Background

Research into the neurobiology of autism spectrum disorders (ASDs) is marked by limitations in the ability to identify reliable, biologically based markers that can confirm diagnosis. However, nearly all perfusion imaging studies in individuals with ASD report decreased cerebral blood flow (CBF) to the temporal cortices [1,2,3,4,5,6,7,8], but see [9] for counterevidence. These results are surprisingly consistent, considering many of these studies differed greatly on key variables: phenotypic classification of ASDs, sample characteristics, scanning task, and CBF scanning parameters. Results to date suggest that hypoperfusion observed in the temporal lobes of individuals with ASD may be a viable marker that spans age and intellectual level in the identification of ASD. However, no study has tested the reliability of hypoperfusion in the temporal lobes of individuals with ASD over time, and the relationship of hypoperfusion to the behavioral phenotype of ASD remains underspecified.

CBF changes are thought to reflect regional changes in neural activity in a region over time [10, 11]. Furthermore, CBF provides absolute quantification of regional brain function. It may therefore provide a more reliable marker of trait-like effects than measures obtained through condition contrasts, such as blood-oxygen-level-dependent scanning that is typically used in task-based functional magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) [12,13,14,15,16,17,18].

Arterial spin labeled (ASL) perfusion MRI utilizes magnetic labeling of endogenous blood water as a tracer for CBF quantification [19]. This non-invasive approach for CBF mapping eliminates the risk of using radioligand tracers with pediatric populations—as have been used in nearly all CBF studies with ASD samples to date. ASL can readily be combined with other MRI modalities. ASL is also particularly sensitive in younger populations, where the increased water and blood flow rates in the brains of children allow for SNR improvements of over 70% relative to adults [20,21,22]. As a result, ASL has seen increased use in the pediatric prevention, identification, and treatment of neurodevelopmental disorders such as periventricular leukomalacia, ischemic stroke, and sickle-cell-related cerebral ischemia [22]. Despite these strengths, ASL has rarely been used to examine CBF differences in ASD.

In functional imaging applications, ASL can be used to detect changes in CBF occurring over several minutes or longer [23, 24]. It can also be used to measure task-specific changes in CBF (analogous to traditional fMRI). For example, perfusion fMRI based on ASL was used to demonstrate activity in the temporal lobe during passive viewing of a video [25], and to accentuate differences in mild cognitive impairment, another disorder frequently associated with temporal lobe function [26]. An ability to measure brain function during a sustained task condition such as passive viewing of a video facilitates implementation in both a clinical setting, and with a broader range of cognitive functioning for individuals with ASD. Furthermore, watching a movie in a relaxed state may also be as close to the context of “natural vision” as can be achieved during an MRI scan. Studies incorporating movies, comics, stories, and other contextually centered stimuli are known to elicit greater temporal pole activation with functional magnetic resonance imaging [27], and resting state fMRI has revealed that the temporal pole has strong functional connectivity with brain areas highly relevant to the processing of social semantic detail and the elicitation of visceral emotional responses [28]. Impaired function of these networks would have implications for social functioning and for disorders like ASD that are characterized by atypical social functioning.

Here, we utilized ASL MRI to compare temporal lobe function during passive video viewing in a cohort of children with ASD to typically developing control (TDC) participants. Although it is likely that ASD is associated with differences in function that are distributed across the brain, we focused the present paper primarily on the temporal lobes, as difference in visual association areas are among the most widely replicated in ASD [29,30,31,32,33]. We hypothesized that regional CBF in the temporal lobe would be reduced in children with ASD as compared to controls, reflecting reduced activity in visual association areas (fusiform gyrus, posterior superior temporal sulcus, temporal pole) and language areas (Wernicke’s area) that have been linked to social cognition and language impairments in ASD [29, 34, 35]. This hypothesis is based on the vast majority of prior perfusion studies highlighting hypoperfusion in the temporal lobe [1,2,3,4,5,6,7,8], meta-analyses of task-based functional MRI using blood-oxygenation-level-dependent response implicating the fusiform gyrus in social cognition [30, 31], and a recent large-scale international study highlighting atypical structural development of the temporal lobes [36]. We also sought to assess the reproducibility of ASL over the time interval of a typical clinical trial of an investigational drug, i.e., about 2 months, so that ASL might prove to be a reliable biomarker that could be easy to implement as an outcome measure (i.e., resting/no onerous task demands) for future treatment trials. Moreover, we sought to better understand blood perfusion in key brain regions by comparing changes in ASD versus TDC measured by ASL during passive video viewing. Furthermore, given the important role of the fusiform gyrus in processing of faces [29, 37], we explored whether perfusion in this area of the temporal lobe was sensitive to individual differences in social function and processing of faces. Alongside our a priori temporal lobe hypotheses, we conducted whole-brain tests for significant areas of hypoperfusion, and tests of global differences in CBF.

Methods

Participants

Seventy-six children (43 ASDs; 33 TDCs) between the ages of 12 and 17 were invited to participate in this study. To be included in the ASD group, children needed a community diagnosis of autism, Asperger’s syndrome, or pervasive developmental disorder; this study conducted data collection prior to the release of DSM-5 [38] so that is why DSM-IV-TR [39] criteria were used. This diagnosis was confirmed by expert, research reliable clinicians who used the revised algorithm for the Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule [40] and the Autism Diagnostic Interview-Revised [41] to inform their diagnostic decision with a DSM-IV-TR checklist. Children were not invited to participate or excluded if they failed to meet criteria for an ASD diagnosis based on expert opinion, had active severe psychiatric symptoms that limited participation (e.g., psychosis, severe depression, mania), parents reported a known genetic disorder (e.g., fragile X), or severe premature birth (< 32 weeks). Nine eligible children withdrew from the study prior to the imaging phase, six were excluded from the current analyses due to poor data quality, and one was removed due to a scanner error. Because ASL data were successfully collected on only two female ASD participants, the analyzed sample was restricted to males, for a final sample of 58 participants (33 ASD, 25 TDC). TDCs were screened and excluded if parents reported any known genetic, language, learning, neurological, or psychiatric disorder, premature birth, any first- or second-degree relative with ASD, or receiving any psychoactive medication. TDCs were also excluded if they presented with elevated symptoms on the parent-report Child and Adolescent Symptom Inventory-Revised [42].

General study procedures

All participants completed three data collection sessions. The first was a behavioral evaluation to confirm children met inclusion and exclusion criteria, to assess cognitive ability, and to characterize participants along broad spectrum of ASD behavior. Measures including the Differential Ability Scales, second edition (DAS-II; [43]), Social Responsiveness Scale-second edition (SRS; [44]) the Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales-II (VABS-II; [45]), and the Benton Facial Recognition Task [46] (see Table 1 for group characteristics). All participants then completed two scan sessions approximately 8 weeks apart to assess scan-rescan reliability (mean difference in days ASD = 8.87 ± 0.73, TDC = 8.97 ± 0.65).

Table 1 Participant characteristics

MRI scanning

A Siemens Verio 3 T scanner with a 32-channel head coil was used for scanning. High-resolution structural MRI data (MPRAGE sequence, .9 × .8 × .8 mm, TR/TE = 2000/3.3 ms) were collected for each participant in order to identify brain regions of interest (ROIs) and register data into standard space. Regional CBF (mL/100 g/min) was measured using pseudocontinuous ASL with 2D gradient-echo echo-planar imaging [47]. The labeling and control RF duration was 1.5 s with post-labeling delay of 1.2 s. Multi-slice perfusion maps with 40 label/control pairs were acquired with TR/TE = 4000/17 ms, flip angle = 900, bandwidth = 3005 Hz/pixel, slice thickness = 5 mm, matrix size = 64 × 64, FOV = 220 × 220 mm, and number of slices = 20.

ASL task procedures

Prior to MRI, participants were desensitized and familiarized with all procedures, and trained in a mock scanner to stay still within 2 mm. All children passively viewed a Discovery Channel video (“Planet Earth: Pole-to-Pole”) without sound on a projection screen during mock scanning, as well as during ASL acquisition at both data acquisition time points. Thus, by time point 2, participants had seen the film three times. The video was initiated at the exact same time point for every subject across all exposures. It intermixed displays of nature scenes (mountains, tundra, clouds, waterfalls, satellite views of earth) 33% of the time, animals in nature scenes (flock of birds, penguins, polar bears) for 60% of the time, and 3 different segments of simple text phrases (“Planet Earth,” “From Pole to Pole,” and “Narrator David Attenborough”) for 7% of the time. During the video, participants were instructed to quietly attend throughout with their eyes open, while a research assistant monitored this at their side in the MRI room. ASL scanning lasted approximately 6 min.

Image analysis

Raw ASL images were motion-corrected in FMRIB Software Library (FSL; [48]) using a 6-parameter rigid body spatial transformation and co-registered to the MPRAGE images of the same session. ASL averaged difference images were converted to mL/100 g/min using a single-compartment model in the perfusion data processing toolbox, ASLtbx [49, 50]. Structural images were segmented into probabilistic gray matter (GM), white matter (WM), and cerebrospinal fluid (CSF) maps using FMRIB’s Automated Segmentation Tool (FAST), and probabilistic averages of GM and WM designations were converted to binary masks (thresholded at 0.5). These binary masks were combined to create a global mask. The global mask was used as an ROI to extract CBF for whole-brain area from the individual CBF map. ASL data were smoothed at 8 mm full width at half maximum. A binary global mask was then created by adding together GM and WM masks. ASL relative CBF (rCBF) maps were generated by normalizing CBF to global CBF for each subject. In higher-level analysis, affine (12 DOF) transformations to a 2 mm MNI152 template were performed using FMRIB’s Linear Image Registration Tool [51].

Statistical analysis plan

To examine group differences in ASL, higher-level group contrasts across the two time points were analyzed in the context of a two-way ANOVA examining the effects of time point (repeated measure) and group. FSL’s Threshold Free Cluster Enhancement (TFCE) was performed [48, 52, 53] for within-method differences and when examining convergence between them. TFCE uses a permuted null-distribution of the max statistic to enhance cluster-like structures without subjecting them to binarization, allowing the data to retain voxel-wise relevance. A 10,000 Monte Carlo permutation test with FDR correction was performed on all univariate analyses, which equates to a confidence limit of ± 0.0044 for the chosen significance alpha = 0.05 [54]. To reduce the high spatial frequency noise often introduced through poor standard deviation estimates obtained in smaller sample sizes, variance smoothing of slope estimates was also performed at 5 mm half width at half maximum [55]. Multiple comparisons correction was limited to a sample-averaged temporal lobe gray matter mask (using segmentation from FSL FAST [56]), thresholded at 0.5. As part of a secondary analysis to ensure significant findings were not the result of an IQ outlier, we re-tested our primary findings while excluding the one child with ASD who had an IQ of 47. Also, to confirm significant findings were not missed due to our a priori focus on the temporal lobe, we also examined group differences in rCBF and absolute CBF across the entire brain, as well as a frontal lobe gray matter mask analysis based on prior findings [9]. The frontal lobe gray matter mask was created and thresholded in the same manner as the temporal lobe mask.

In order to assess the scan-re-scan reliability of the rCBF measures within each group, intra-class correlations (ICC) were estimated across the two sessions using a mask for the frontal, insula, occipital, parietal, and temporal lobes. For each mask, we multiplied a standard anatomical mask from the MNI atlas with a sample-averaged gray matter mask that was generated by segmenting the T1 scan with FSL’s FAST and thresholding it at 0.5. Each ICC was a two-way ANOVA mixed model with consistency agreement (Shrout and Fliess’s ICC(3,1) model); these ICCs were calculated using custom scripts in R [57] incorporating the ‘irr’ [58] package.

To explore relationships with ASD symptoms and facial recognition, Pearson product-moment correlation and simple linear regression were used to relate rCBF and various behavioral variables only within the ASD group. Controls were not included in these analyses as their face recognition scores and parent ratings had limited variance (ceiling effects). All significance values were subjected to false discovery rate multiple comparisons correction (alpha = 0.05); 95% confidence intervals were calculated where appropriate.

Results

Significant group differences in participant characteristics and behavioral measures

ASD and control groups were matched on age, but not IQ (see Table 1). As predicted, groups differed significantly in parent ratings of communication and socialization adaptive behaviors on the VABS-II, ASD traits on the SRS-2, as well as the Benton Facial Recognition Test.

Significant temporal lobe group differences in pCASL

The TDC group showed significantly greater relative perfusion than the ASD group in four key clusters, which include the bilateral fusiform gyrus and the inferior temporal gyrus (see Fig. 1; Table 2). There was no evidence of greater relative perfusion in the ASD group compared to the TDC group.

Fig. 1
figure 1

TDC > ASD regional cerebral blood flow (rCBF; p < 0.05 corrected with FSL’s threshold-free cluster enhancement algorithm within randomize). Coordinates are in MNI space and are rendered in radiological orientation (right side of image is the left hemisphere). The bar graph illustrates how the main effects of group were present in both individual scan sessions (i.e., no interaction effect with scan session) for the left fusiform gyrus cluster

Table 2 Group differences in rCBF

Acceptable reliability of pCASL

ICCs ranged from good (0.50 < ICC < 0.75) to excellent (0.75 < ICC < 0.90) across both groups. See Table 3 for details.

Table 3 Intra-class correlations (ICC) for each cortical region by diagnostic group

Correlation with behavioral metrics show perfusion relationships with face recognition performance

We correlated mean rCBF values within the four clusters that differed between groups with clinician ratings of ASD symptoms, parent ratings of socialization skills, and performance on a face recognition task. There was a moderate relationship between the largest cluster in the left fusiform gyrus and performance on the Benton Facial Recognition Test (r = 0.52, p < 0.05 (FDR-adjusted), [0.20, 0.73]; see Fig. 2). Partial correlations remained significant when removing variability related to age (r = 0.46, p < 0.05, FDR-adjusted) and FSIQ (r = 0.50, p < 0.05, FDR-adjusted). All other correlations were non-significant (all rs < |0.24|, ps > 0.19).

Fig. 2
figure 2

This scatterplot highlights the correlation between rCBF in the left fusiform gyrus and child performance on the Benton Facial Recognition Task

Secondary analyses confirm primary analysis and yield no additional results

Secondary analyses tested for group differences in rCBF in the temporal lobe after removing the child with ASD and an IQ of 47. Of the four significant clusters identified in the primary analysis, only the largest cluster in the left fusiform gyrus remained significant, as did its correlation with the Benton Facial Recognition task (r = 0.51, p < 0.005). Additional analyses examining rCBF and absolute CBF across the entire brain, as well as within a frontal lobe gray matter mask yielded no significant group differences.

Discussion

Children with ASD demonstrated reductions in rCBF in the bilateral fusiform gyrus and right inferior temporal lobe regions compared to controls—areas critical for social perception and cognition—while watching a video of natural scenes. These findings replicate the fusiform hypoactivation literature [30, 31, 37] but using ASL rather than BOLD fMRI. Furthermore, individual differences in rCBF of the left fusiform gyrus—the cluster with the greatest spatial extent—were associated with facial recognition performance, but not ASD symptoms. While findings of the left (not the right) fusiform gyrus being specifically correlated with face identity recognition skills was a bit surprising, bilateral activation during fMRI social perceptual tasks is the norm. Correlations between the left fusiform’s rCBF and the Benton Facial Recognition Test and not ASD symptom severity is unsurprising as the performance-based measure of facial recognition is more tightly coupled to the putative role of the fusiform gyrus than a broad stroke measure of ASD symptoms [59], perhaps due to the increased granularity and better psychometrics for the Benton Facial Recognition Test vs. the ADOS or the SRS-2.

This study is the first to evaluate the reliability of rCBF in people with ASD using pseudocontinuous ASL; over the same time period as most clinical trials, we observed moderate-to-good reliability for perfusion across gray matter cortex in both ASD and TDC groups. The present study moves us closer to identifying a biologically based marker of ASD that relates to a basic social perception deficit, while highlighting the value of ASL neuroimaging to the field of ASD. These results need to be replicated, but suggest that ASL could provide useful biological markers of ASD in a manner that could also be useful as an outcome measure for intervention trials.

The temporal lobe, and the fusiform gyrus more specifically, has been implicated in the core social and communication impairments in individuals with ASD [30, 31, 37]. In particular, reduced activation of the fusiform during the processing of faces has led to hypotheses focusing on how social information may be de-prioritized in favor of non-social information [29, 34, 60,61,62,63]. However, despite this body of work on the role of the fusiform gyrus in ASD symptoms, there have been numerous negative findings [64,65,66,67,68,69] raising concern about the reliability of this brain difference.

The present study represents a first step in addressing some of these existing concerns. It directly addresses the issue of reliability by demonstrating moderate to good reliability with ICCs across an ~ 8-week interval, and a lack of interaction effects between diagnostic group and session. With a growing emphasis on reproducibility in neuroscience research [70], the demonstration of reliability of CBF across the cortex in both samples is an important foundational step, and it aligns with other efforts to demonstrate reproducibility in functional connectivity (e.g., [71]). The stability of the group difference over time, suggests that measuring rCBF in the fusiform may be a reasonable biologically based marker for clinical trials aiming to improve social perception. However, additional work is needed to identify normative trajectories of rCBF across age, sex, and IQ so that we may quantify when ‘atypical’ levels have been reached at the individual level before this measure could be deemed an appropriate outcome for clinical trial use.

The present study also showed that rCBF has a dimensional relationship with facial recognition performance, such that greater perfusion in children with ASD is associated with better performance on a facial recognition task. This type of effect provides convergent evidence regarding the fusiform’s role in impairments seen in the ASD phenotype from an unrelated MRI modality (ASL vs. blood-oxygen-dependent-level) that has a stronger relationship to neural activity than other functional MRI sequences like BOLD fMRI.

To the best of our knowledge there is only one prior study evaluating rCBF in ASD using the ASL method, which is also the lone study failing to report hypoperfusion in ASD [9]. In Jann et al. (2015), hyperperfusion was observed in inferior temporal regions for the ASD group relative to controls, and no hypoperfusion was observed for the ASD group. There are methodological differences that may account for these discrepancies between the studies. One is that our study had children engage in a passive viewing task that included biological motion (animal movements), whereas the prior study had children rest with no visual stimulation. The second is that the prior study used a different ASL implementation that may have provided lower effective resolution despite identical nominal resolution. In particular, a pure-resting state with limited external stimulation may have influenced the degree of temporal cortex engagement for controls more than the ASD group. While speculative, there is some evidence that employing social stimuli built around context, narrative, or familiarity are much better at eliciting activation in temporal regions [27]. Indirect evidence of this possible explanation comes from a prior study of temporal lobe pathology in patients with mild cognitive impairment. These patients showed enhanced differences in temporal lobe CBF compared to controls during a memory task [26]. Future studies may seek to examine whether the presence and absence of complex, biologically meaningful information alters perfusion in temporal regions during ASL scans to a greater degree for typically developing children or neurotypical adults over those with an ASD diagnosis.

The present study has some notable limitations. This study does not include females, and so our findings of perfusion differences in the fusiform may be subject to interactions with biological sex. Future studies should directly target the inclusion of females to specify rCBF relative to females without ASD and males with ASD. This will inform whether the fusiform is a general marker or one specific to males. While children were instructed to watch the video, we did not monitor eye movements to confirm compliance. Future investigations should consider monitoring engagement with videos to determine if this influences results and also as a potential criterion for inclusion in analyses. This study had a wider IQ range in the ASD group than the TDC group leading to a significant difference with a large effect. Of note, while the ASD group continued to show lower relative perfusion in the largest cluster in the left fusiform gyrus relative to controls, the other three clusters in the temporal lobes were no longer significant. This pattern of finding suggests that lower IQ may be associated with some degree with lower relative perfusion in the temporal lobes. However, the robustness of group differences in the left fusiform gyrus and the correlation between relative perfusion in this region and face recognition skills, suggests the findings may be specific to social functioning and not merely an indicator of cognitive impairment. Furthermore, our study offers optimism that ASL could be implemented to identify relevant biomarkers for individuals with co-occurring ASD and intellectual disability diagnoses.

Conclusions

Identification of a reliable biological marker in ASD remains elusive. The present study built upon existing functional MRI studies that have implicated the fusiform gyrus in the face processing impairments that are characteristic of the ASD phenotype. The present study measured rCBF using a non-invasive method, pseudocontinuous ASL. The results confirm early reports of hypoperfusion in the fusiform gyrus of individuals with ASD, and that greater perfusion in ASD is also associated with better performance on a face recognition task. Further, ASL was shown to have reasonably good reliability across an ~ 8-week period, which is notable since this time period corresponds with that of an average clinical trial. Thus, the present study has extended prior work in the fusiform in ASD by demonstrating its reliability and sensitivity to individual differences in ASD using an underutilized metric of neural function. This study has also provided the preliminary psychometric data needed for pursuing perfusion in the fusiform gyrus as a potential target in treatment studies.