and abstract, and application of inclusion/exclusion criteria to full text articles. A further 10 articles identified through forward and backward citation and by content experts were included, resulting in a total of 65 articles in the review. [Fig. 1].

Fig. 1
figure 1

Study flow diagram

Direct/intrinsic factors

Fear of malpractice and litigation

Thirty-four articles cited fear of malpractice and litigation as a cause for unnecessary testing and diagnosis. A number of studies demonstrated a positive correlation between likelihood of ordering imaging tests and level of litigation concern [31,32,33,34]. Hoffman and Kanzaria [35] referred to a survey of United States (US) emergency physicians where 97% of respondents ordered advanced imaging tests due to fear of litigation and missing a low probability diagnosis despite feeling the tests were unnecessary. Sanabria et al. [36] showed that pathologists concerned about litigation tended to lower implicit disease thresholds for indeterminate and malignant tumour diagnoses. Conversely, a number of studies deemed malpractice not to be a major driving factor for ordering tests [37,38,39].

Clinician knowledge and understanding

Twenty-five articles highlighted unnecessary testing as a compensatory measure for lack of knowledge and understanding. For most studies, this related to lack of knowledge and understanding of the drivers of overuse, the natural history of disease, and appropriate management pathways. Cardiologists and non-cardiologists [37] reported that greater training and experience helped them understand when to perform an echocardiogram and in which patients as they “understand the natural course of disease” [37]. Through their novel reflective writing program, Caverly et al. [40] demonstrated that greater cognisance of the drivers of overuse can positively impact clinical decision making and test ordering behaviour through empowerment to identify, discuss and avoid overuse.

Wegwarth et al’s [41] randomised trial assessing 412 primary care physicians’ understanding of common screening statistics found that lack of knowledge and understanding of significance of test properties and results also encourages overtesting. Physicians were more likely to recommend a test when presented with evidence based on increased 5 yearly survival rates (which tend to overestimate the benefit of early detection and treatment because of lead time bias and overdiagnosis) as compared with reduced mortality rates (which give an unbiased estimate of benefits) [41].

Intolerance of uncertainty and risk aversion

This factor was cited in 24 articles. In the 2017 American Board of Internal Medicine (ABIM) Foundation survey, [42] among the commonest reasons for ordering low value tests were desire to reduce uncertainty (84%) and “just to be safe” (78%) [39]. Egerton-Warburton et al. [43] found that over half of 1029 emergency physicians in the study reduced their implicit “test threshold” well below the explicit threshold set using empirical data on test accuracy and risk of harm from the disease, due to their discomfort with diagnostic uncertainty [43]. Coon et al. [23] also highlighted the “shotgun approach” [23] of ordering a broad range of tests and hoping for a positive result somewhere, which is often used in situations of diagnostic uncertainty. A US survey [44] of primary care providers’ and gynaecologists’ breast cancer screening practices showed that those with greater levels of anticipated regret in missing serious disease were more likely to recommend mammography [44]. However, in assessing factors influencing tendency to order imaging using hypothetical scenarios, Kini et al’s [33] survey of cardiologists and general practitioners did not find a significant association between risk aversion and tendency to order cardiac stress tests and echocardiography (likely due to their small sample size) [33].

Cognitive biases and previous experiences

Unnecessary testing often occurs as a result of cognitive biases; two significant cognitive biases were found in 12 articles. The first was “availability bias” [4, 45] which occurs when the likelihood of future events is estimated based on ease of recall of similar events. Closely associated is the impact of previous experience of clinical events. Clinicians with recent negative experiences or “recent medical blunders” [46] were more likely to adopt an aggressive approach to diagnostic testing [10, 38, 45, 46]. Gyftopoulos et al. [47] and Sanabria et al. [36] also suggested that positive experiences from test ordering can increase the likelihood of ordering further tests in the future.

The second cognitive error is “representative bias” [10, 15, 48] or “base rate neglect” [10, 15, 43] which occurs when failing to take into account pre-test probability in estimating post-test probability in the setting of a positive test result [15, 21, 43]. By overinterpreting positive results, clinicians are more likely to order further tests. In Austin’s [48] survey, it was found that 10% of physicians had incorrectly deemed positive predictive value to be the same between screening (where pre-test probability is low) and diagnostic (where pre-test probability is high) tests.

Sense of medical obligation

This factor was cited in 6 articles. Testing based on a sense of medical obligation was driven by the clinician’s need to show that at least something was being done for the patient [46, 49]. Simmonds et al. showed that when faced with the decision as to whether to disclose a diagnosis of clinically inconsequential chronic kidney disease, some general practitioners felt morally bound to respect patient autonomy and disclose results so as to allow the patients to make their own health decisions [50].

Indirect/extrinsic factors

Pressure from patients and doctor-patient relationship

This was reported in 29 articles. Van der Weijden et al. [46] noted that not only were anxious patients more likely to request tests, but general practitioners conscious of long term relationships with patients were more likely to meet these requests. In the 2014 and 2017 American Board of Internal Medicine survey, [42] a large proportion of participants cited desire to keep patients happy, patient’s insistence, and the idea that patients should make the final decision, as reasons for ordering tests. Gogineni et al. [51] and Griffith et al. [52] showed that clinicians were more likely to acquiesce to patient demands for tests the clinician judged as unnecessary if patients threatened to see another clinician. Conversely, He [38] and Siedlikowski et al. [53] found that the better the relationship, the less likely patients were to demand unnecessary tests, and the less likely doctors would be to order unnecessary tests.

Guidelines, protocols and policies

Interpretations and attitudes toward guidelines, protocols and policies were found to significantly influence test ordering behaviour in 21 articles. Akerman et al. [54] found that that there was a drop in prostate screening rates from 91.7 to 80.4% when new recommendations from the Canadian and United States Preventive Services Task Force were released following evidence of little net benefit from screening. A number of studies showed that non-existent or discordant guidelines can result in overtesting [45, 53, 55,56,57]. However, some studies demonstrated that the use of protocols resulted in inappropriate use of tests and overtesting [36, 37, 58]. Alber et al. [49] and Bishop et al. [59] found that doctors based their test ordering decisions on individual patient cases, using guidelines “as a guide rather than strict rules” [59]. This resulted in variation in test ordering behaviour, with some more likely to test while others less likely [49, 59].

Financial incentives and ownership of tests

This was reported in 21 articles. While financial incentives refer to the remuneration received from directly ordering a test, ownership of tests results in a personal, vested interest from more tests being performed, as well as remuneration on a larger scale. Moynihan and Doust [1] and Sanabria et al. [36] refer to the concept of “physician induced demand” [21] whereby physicians can order return visits and perform diagnostic tests when indications are vague or controversial. Pickles et al. [60] showed that Australian general practitioners (fee-for-service health system) were more likely to order prostate specific antigen tests than their United Kingdom counterparts (no fee-for-service health system). Fonesca et al. [37] suggested that clinicians working in private healthcare settings were more likely to be driven by economic incentives in regards to test ordering. Physicians who own imaging equipment engage in more testing with similar clinical outcomes, [15] implying a financial conflict of interest as a driver of overtesting. Conversely, general practitioners in Simmonds et al’s [50] study provide a case study of how clinicians may resist financial incentives to test when they are motivated and committed to preventing overtesting. GPs in this study resisted financial incentives to keep a register and monitor patients with stage 3 chronic kidney disease, as this conflicted with their beliefs around the meaning of mild reduction in kidney function and were concerned that “they’re creating an illness that doesn’t exist [50].

Pressure from colleagues

Pressure from other clinicians was as a driver of unnecessary testing in 13 articles. Fonesca et al’s [37] cardiologists admitted to ordering echocardiograms when colleagues deemed it necessary despite themselves thinking otherwise. D’Souza et al. [61] also showed that junior doctor test ordering was significantly influenced by their peers, colleagues and supervisors. Siedokowski et al. [53] found that as many as 89.6% of physicians would order a screening test they would not have otherwise ordered if specialists had recommended the test.

Time constraints

Time constraints for clinical assessment was another factor behind excessive testing in 13 articles. A number of studies recognised the time pressures in the work environment which limits time with patients and encourages physicians to provide a test just to expedite the clinical encounter [34, 46, 59, 62, 63]. Ellen and Horowitz’s [64] survey of Israeli nurses showed that more than half felt that giving physicians more time to discuss alternative tests would reduce overuse. On the other hand, Murphy et al. [65] highlighted that some general practitioners felt doing an annual general check-up on otherwise healthy patients represented a waste of their limited time that was better spent attending to sick patients.

Availability and ease of access to tests

This factor was reported in 10 articles. Tests were more likely to be ordered when logistically easier, for example, during day shifts as opposed to evening or night shift, [64] when in closer proximity, [21] when there was little resistance in test ordering [47] or when able to order from desktop devices [46]. Fonesca et al. [37] also noted that waiting times and patient physical mobility affected likelihood of echocardiogram test ordering. Having the available technology and equipment was also a key predictor of testing ordering, such was the case in tertiary hospitals [38].

Pre-emptive testing to facilitate subsequent care

Reported in 10 articles, uncertainty about what future tests would be required, a desire to avoid delays in a patient’s care pathway and testing to establish a “baseline” were recognised as causes for potentially unnecessary testing. Amongst Irish interns, Flynn et al. [55] noted that almost half requested tests they felt their consultant would want, to ensure that cases were never cancelled due to a lack of data. Sears et al. [34] found that 76% of physicians felt they couldn’t refer a patient to a specialist without having magnetic resonance imaging done first [34]. Similarly, Alber et al. [49] highlighted that some general practitioners considered “medical overuse in inpatient care as a welcome diagnostic work-up and baseline for the subsequent outpatient care” [49] with Munce et al. [66] noting that some family physicians ordered bone mineral density tests in asymptomatic women at menopause to obtain a “baseline” [66].

Contemporary medical practice and new technology

Four articles highlighted that in contemporary medical practice, emphasis and even reliance on technical tests with a relatively lower priority given to history taking and physical examination, contributes to overtesting [7, 36, 37, 49]. Lysdahl and Hoffman [67] also showed that improved radiological technology was a major cause of overall increased investigation volume that, in turn, predisposed to unnecessary investigation.

Results of expert group discussion

There were 15 participants at the Preventing Overdiagnosis conference workshop. All but one participant were either academics/researchers or medical doctors and most had been working for at least five years. [Supplementary File 3].

Participants generally agreed with the importance of factors identified through the narrative review. A number of additional suggestions were made and subsequently integrated into the existing framework. Physical vulnerabilities (such as being fatigued or hungry) and language barriers were included in “time constraints” as participants felt that such motivational factors for test ordering were based on the desire to reduce patient-contact time. The notion of wanting to fit into the existing medical culture was included in “pressure from colleagues”.

Participants believed that the initial framework of “direct/intrinsic” and “indirect/extrinsic” factors was too dichotomous and artificial, and did not adequately illustrate the overlap and interaction between all the factors. Participants considered that most of what was conceptualised as “indirect/extrinsic” is actually moderated by the “direct/intrinsic” values and perceptions of the individual clinician, and that neither of these properties are fixed but change over time. Participants suggested modifying the proposed framework to better highlight the complex relationship between factors influencing clinician test ordering behaviour to aid in the development of the scale measuring clinician knowledge, attitudes and practices around overtesting.

Based on this feedback, we revised the initial framework to incorporate the factors in a new framework of: “intrapersonal”, “interpersonal” and “environment/context” factors. We believe that these categories provide a more useful framework for informing construction of a scale to measure clinician knowledge, attitudes and practices on overtesting. The total number of articles reporting on each factor and quotes (from participants involved in qualitative interview studies) reflecting clinician attitudes and reactions are provided in (Tables 1, 2 and 3). Detailed analysis of workshop data is available via [Supplementary File 4].

Table 1 “Intrapersonal factors” affecting clinician test ordering behaviour, with number of articles and quotes from articles
Table 2 “Interpersonal factors” affecting clinician test ordering behaviour, with number of articles and quotes from articles
Table 3 “Environment/context factors” affecting clinician test ordering behaviour, with number of articles and quotes from articles

Discussion

This narrative literature review, and subsequent small group discussion with experts in preventing overtesting, highlights the myriad of factors that influence a clinician’s decision to order medical tests, often unnecessarily or against evidence-based recommendations. We present a framework for understanding factors that influence clinicians to overtest. These were grouped within a final framework of “intrapersonal”, “interpersonal” and “environment/context” factors. The most commonly cited factors were “fear of malpractice and litigation”, “pressure from patients and doctor-patient relationship”, “clinician knowledge and understanding”, “guidelines, protocols and policies” and “intolerance of uncertainty and risk aversion”.

We are aware of no other systematic evidence synthesis exploring in detail the factors that influence clinician decisions to order unnecessary tests. Pathirana’s [7] analysis article explored the drivers of overdiagnosis which are closely linked to that of overtesting and mapped them to potential solutions but did not focus on the drivers from a clinician perspective. Siedlikowski et al. [53] and Sharma et al. [57] identified a number of factors influencing clinicians’ recommendations for mammography screening, but not test requesting behaviour more broadly.

One intended outcome of this thematic framework is that it will help clinicians become more aware of their test requesting behaviour, and that this self-reflection may be a critical first step to the behavioural changes needed to prevent overtesting. The grouping of “intrapersonal”, “interpersonal” and “environment/context” clearly reflect the broad range of pressures faced by clinicians on a daily basis. For example, with societies becoming increasingly punitive [68] for errors of omission rather than commission, [23] clinicians are understandably motivated to order tests as a strategy for lowering legal risk and avoiding the financial and emotional consequences of litigation [35]. Moreover, pressures and demands from patients is a growing issue in modern healthcare [51] partly driven by information publicly available on the Internet [37, 52] and social media [21, 60]. Such information may be low quality, unreliable or incomplete, [52] and patients may have unrealistic or ill-informed expectations [88]. The culture of “shame and blame” [35] and medical education that instils fear of uncertainty [24] further encourages overtesting. Through greater awareness of their test requesting behaviour, clinicians may be more likely to only request tests when these may helpfully inform their clinical decision making, and ultimately improve health outcomes [12].

This framework may also provide a basis for developing interventions to prevent overtesting by targeting a specific group of factors and preferably all groups in the framework rather than individual factors alone. Too narrow a focus, for example, increasing clinician knowledge without taking into account the impact of financial incentives, may cause an intervention to be less effective.

The results of this study and framework will also be used to inform the development of a scale to assess clinician knowledge, attitudes and practices around overtesting and to measure the impact of clinician-targeted interventions to prevent overtesting and its unintended consequences.

Strengths of our study are the systematic approach to retrieving the literature, use of at least two authors at each step of the review process and rigour in thematic analysis of the data with multiple iterations through discussion with all authors. We were able to engage with content experts at the Preventing Overdiagnosis Conference workshop, and revise the initial proposed framework. Our final framework is not only more useful for measuring clinician-targeted strategies and interventions, but also explores the complex interplay of factors that occurs in real clinical practice. A limitation of our study was that we searched only one database which may have limited the number of articles included. However, given the richness of data and complexity of the thematic framework, it seems unlikely that we missed important factors that influence test ordering behaviour. This is also supported by the fact that the small group discussion did not generate any new, distinct factor groups.

Conclusions

Although medical tests are integral to clinical management, clinicians have the ability and responsibility to limit overtesting and to prevent harm to patients and the healthcare system. This thematic framework describes factors that influence decisions to request tests as a first step in developing clinician-targeted interventions to reduce overtesting.