Skip to main content
Log in

How associations between products and numbers in brand names affect consumer attitudes: introducing multi-context numbers

  • Original Article
  • Published:
Journal of Brand Management Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Drawing on numerical cognition research, we identify a set of multi-context numbers (MCN) that originate from the decimal (10), duodecimal (12) and sexagesimal (60) numeral systems frequently used in numerous domains (e.g., 10, 12, 20, 24, 60, 360, 100). We propose and show that inclusion of MCN in alphanumeric brand names (ABN) generates more favorable consumer attitudes and higher preferences for product extensions in different domains. We examine three types of fit, between (1) parent brands and numbers, (2) product categories and numbers and (3) parent brands and product categories. We find that the effects of ABN numbers are mainly mediated by product–number associations. Accordingly, while some numbers that are strongly associated with the product category (e.g., 401 and retirement services) or the parent brand (e.g., Heinz and 57; Levi’s and 501) or that are familiar to consumers (e.g., 18, 21) generate favorable consumer responses in specific contexts, the same numbers fail in other product domains (e.g., 401/57/18/21 taxi service). In four empirical studies, we demonstrate that MCN in ABN can achieve and maintain favorable consumer responses and receive higher preferences than other very familiar numbers in various product extension contexts, regardless of parent brand names or product categories. Our findings suggest that it is ideal to use MCN in new extensions.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Aaker, D.A., and K.L. Keller. 1990. Consumer evaluations of brand extensions. Journal of Marketing 54(1): 27–41.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ang, S. 1997. Chinese consumers’ perception of alpha-numeric brand names. Asia Pacific Journal of Marketing and Logistics 8 (3): 31–48.

    Google Scholar 

  • Auh, S., and E. Shih. 2009. Brand name and consumer inference making in multigenerational product introduction context. Journal of Brand Management 16 (7): 439–454.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bietenholz, W. 2013. Should we revitalize the Maya numerals? The Mathematical Intelligencer 35 (4): 21–26.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bornstein, R.F., and P.R. D’Agostino. 1994. The attribution and discounting of perceptual fluency: Preliminary tests of a perceptual fluency/attributional model of the mere exposure effect. Social Cognition 12 (2): 103–128.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Boyd, C.W. 1985. Point of view: Alpha-numeric brand names. Journal of Advertising Research 25 (5): 48–52.

    Google Scholar 

  • Coupland, N. 2011. How frequent are numbers? Language and Communication 31(1): 27–37.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dehaene, S. 1997. The number sense. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dehaene, S. 2001. Précis of the number sense. Mind and Language 16(1): 16–36.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dehaene, S., and J. Mehler. 1992. Cross-linguistic regularities in frequency of number words. Cognition 43(1): 1–29.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fitousi, D. 2010. Dissociating between cardinal and ordinal and between the value and size magnitude of coins. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review 17 (6): 889–894.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gunasti, K., and B. Devezer. 2016. How competitor brands affect within-brand choice. Marketing Letters 27 (4): 715–727.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gunasti, K., and T. Ozcan. 2016. Consumer reactions to round numbers in brand names. Marketing Letters 27 (2): 309–322.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gunasti, K., and W.T. Ross Jr. 2010. How and when alphanumeric brand names affect consumer preferences. Journal of Marketing Research 47 (6): 1177–1192.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hayes, A.F. 2013. Introduction to mediation, moderation, and conditional process analysis. New York: The Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hayes, A.F., and K.J. Preacher. 2014. Statistical mediation analysis with a multicategorical independent variable. British Journal of Mathematical and Statistical Psychology 67 (3): 451–470.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ifrah, G. 2000. The universal history of numbers: From prehistory to the invention of the computer. New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jansen, C., and M. Pollmann. 2001. On round numbers: Pragmatic aspects of numerical expressions. Journal of Quantitative Linguistics 8 (3): 187–201.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kara, S., K. Gunasti, and W.T. Ross Jr. 2015. Is it the ‘alpha’ or the ‘numeric’?: Consumers’ evaluation of letter versus number changes in alphanumeric brand names. Journal of Brand Management 22 (6): 515–535.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kandel, S., O. Sarig, and A. Wohl. 2001. Do investors prefer round stock prices? Evidence from Israeli IPO auctions. Journal of Banking and Finance 25 (8): 1543–1551.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kashmiri, S., and V. Mahajan. 2010. What’s in a name? An analysis of the strategic behavior of family firms. International Journal of Research in Marketing 27 (3): 271–280.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • King, D., and C. Janiszewski. 2011. The sources and consequences of the fluent processing of numbers. Journal of Marketing Research 48 (2): 327–341.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kwong, J.Y.Y., and E.K.F. Wong. 2006. The role of ratio differences in the framing of numerical information. International Journal of Research in Marketing 23 (4): 385–394.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lee, Y.H., and K.S. Ang. 2003. Brand name suggestiveness: A Chinese language perspective. International Journal of Research in Marketing 20 (4): 323–335.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lotz, J. 1955. On language and culture. International Journal of American Linguistics 21 (2): 187–189.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Macey, S.L. 1989. The dynamics of progress: Time, method, and measure. Atlanta: University of Georgia Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mandler, G. 1982. The structure of value: Accounting for taste. In Affect and cognition: The 17th annual Carnegie symposium, ed. M.S. Clark and S.T. Fiske, 3–36. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Meyers-Levy, J., T.A. Louie, and M.T. Curren. 1994. How does the congruity of brand names affect evaluations of brand name extensions? Journal of Applied Psychology 79(1): 46–53.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mao, H., and H.S. Krishnan. 2006. Effects of prototype and exemplar fit on brand extension evaluations: A two-process contingency model. Journal of Consumer Research 33(1): 41–49.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Michel, J.B., Y.K. Shen, A.P. Aiden, A. Veres, M.K. Gray, J.P. Pickett, D. Hoiberg, D. Clancy, P. Norvig, J. Orwant, and S. Pinker. 2011. Quantitative analysis of culture using millions of digitized books. Science 331 (6014): 176–182.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Park, C.W., S. Milberg, and R. Lawson. 1991. Evaluation of brand extensions: the role of product feature similarity and brand concept consistency. Journal of Consumer Research 18 (2): 41–49.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pavia, T.M., and J.A. Costa. 1993. The winning number: Consumer perceptions of alphanumeric brand names. Journal of Marketing 57 (July): 85–98.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schiffman, J. 1982. Fundamental operations in the duodecimal system. The Duodecimal Bulletin 31: 15–20.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schimmel, A. 1993. The mystery of numbers. NY: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schindler, R.M., and P.N. Kirby. 1997. Patterns of rightmost digits used in advertising prices: Implications for nine-ending effects. Journal of Consumer Research 24 (2): 192–201.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sigurd, B. 1988. Round numbers. Language in Society 17 (2): 243–252.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stiving, M., and R.S. Winer. 1997. An empirical analysis of price endings with scanner data. Journal of Consumer Research 24(1): 57–67.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Thomas, M., and V. Morwitz. 2009. The ease of computation effect: The interplay of metacognitive experiences and naïve theories in judgments of price differences. Journal of Marketing Research 46(1): 81–92.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Whittlesea, B.W.A., and L.D. Williams. 2000. The source of feelings of familiarity: The discrepancy-attribution hypothesis. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition 26 (3): 547–565.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Yan, D., and R. Duclos. 2013. Making sense of numbers: Effects of alphanumeric brands on consumer inference. International Journal of Research in Marketing 30 (2): 179–184.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zajonc, R.B. 1968. Attitudinal effects of mere exposure. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 9 (2): 1–27.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Timucin Ozcan.

Ethics declarations

Conflict of interest

All authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.

Electronic supplementary material

Below is the link to the electronic supplementary material.

Supplementary material 1 (DOCX 3437 kb)

Appendices

Appendix 1

Common numeral system

Uses

Decimal (10)

History/Origins:

Dates back to beginning of writing. Written evidence of its use in ancient Egyptian and Cretan hieroglyphs. Based on human anatomy, currently used by all modern civilizations

All metric system measures (height, weight, volume, length, area)

Military units, ranks, money bills, rankings, ratings, percentages, pricing, grouping, financial indices

Roman, Greek, Brahmi, Chinese, Hindu-Arabic numerals are all based on the decimal system including special notations for 1, 10, 100, 5, 50, 500…etc

Duodecimal/dozenal (12)

History/Origins:

Dates back to Sumerians and Babylonians. Based on human anatomy and single-hand counting method using thumb to count 4 × 3 finger bones

Widely adopted in Anglo-Saxon cultures and continued even after decimalization

All non-metric measurements of length/area/ weight

1 ft = 12 inches, 1sq ft = 144 sq inches, 12 ounce = 1 tory pound

Monetary/Math: 1 shilling = 12 pence, 240 pence = 1 pound sterling (English and Irish), prices quoted as 12ths, Roman fraction system in 12s

Packaging/grouping: dozen, 12-pack, 24-pack, gross = 144 (12 dozens), great gross = 123 = 1728

Time: 1 year = 12 months, 1 day = 24 h, day/night (am/pm) = 12 h, ½ year = 6 months, Chinese calendar has 12 year cycles, 12 lunar cycles

Babylonians originally had 12 h in a day

Other: 12 zodiac signs, 12 apostles, 12 imams, 12 wars, 12 petals, 12 jurors

12 Functional keys on key boards (F1-12) and telephones (0–9,*,#,)

12 notes in an octave, 12 teams in rugby, soccer leagues, finals, etc

Sexagesimal (60)

History/Origins:

Dates back to 3100BC Sumerians and Babylonians. It is a combination of the single-hand counting method (12 system) with the right-hand counting (× 5) to reach 60.

It became popular in second- and eighteenth-century mathematics and astronomy especially for Hellenistic civilizations

Time: 1 h = 60, 1 min = 60 s (i.e., 2nd order [1/60] of an hour)

e.g., 4:22:33 = 4 × 602 + 22 × 601 + 33 × 600 s

Chinese calendar has a sexagenary cycle, in which days or years are named by positions in a sequence of ten stems and in another sequence of 12 branches. The same stem and branch repeat every 60 steps throughout this cycle.

Geometry/trigonometry, mathematical astronomy (fractions), arcs, circle,

angles, degrees, 360, 180, 90, 60, 30

Geographic locations: Degrees of Parallels and Meridians, Seconds

French: 70 = soixante-dix (sixty ten) 75 = soixante-quinze (sixty fifteen)

Other: 60 mph as a common speed limit and reference for acceleration 0–60

Binary (2)

History/Origins:

Morse code, data processing

In 1617, John Napier’s location arithmetic system. Multiples of 2 are often observed in technology contexts: 32, 64, 128, 256, 512, 1024

Appendix 2

Study 1: Experimental scenario and an example for the choice set

“Imagine that some well-known brands are planning to sell certain products that they don’t offer right now. To launch these products, brand managers are going back and forth with using brand name number combinations such as Porsche 911 or Heinz 57. You are asked to evaluate these brands with or without brand name number combinations and pick one of them as your choice.”

Please pick one of the alternatives as your preference.

Intel 10 Game Console

Intel Game Console

Study 2: stimuli

Number sets rated for each brand/product

Brand/product sets listed in the order of matching associations with numbers

Specific semantic associations

100 (MCN)

Holmes air purifier*

MCN

69

Trojan condoms

Has a sexual reference

86

Raid bug spray

Means “to terminate” in slang language

101

Sushi for Beginners Text Book

Associated with introductory courses

360 (MCN)

Clif protein bar*

MCN

312

Chicago taxi service

Chicago’s area code—participants’ location

314

St. Louis limo service

St. Louis’ area code—participants’ location

401

H&R Block retirement software

Number used in retirement plans

24 (MCN)

Cuisinart toaster*

MCN

21

Smirnoff vodka

Legal age for drinking

23

Nike Jordan shoes

Michael Jordan’s jersey number

28

February Fashion Magazine

Total number of days in February

1000 (MCN)

Ikea sofa bed*

MCN

1024

Dell hard disk drive

Bits of data computing

1040

TurboTax tax software

Number label on federal tax forms

1080

Sony HDTV

Associated with HDTV resolutions

  1. *These products were not associated with any of the numbers
  2. All four numbers in each set were rated for all four products (e.g., 100, 69, 101, 86) and were compared as brand names for each of Holmes, Trojan, Raid and Sushi for Beginners
  3. MCN Multi-context numbers

Study 3: Stimulus example

Which one of these ketchup alternatives would you pick?

figure a

Please rate your attitude toward the following brand name number combinations (1–7 Extremely dislike/Extremely like)

Baskin Robbins 31 Ketchup, Baskin Robbins 57 Ketchup

Please indicate your agreement with the following statements:

  • Baskin Robbins is strongly associated with Ketchup

  • Ketchups are strongly associated with number 31

  • Baskin Robbins is strongly associated with number 31

  • Ketchups are strongly associated with number 57

  • Baskin Robbins is strongly associated with number 57

  • Ketchup is a typical product for Baskin Robbins

  • 31 is a typical brand name number for Ketchup

  • 31 is a typical number for Baskin Robbins brand

  • 57 is a typical brand name number for Ketchup

  • 57 is a typical number for Baskin Robbins brand

Appendix 4

Detailed results of mediation model 1—based on d1 coding as discussed in “Appendix 3” in ESM (d1=0, d2=0 for 360; d1=1, d2=0 for 401; d1=0, d2=1 for 860, and 360 acts as a control group)

Model 4: Y = Attitude; X = No401; Mediator1 = Product number; Mediator2 = Brand Number; Control = No860

Outcome: product number

R = .3392, R2 = .1150, F(2,290) =  18.8472, p < .001

 

B

Se

T

p

LLCI

ULCI

Constant

3.3535

.1552

21.6110

.0000

3.0481

3.6590

No401

1.3307

.2218

6.0007

.0000

.8942

1.7671

No860

.4040

.2195

1.8411

.0666

− .0279

.8360

Outcome: brand number

R = .2853, R2 = .0814, F(2,290) = 12.8509, p < .0001

 

B

se

T

p

LLCI

ULCI

Constant

3.3131

.1334

24.8332

.0000

3.0505

3.5757

No401

.6763

.1907

3.5475

.0005

.3011

1.0516

No860

− .2626

.1887

− 1.3919

.1650

− .6340

.1087

Outcome: attitude

R =  .3927, R2 = .1542, F(4,288) =  13.1315, p < .001

 

b

Se

T

p

LLCI

ULCI

Constant

3.9390

.2970

13.2609

.0000

3.3544

4.5236

Product number

.2582

.0626

4.1237

.0000

.1350

.3814

Brand number

.2418

.0728

3.3207

.0010

.0985

.3852

No401

− .6500

.2334

− 2.7845

.0057

− 1.1095

− .1906

No860

− .6368

.2203

− 2.8899

.0041

− 1.0705

− .2031

Total effect model

Outcome: attitudes

R =  .1551, R2 = .0240, F(2,290) =  3.5724, p < .001

 

B

se

T

p

LLCI

ULCI

Constant

5.6061

.1644

34.0937

.0000

5.2824

5.9297

No401

− .1429

.2350

− .6082

.5436

− .6054

.3196

No860

− .5960

.2325

− 2.5628

.0109

− 1.0536

− .1383

Total, direct and indirect effects

Total effect of X on Y

B

SE

t

P

LLCI

ULCI

 

− .1429

.2350

− .6082

.5436

− .6054

.3196

 

Direct effect of X on Y

B

SE

t

P

LLCI

ULCI

− .6500

.2334

− 2.7845

.0057

− 1.1095

− .1906

Indirect effect of X on Y

 

b

Boot SE

BootLLCI

BootULCI

Total

.5071

.1204

.2953

.7637

Product number

.3436

.1066

.1612

.5803

Brand number

.1636

.0773

.0455

.3594

Contrast

.1800

.1420

− .0990

.4690

Contrast: Product number minus brand number

Detailed results of mediation Model 2 based on d2 coding as explained in “Appendix 3” in ESM (d1 = 0, d2 = 0 for 360; d1 = 1, d2 = 0 for 401; d1 = 0, d2 = 1 for 860, and 360 acts as a control group)

Model 4: Y  =  Attitude; X  =  No860; Mediator1  =  Product number; Mediator2  =  Brand number; Control =  No401

Outcome: product number

R =  .3392, R2 = .1150, F(2,290) =  18.8472, p < .001

 

B

se

T

p

LLCI

ULCI

Constant

3.3535

.1552

21.6110

.0000

3.0481

3.6590

No860

.4040

.2195

1.8411

.0666

− .0279

.8360

No401

1.3307

.2218

6.0007

.0000

.8942

1.7671

Outcome: brand number

R = .2853, R2 = .0814, F(2,290) = 12.8509, p < .0001

 

B

se

T

p

LLCI

ULCI

Constant

3.3131

.1334

24.8332

.0000

3.0505

3.5757

No860

− .2626

.1887

− 1.3919

.1650

− .6340

.1087

No401

.6763

.1907

3.5475

.0005

.3011

1.0516

Outcome: attitude

R = .3927, R2 = .1542, F(4,288) = 13.1315, p < .001

 

b

se

T

p

LLCI

ULCI

Constant

3.9390

.2970

13.2609

.0000

3.3544

4.5236

Product number

.2582

.0626

4.1237

.0000

.1350

.3814

Brand number

.2418

.0728

3.3207

.0010

.0985

.3852

No860

− .6368

.2203

− 2.8899

.0041

− 1.0705

− .2031

No401

− .6500

.2334

− 2.7845

.0057

− 1.1095

− .1906

Total effect model

Outcome: attitudes

R = .1551, R2 = .0240, F(2,290) = 3.5724, p < .001

 

B

se

t

p

LLCI

ULCI

Constant

5.6061

.1644

34.0937

.0000

5.2824

5.9297

No860

− .5960

.2325

− 2.5628

.0109

− 1.0536

-.1383

No401

− .1429

.2350

− .6082

.5436

− .6054

.3196

Total, direct, and indirect effects

Total effect of X on Y

b

SE

t

P

LLCI

ULCI

− .5960

.2325

− 2.5628

.0109

− 1.0536

− .1383

Direct effect of X on Y

b

SE

t

P

LLCI

ULCI

− .6368

.2203

− 2.8899

.0041

− 1.0705

− .2031

Indirect effect of X on Y

 

b

Boot SE

BootLLCI

BootULCI

Total

.0408

.0954

− .1389

.2391

Product number

.1043

.0641

.0059

.2606

Brand number

− .0635

.0503

− .1957

.0089

(C1)

.1678

.0646

.0560

.3082

Contrast: product number minus brand number

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Ozcan, T., Gunasti, K. How associations between products and numbers in brand names affect consumer attitudes: introducing multi-context numbers. J Brand Manag 26, 176–194 (2019). https://doi.org/10.1057/s41262-018-0125-1

Download citation

  • Revised:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1057/s41262-018-0125-1

Keywords

Navigation