Skip to main content

Abstract

Psycholinguistic methods provide insights into the mental representation of language. In the first part of this chapter, we discuss common psycholinguistic tasks and measures together with their strengths and limitations. We review traditional behavioral measures such as response time as well as newer measures, including eye-tracking and event-related potentials. In the second part of the chapter, we discuss the major experimental paradigms that are employed with psycholinguistic tasks and measures and consider the research conducted with these paradigms. We review classic priming paradigms as well as state-of-the-art paradigms that are particularly useful for applied linguistics research, such as artificial linguistic systems. Finally, we consider future directions in applied linguistics research using psycholinguistic methodologies.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 299.00
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 379.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 379.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

References

  • Altarriba, J., & Basnight-Brown, D. M. (2007). Methodological considerations in performing semantic- and translation-priming experiments across languages. Behavior Research Methods, 39(1), 1–18.

    Google Scholar 

  • Altmann, G. T. M., & Kamide, Y. (2007). The real-time mediation of visual attention by language and world knowledge: Linking anticipatory (and other) eye movements to linguistic processing. Journal of Memory and Language, 57(4), 502–518.

    Google Scholar 

  • Arnold, J. E., Fagnano, M., & Tanenhaus, M. K. (2003). Disfluencies signal theee, um, new information. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 32(1), 25–36.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bahlmann, J., Gunter, T. C., & Friederici, A. D. (2006). Hierarchical and linear sequence processing: An electrophysiological exploration of two different grammar types. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 18(11), 1829–1842.

    Google Scholar 

  • Balukas, C., & Koops, C. (2015). Spanish-English bilingual voice onset time in spontaneous code-switching. International Journal of Bilingualism, 19(4), 423–443.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barber, H. A., Otten, L. J., Kousta, S.-T., & Vigliocco, G. (2013). Concreteness in word processing: ERP and behavioral effects in a lexical decision task. Brain and Language, 125(1), 47–53.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bartolotti, J., & Marian, V. (2012). Language learning and control in monolinguals and bilinguals. Cognitive Science, 36(6), 1129–1147.

    Google Scholar 

  • Basnight-Brown, D. M., & Altarriba, J. (2007). Differences in semantic and translation priming across languages: The role of language direction and language dominance. Memory & Cognition, 35(5), 953–965.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bates, E., D’Amico, S., Jacobsen, T., Székely, A., Andonova, E., Devescovi, A., & Pléh, C. (2003). Timed picture naming in seven languages. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 10(2), 344–380.

    Google Scholar 

  • Batterink, L., & Neville, H. (2011). Implicit and explicit mechanisms of word learning in a narrative context: An event-related potential study. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 23(11), 3181–3196.

    Google Scholar 

  • Batterink, L., & Neville, H. (2013). Implicit and explicit second language training recruit common neural mechanisms for syntactic processing. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 25(6), 936–951.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bisson, M.-J., Van Heuven, W. J. B., Conklin, K., & Tunney, R. J. (2012). Processing of native and foreign language subtitles in films: An eye-tracking study. Applied Psycholinguistics, 35(2), 399–418.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bock, J. K. (1986). Syntactic persistence in language production. Cognitive Psychology, 18(3), 355–387.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bortfeld, H., Leon, S. D., Bloom, J. E., Schober, M. F., & Brennan, S. E. (2001). Disfluency rates in conversation: Effects of age, relationship, topic, role, and gender. Language and Speech, 44(2), 123–147.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bosker, H. R., Quené, H., Sanders, T., & de Jong, N. H. (2014). Native ‘um’s elicit prediction of low-frequency referents, but non-native ‘um’s do not. Journal of Memory and Language, 75, 104–116.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bowden, H. W. (2016). Assessing second language oral proficiency for research. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 38(4), 647–675.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bowden, H. W., Steinhauer, K., Sanz, C., & Ullman, M. T. (2013). Native-like brain processing of syntax can be attained by university foreign language learners. Neuropsychologia, 51(13), 2492–2511.

    Google Scholar 

  • Breitenstein, C., Zwitserlood, P., de Vries, M. H., Feldhues, C., Knecht, S., & Dobel, C. (2007). Five days versus a lifetime: Intense associative vocabulary training generates lexically integrated words. Restorative Neurology and Neuroscience, 25(5&6), 493–500.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carey, S., & Bartlett, E. (1978). Acquiring a single new word. Papers and Reports on Child Language Development, 15, 17–29.

    Google Scholar 

  • Corley, M., & Hartsuiker, R. J. (2011). Why um helps auditory word recognition: The temporal delay hypothesis. PLoS ONE, 6(5), e19792.

    Google Scholar 

  • Crinion, J., Turner, R., Grogan, A., Hanakawa, T., Noppeney, U., Devlin, J. T., … Usui, K. (2006). Language control in the bilingual brain. Science, 312(5779), 1537–1540.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dahan, D., & Tanenhaus, M. K. (2004). Continuous mapping from sound to meaning in spoken-language comprehension: Immediate effects of verb-based thematic constraints. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 30(2), 498.

    Google Scholar 

  • DeLong, K. A., Groppe, D. M., Urbach, T. P., & Kutas, M. (2012). Thinking ahead or not? Natural aging and anticipation during reading. Brain and Language, 121(3), 226–239.

    Google Scholar 

  • Duñabeitia, J. A., Crepaldi, D., Meyer, A. S., New, B., Pliatsikas, C., Smolka, E., & Brysbaert, M. (2017). MultiPic: A standardized set of 750 drawings with norms for six European languages. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 22, 1–24.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dussias, P. E., Kroff, J. R. V., Tamargo, R. E. G., & Gerfen, C. (2013). When gender and looking go hand in hand. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 35(2), 353.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ellis, R. (2005). Measuring implicit and explicit knowledge of a second language: A psychometric study. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 27(2), 141–172.

    Google Scholar 

  • Erlam, R. (2006). Elicited imitation as a measure of L2 implicit knowledge: An empirical validation study. Applied Linguistics, 27(3), 464–491.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ettlinger, M., Morgan-Short, K., Faretta-Stutenberg, M., & Wong, P. (2016). The relationship between artificial and second language learning. Cognitive Science, 40(4), 822–847.

    Google Scholar 

  • Federmeier, K. D., & Kutas, M. (1999). A rose by any other name: Long-term memory structure and sentence processing. Journal of Memory and Language, 41(4), 469–495.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fine, A. B., & Jaeger, F. T. (2013). Evidence for implicit learning in syntactic comprehension. Cognitive Science, 37(3), 578–591.

    Google Scholar 

  • Finkbeiner, M., & Nicol, J. (2003). Semantic category effects in second language word learning. Applied Psycholinguistics, 24(03), 369–383.

    Google Scholar 

  • Flege, J. E. (1991). Age of learning affects the authenticity of voice-onset time (VOT) in stop consonants produced in a second language. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 89(1), 395–411.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fox Tree, J. E. (1995). The effects of false starts and repetitions on the processing of subsequent words in spontaneous speech. Journal of Memory and Language, 34(6), 709–738.

    Google Scholar 

  • Freeman, J. B., & Ambady, N. (2010). MouseTracker: Software for studying real-time mental processing using a computer mouse-tracking method. Behavior Research Methods, 42(1), 226–241.

    Google Scholar 

  • Frenck-Mestre, C., & Prince, P. (1997). Second language autonomy. Journal of Memory and Language, 37(4), 481–501.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gillon Dowens, M., Guo, T., Guo, J., Barber, H., & Carreiras, M. (2011). Gender and number processing in Chinese learners of Spanish—Evidence from event related potentials. Neuropsychologia, 49, 1651–1659.

    Google Scholar 

  • Godfroid, A., Housen, A., & Boers, F. (2013). An eye for words: Gauging the role of attention in L2 vocabulary acquisition by means of eye tracking. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 35(3), 483–517.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goslin, J., Duffy, H., & Floccia, C. (2012). An ERP investigation of regional and foreign accent processing. Brain and Language, 122(2), 92–102.

    Google Scholar 

  • Grey, S., Cox, J. C., Serafini, E. J., & Sanz, C. (2015). The role of individual differences in the study abroad context: Cognitive capacity and language development during short-term intensive language exposure. Modern Language Journal, 99(1), 137–157.

    Google Scholar 

  • Grey, S., & Van Hell, J. G. (2017). Foreign-accented speaker identity affects neural correlates of sentence comprehension. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 42, 93–108.

    Google Scholar 

  • Grey, S., Williams, J. N., & Rebuschat, P. (2014). Incidental exposure and L3 learning of morphosyntax. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 36(4), 611–645.

    Google Scholar 

  • Grodner, D., & Sedivy, J. (2011). The effect of speaker-specific information on pragmatic inferences. The processing and acquisition of reference, 2327, 239–272.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hamrick, P., & Sachs, R. (2017). Establishing evidence of learning in experiments employing artificial linguistic systems. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 1–17.

    Google Scholar 

  • Harley, T. A. (2013). The psychology of language: From data to theory. East Sussex: Psychology Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hartsuiker, R. J., Pickering, M. J., & Veltkamp, E. (2004). Is syntax separate or shared between languages? Cross-linguistic syntactic priming in Spanish-English bilinguals. Psychological Science, 15(6), 409–414.

    Google Scholar 

  • Huettig, F., Rommers, J., & Meyer, A. S. (2011). Using the visual world paradigm to study language processing: A review and critical evaluation. Acta Psychologica, 137(2), 151–171.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kaan, E., & Swaab, T. Y. (2003). Repair, revision, and complexity in syntactic reanalysis: An electrophysiological differentiation. Journal of Cognitive Neuoscience, 15, 98–110.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kamide, Y., Altmann, G. T. M., & Haywood, S. L. (2003). The time-course of prediction in incremental sentence processing: Evidence from anticipatory eye movements. Journal of Memory and Language, 49(1), 133–156.

    Google Scholar 

  • Karuza, E. A., Newport, E. L., Aslin, R. N., Starling, S. J., Tivarus, M. E., & Bavelier, D. (2013). The neural correlates of statistical learning in a word segmentation task: An fMRI study. Brain and Language, 127(1), 46–54.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kootstra, G. J., van Hell, J. G., & Dijkstra, T. (2010). Syntactic alignment and shared word order in code-switched sentence production: Evidence from bilingual monologue and dialogue. Journal of Memory and Language, 63(2), 210–231.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kroll, J. F., Van Hell, J. G., Tokowicz, N., & Green, D. W. (2010). The revised hierarchical model: A critical review and assessment. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 13(03), 373–381.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lan, Y.-J., Fang, S.-Y., Legault, J., & Li, P. (2015). Second language acquisition of Mandarin Chinese vocabulary: Context of learning effects. Educational Technology Research and Development, 63(5), 671–690.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lisker, L., & Abramson, A. S. (1964). A cross-language study of voicing in initial stops: Acoustical measurements. Word, 20(3), 384–422.

    Google Scholar 

  • Luck, S. J. (2014). An introduction to the event-related potential technique. Cambridge, MA: MIT press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Martin, C. D., Thierry, G., Kuipers, J.-R., Boutonnet, B., Foucart, A., & Costa, A. (2013). Bilinguals reading in their second language do not predict upcoming words as native readers do. Journal of Memory and Language, 69(4), 574–588.

    Google Scholar 

  • McDonough, K., & Mackey, A. (2008). Syntactic priming and ESL question development. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 30(1), 31–47.

    Google Scholar 

  • McDonough, K., & Trofimovich, P. (2009). Using priming methods in second language research. New York, NY: Taylor & Francis.

    Google Scholar 

  • McLaughlin, J., Osterhout, L., & Kim, A. (2004). Neural correlates of second-language word learning: Minimal instruction produces rapid change. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 7, 703–704.

    Google Scholar 

  • Morett, L. M., & Macwhinney, B. (2013). Syntactic transfer in English-speaking Spanish learners. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 16(1), 132–151.

    Google Scholar 

  • Morgan-Short, K. (2014). Electrophysiological approaches to understanding second language acquisition: A field reaching its potential. Annual Review of Applied Linguistics, 34, 15–36.

    Google Scholar 

  • Morgan-Short, K., Sanz, C., Steinhauer, K., & Ullman, M. T. (2010). Second language acquisition of gender agreement in explicit and implicit training conditions: An event-related potential study. Language Learning, 60(1), 154–193.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mueller, J. L., Hahne, A., Fujii, Y., & Friederici, A. D. (2005). Native and nonnative speakers’ processing of a miniature version of Japanese as revealed by ERPs. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 17(8), 1229–1244.

    Google Scholar 

  • Neville, H. J., Nicol, J. L., Barss, A., Forster, K. I., & Garrett, M. F. (1991). Syntactically based sentence processing classes: Evidence from event-related brain potentials. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 3(2), 151–165.

    Google Scholar 

  • Newman, A. J., Tremblay, A., Nichols, E. S., Neville, H. J., & Ullman, M. T. (2012). The influence of language proficiency on lexical-semantic processing in native and late learners of English. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 24, 1205–1223.

    Google Scholar 

  • Newman, R. L., & Connolly, J. F. (2009). Electrophysiological markers of pre-lexical speech processing: Evidence for bottom–up and top–down effects on spoken word processing. Biological Psychology, 80(1), 114–121.

    Google Scholar 

  • Oviatt, S. (1995). Predicting spoken disfluencies during human–computer interaction. Computer Speech & Language, 9(1), 19–35.

    Google Scholar 

  • Perruchet, P., & Pacteau, C. (1990). Synthetic grammar learning: Implicit rule abstraction or explicit fragmentary knowledge? Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 119(3), 264.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pickering, M. J., & Ferreira, V. S. (2008). Structural priming: A critical review. Psychological Bulletin, 134(3), 427.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rassaei, E., Moinzadeh, A., & Youhannaee, M. (2012). Effects of recasts and metalinguistic corrective feedback on the acquisition of implicit and explicit L2 knowledge. The Journal of Language Teaching and Learning, 2(1), 59–75.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rayner, K. (1998). Eye movements in reading and information processing: 20 years of research. Psychological Bulletin, 124, 372–422.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reber, A. S. (1967). Implicit learning of artificial grammars. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 6, 855–863.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reber, A. S. (1990). On the primacy of the implicit: Comment on Perruchet and Pacteau. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 119(3), 340–342.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rebuschat, P., & Williams, J. N. (2012). Implicit and explicit knowledge in second language acquisition. Applied Psycholinguistics, 33(4), 829–856.

    Google Scholar 

  • Robinson, P. (1997). Generalizability and automaticity of second language learning under implicit, incidental, enhanced, and instructed conditions. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 19(02), 223–247.

    Google Scholar 

  • Robinson, P. (2005). Cognitive abilities, chunk-strength, and frequency effects in implicit artificial grammar and incidental L2 leaning: Replications of Reber, Walkenfeld, and Hernstadt (1991) and Knowlton and Squire (1996) and their relevance for SLA. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 27(2), 235–268.

    Google Scholar 

  • Roche, J. M., Peters, B., & Dale, R. (2015). “Your tone says it all”: The processing and interpretation of affective language. Speech Communication, 66, 47–64.

    Google Scholar 

  • Saffran, J. R., Aslin, R. N., & Newport, E. L. (1996). Statistical learning by 8-month-old infants. Science, 274(5294), 1926–1928.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sanz, C., & Grey, S. (2015). Effects of conditions on L2 development: Beyond accuracy. In P. Rebuschat (Ed.), Implicit and explicit learning of languages (pp. 301–324). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.

    Google Scholar 

  • Segalowitz, N. S., & Segalowitz, S. J. (1993). Skilled performance, practice, and the differentiation of speed-up from automatization effects: Evidence from second language word recognition. Applied Psycholinguistics, 14, 369–369.

    Google Scholar 

  • Spada, N., Shiu, J. L. J., & Tomita, Y. (2015). Validating an elicited imitation task as a measure of implicit knowledge: Comparisons with other validation studies. Language Learning, 65(3), 723–751.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stafford, C. A., Bowden, H. W., & Sanz, C. (2012). Optimizing language instruction: Matters of explicitness, practice, and cue learning. Language Learning, 62(3), 741–768.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sunderman, G., & Kroll, J. F. (2009). When study abroad fails to deliver: The internal resources threshold effect. Applied Psycholinguistics, 30, 79–99.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tagarelli, K. M. (2014). The neurocognition of adult second language learning: An fMRI study. (Doctoral Dissertation), Georgetown University.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tagarelli, K. M., Ruiz, S., Moreno Vega, J. L., & Rebuschat, P. (2016). Variability in second language learning: The roles of individual differences, learning conditions, and linguistic complexity. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 38(2), 293–316.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tanner, D., & Van Hell, J. G. (2014). ERPs reveal individual differences in morphosyntactic processing. Neuropsychologia, 56, 289–301.

    Google Scholar 

  • Weiss, D. J., Poepsel, T., & Gerfen, C. (2015). Tracking multiple inputs. In P. Rebuschat (Ed.), Implicit and explicit learning of languages (pp. 167–190). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wu, S. L., & Ortega, L. (2013). Measuring global oral proficiency in SLA research: A new elicited imitation test of L2 Chinese. Foreign Language Annals, 46(4), 680–704.

    Google Scholar 

  • Yan, X., Maeda, Y., Lv, J., & Ginther, A. (2016). Elicited imitation as a measure of second language proficiency: A narrative review and meta-analysis. Language Testing, 33(4), 497–528.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Sarah Grey .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Copyright information

© 2018 The Author(s)

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Grey, S., Tagarelli, K.M. (2018). Psycholinguistic Methods. In: Phakiti, A., De Costa, P., Plonsky, L., Starfield, S. (eds) The Palgrave Handbook of Applied Linguistics Research Methodology. Palgrave Macmillan, London. https://doi.org/10.1057/978-1-137-59900-1_14

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1057/978-1-137-59900-1_14

  • Publisher Name: Palgrave Macmillan, London

  • Print ISBN: 978-1-137-59899-8

  • Online ISBN: 978-1-137-59900-1

  • eBook Packages: Social SciencesSocial Sciences (R0)

Publish with us

Policies and ethics