Skip to main content

Empathy’s Translations: Three Paths from Einfühlung into Anglo-American Psychology

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
Empathy

Abstract

The term “empathy” appeared as a translation of the German Einfühlung in Anglophone psychology in the first decade of the twentieth century. Contrary to popular understanding, Edward B. Titchener was not the only one to introduce the term. James Ward also offered the translation, and J. Mark Baldwin suggested “semblance” as a better translation for Einfühlung than empathy. These divergent translations demonstrate the polysemic character of the term: Ward saw empathy as a means to personify nature and objects in line with his panpsychism; Baldwin viewed semblance as part of play activity; and Titchener defined empathy as a kinaesthetic mental image projected into an object. Lanzoni explores these multiple translations of Einfühlung and reflects on their significance for contemporary notions of empathy in social neuroscience.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 59.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 79.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 109.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. 1.

    Allport’s exposure to Einfühlung theory guided him to his own holistic version of personality and social psychology.

  2. 2.

    Lipps described the forms of Einfühlung as general apperceptive Einfühlung for common objects; empirical Einfühlung of nature; mood Einfühlung, and Einfühlung with others, achieved through the sensory perception of living beings. See Lipps (1903, Chapter 14, “Die Einfühlung”, pp. 187–202). For a definition of aesthetic Einfühlung as an unmediated experience in which one’s own feelings are experienced in the aesthetic object, see Lipps (1902, p. 368). See also (Lipps, 1903/1960).

  3. 3.

    Dilthey’s 1894 outline of a verstehende psychology, or a descriptive psychology in contrast to an explanatory or causal psychology included psychological and historical methods of transferring one’s self into expressions of another’s life as a way of re-experiencing, a sich hineinversetzen (similar to Einfühlung) (see Dilthey, 1894/1977; see also Makkreel; 2000; Ringer, 1969, pp. 81–127). Thomas Friedrich and Jörg Gleiter argue that Einfühlungsästhetik, as it emerged in the work of the Vischers, found aesthetics to be a special case of everyday sensory experience – a way of perceiving the expressive and soulful content of objects. Einfühlungsästhetik also influenced architectural theories of the embodiment of space, exemplified in Heinrich Wölfflin’s Prolegomena zu einer Psychologie der Architektur (1886) and August Schmarsow’s Über den Werth der Dimensionen im menschlichen Raumgebilde (1896), both reprinted in Friedrich & Gleiter (2007).

  4. 4.

    The English “empathy” did appear a few years earlier, in 1895 – but not as a translation of Einfühlung. It was an energy concept described by E. L. Hinman in his review of a paper written by K. Lasswitz, Ueber psychophysische Energie und ihre Factoren [“On Psychophysical Energy and Its Factors”]. He writes, “For the capacity factor of psychophysical energy the name ‘empathy’ is proposed. Empathy is then a physical quantity, a physiological brain-function, and is defined as the relation of the whole energy at any change of the central organ to the intensity” (Hinman, 1895, p. 673). For details on the initial translation of Einfühlung as “enpathy” and its correction in the pages of The Philosophical Review in November 1908, see Lanzoni (2012b).

  5. 5.

    See Chapter 10: Andrea Pinotti, on the history of empathy with inanimate objects, in this volume. See also Pinotti (2010).

  6. 6.

    For biographical information on Baldwin, see Baldwin (1930) and Sokal (1997).

  7. 7.

    For more on the Baldwin effect and on Baldwin’s legacy in psychology, see Wozniak (2004, 2009a) and Richards (1987).

  8. 8.

    Volume 1 was entitled Functional Logic, or Genetic Theory of Knowledge (1906/1975a), Volume 2 Experimental Logic, or Genetic Theory of Thought (1908/1975b), Volume 3 Interest and Art Being Real Logic. I Genetic Epistemology (1911/1975c), and Volume 4 The Genetic Theory of Reality (1915/1975d). See also Wozniak (1998).

  9. 9.

    He labelled the play instinct the “lower semblant” (Baldwin, 1911/1975c, part IV, “Semblance and the Aesthetic”, p. 157).

  10. 10.

    Urban preferred the German Einfühlung: “We shall accordingly use the term to designate the entire process (projection, imitation, and ejection) involved in the activities of characterisation and participation, and shall consider it, more over, in its aspect of affective-conative process” (Urban, 1909, p. 235).

  11. 11.

    In this book he uses “semblance” and “empathy” as synonyms.

  12. 12.

    The title page of the journal Mind, for October 1908 reads: “Mind, a quarterly review of Psychology and Philosophy, edited by Professor G. F. Stout, with the co-operation of E. B. Titchener, American Editorial Representative, and of Dr. E. Caird, Professor Ward, Professor Pringle-Pattison, and other members of an advisory committee.” In this volume, “empathy” is mentioned in the “Philosophical Periodicals” section: “We attain to consciousness of the existence of beings analogous to ourselves by way of empathy, which is based mainly upon the impulse to imitation; we communicate and understand ideas by the gradual growth of the speech-function” (Mind, Oct. 1908, p. 593). It is unclear who wrote these periodical sections, as both Ward and Titchener were editorial contributors. Titchener had been paid for writing on periodicals in the journal in the 1890s, evidenced by Stout’s letter of thanks to Titchener for penning “fresh Notices of Periodicals” (G.F. Stout to Titchener, May 17, 1894. E. B. Titchener Archive, Cornell University Special Collections). Ward thanks Titchener for his visit in a letter (Ward to Titchener, November 28, 1904, E. B. Titchener Archive 14/23/545).

  13. 13.

    Baldwin to Titchener, March 10, 1910; E. B. Titchener Archive 14/23/545 Box 2.

  14. 14.

    Although Baldwin’s genetic logic was not of great influence in the United States, Wilbur Urban discussed Baldwin’s ideas at Trinity College in Hartford, Connecticut. Urban preferred his own translations of “sympathetic participation” or “affective projection”, although he did invoke “empathy” when he analysed how it was “through sympathetic projection of my own feeling, I may apprehend the inner life of others, how subjective feeling in me may become the bearer of an objective meaning and reference”. To him, feelings had a cognitive character: “feelings have as their presuppositions judgments and assumptions” (Urban, 1917, p, 281).

  15. 15.

    Titchener’s conception of the kinaesthetic image was adapted by Vernon Lee (Violet Paget) to explain aesthetic empathy of the formal-dynamic type (Lee & Anstruther-Thompson, 1912, p. 148; see Lanzoni, 2009).

  16. 16.

    Titchener alluded to aesthetic Einfühlung theory in 1899 when he spoke of aesthetic sentiments as “one’s own emotions, projected into other people or into external nature, and refound there by one’s active attention” (Titchener, 1916, p. 330). For more on Titchener’s view of empathy see Titchener (1909a, 1910, 1915).

  17. 17.

    Ward to James Frazer, Nov. 11, 1915 (Add. Ms. b. 37/331), Trinity College Library Master and Fellows of Trinity College Cambridge.

  18. 18.

    R.F. Hoernle attributes the translation to Ward in his review of A.C. Macmillan, The Crowning Phase of the Critical Philosophy; A Study in Kant’s Critique of Judgment (London: Macmillan & Co., 1912) in Mind New Series, Vol. 23, No. 92 (Oct. 1914), 597–604, p. 600. See also C. Spearman’s review of C. S. Myers, Text-book of Experimental Psychology, “New Books” in Mind New Series, vol. 18, no. 72, October 1909, 617–18; (Valentine, 1912).

  19. 19.

    He published a portion of it entitled “An Attempt to interpret Fechner’s Law” in the journal Mind in 1876 (Ward, 1927, p. 53).

  20. 20.

    Ackerman describes Robertson Smith as the first “to apply the comparative evolutionary anthropological approach to the study of an entire family of religions, the Semitic” (Ackerman, 1987, p. 58). He cites a letter Frazer wrote to Jackson in 1888, which seems to refer to Ward. Therein Frazer writes that one’s way of looking at the world is a product of a long period of cultural growth and change, an idea that a psychologist should be interested in, preferably a modern physiological psychologist (Ackerman, 1987, p. 89).

  21. 21.

    Ward to Frazer, Nov. 11, 1915 (Add. Ms. B. 37/331), Master and Fellows of Trinity College Cambridge.

  22. 22.

    On the differences that Titchener spelled out between a kinaesthetic image and sensation, see Lanzoni (2012b).

References

  • Ackerman, R. (1987). J. G. Frazer: His Life and Work. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Allesch, C. G. (1987). Geschichte der psychologischen Ästhetik [History of psychological aesthetics]. Göttingen: Verlag für Psychologie.

    Google Scholar 

  • Allport, G. (1945). Psychology of Participation. Psychological Review, 52(3),117–132.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baldwin, J. M. (Ed.) (1901–05). Dictionary of Philosophy and Psychology (vol. 2). New York: Macmillan.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baldwin, J. M. (1906/1975a). Functional Logic, or Genetic Theory of Knowledge. In Thought and Things: A Study of the Development and Meaning of Thought or Genetic Logic (vol. 1). New York: Arno Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baldwin, J. M. (1908/1975b). Experimental Logic, or Genetic Theory of Thought. In Thought and Things: A Study of the Development and Meaning of Thought or Genetic Logic (vol. 2). New York: Arno Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baldwin, J. M. (1911/1975c). Interest and Art Being Real Logic. I. Genetic Epistemology. In Thought and Things: A Study of the Development and Meaning of Thought or Genetic Logic (vol. 3). New York: Arno Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baldwin, J. M. (1913). History of Psychology: A Sketch and an Interpretation (vol. 2). New York: G.P. Putnam and Sons.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Baldwin, J. M. (1915/1975d). The Genetic Theory of Reality. In Thought and Things: A Study of the Development and Meaning of Thought or Genetic Logic (vol. 4). New York: Arno Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baldwin, J. M. (1917). Pancalism. In J. Hastings (Ed.). Encyclopedia of Religion and Ethics (vol. 9, pp. 599). New York: Charles Scribner’s Sons.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baldwin, J. M. (1930). James Mark Baldwin. In C. Murchison (Ed.). A History of Psychology in Autobiography (vol. 1, pp. 1–30). Washington, D.C.: American Psychological Association.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barasch, M. (1998). Modern Theories of Art, 2: From Impressionism to Kandinsky. New York: NY University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brock, A. (1991). Imageless Thought or Stimulus Error? The Social Construction of Private Experience. In W. R. Woodward & R. S. Cohen (Eds.). World Views and Scientific Discipline Formation (pp. 97–106). Dordrecht: Kluwer.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Cahan, E. D. (1984). The Genetic Psychologies of James Mark Baldwin and Jean Piaget. Developmental Psychology, 20(1), 128–135.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Clark, A. & Chalmers, D. (1998). The Extended Mind. Analysis, 58, 7–19.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Clarke, H. M. (1911). Conscious Attitudes. The American Journal of Psychology, 22(2), 214–249.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Danziger, K. (1990). Constructing the Subject: Historical Origins of Psychological Research. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • De Waal, F. (2009). The Age of Empathy: Nature’s Lessons For a Kinder Society. New York: Harmony Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dilthey, W. (1894/1977). Ideas Concerning a Descriptive and Analytic Psychology and The Understanding of Other Persons and Their Expressions of Life. In Descriptive Psychology and Historical Understanding (pp. 21–120, Zaner & Heiges, Trans.). The Hague: Martinus Nijhoff.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Di Pellegrino, G., Fadiga, L., Fogassi, L., Gallese, V., & Rizzolatti, G. (1992). Understanding Motor Events: A Neurophysiological Study. Experimental Brain Research, 91, 176–180.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Evans, R. B. (1990). The Scientific and Psychological Positions of E.B. Titchener. In R. Leys & R. B. Evans (Eds.). Defining American Psychology. The Correspondence between Adolf Meyer and E.B. Titchener (pp. 1–39). Baltimore: Johns Hopkins University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fadiga, L., Pavesi, L. G., & Rizzolatti, G. (1996). Motor Facilitation during Observation: A Magnetic Simulation Study. Journal of Neurophysiology, 73, 2608–2611.

    Google Scholar 

  • Frazer, J. G. (1900). The Golden Bough: The King of the Wood, The Perils of the Soul (2nd ed., vol. 1). London: Macmillan.

    Google Scholar 

  • Freedberg, D., & Gallese, V. (2007). Motion, Emotion and Empathy in Esthetic Experience. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11(5), 197–203.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Friedrich, T. & Gleiter, J. (Eds) (2007). Einfühlung und phänomenologische Reduktion: Grundlagentexte zu Architektur, Design und Kunst [Empathy and the phenomenological reduction. Basic texts on architecture, design, and fine arts]. Berlin: Lit Verlag Dr. W. Hopf.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gallese, V. (2001). The ‘Shared Manifold’ Hypothesis: From Mirror Neurons to Empathy. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 8, 33–50.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gallese, V. & Goldman, A. (1998). Mirror neurons and the simulation theory of mind reading. Trends in Cognitive Science, 2, 493–501.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gordon, K. (1934). A Device for Demonstrating Empathy. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 17(6), 892–893.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Groos, K. (1901). The Play of Man. New York: D. Appleton.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Heidbreder, E. (1933). Seven Psychologies. New York: Appleton-Century Crofts, Inc.

    Google Scholar 

  • Henle, M. (1971). Did Titchener Commit the Stimulus Error? The Problem of Meaning in Structural Psychology. Journal of the History of the Behavioral Sciences, 7(3), 277–282.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hindeland, M. J. (1971). Edward Bradford Titchener: A Pioneer in Perception. Journal of the History of the Behavioral Sciences, 7(1), 23–28.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hinman, E. L. (1895). Ueber psychophysische Energie und ihre Factoren. K. Lasswitz. The Philosophical Review, 4(6), 672–673.

    Google Scholar 

  • Holt, J. (2007, November 18). Mind of a Rock. New York Times. Retrieved from http://www.nytimes.com/2007/11/18/magazine/18wwln-lede-t.html

  • Jahoda, G. (2005). Theodor Lipps and the Shift from ‘Sympathy’ to ‘Empathy’. Journal of the History of the Behavioral Sciences, 4(2), 151–163.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jarzombek, Mark (2000). The Psychologizing of Modernity: Art, Architecture and History. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Koss, J. (2006). On the Limits of Empathy. The Art Bulletin, 88(1), 139–157.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kroker, K. (2003). The Progress of Introspection in America, 1896–1938. Studies in history and philosophy of biological and biomedical sciences, 34, 77–108.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kusch, M. (1999). Psychological Knowledge: A Social History and Philosophy. London: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Langfeld, H. S. (1920/1967). The Aesthetic Attitude. Port Washington, NY: Kennikat Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lanzoni, S. (2009). Practicing Psychology in the Art Gallery: Vernon Lee’s Aesthetics of Empathy. Journal of the History of the Behavioral Sciences, 45(4), 330–354.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lanzoni, S. (2012a). Introduction [Special Issue: Emotions and the Sciences: Varieties of Empathy in Science, Art and History]. Science in Context, 25(3), 287–300.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lanzoni, S. (2012b). Empathy In Translation: Movement and Image in the Psychological Laboratory. Science in Context, 25(3), 301–327.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lee, V., & Anstruther-Thomson, C. (1897). Beauty and Ugliness. Contemporary Review, 72, 544–69; 669–88.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lee, V., & Anstruther-Thomson, C. (1912). Beauty and Ugliness and Other Studies in Psychological Aesthetics. London: John Lane, Bodley Head.

    Google Scholar 

  • Leys, R. (2014). ‘Both of us Disgusted in my Insula’: Mirror-Neuron Theory and Emotional Empathy. In F. Biess &D. M. Gross (Eds.). Science and Emotions after 1945: A Transatlantic Perspective (pp. 67–95). Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Lipps, Th. (1902). Von der Form der Aesthetischen Apperception [The form of aesthetic apperception]. In Philosophische Abhandlungen. Gedenkschrift für Rudolf Haym (pp. 365–406). Halle: Max Niemeyer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lipps, Th. (1903). Leitfaden der Psychologie [Guidelines to psychology]. Leipzig: Wilhelm Engelmann.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lipps, Th. (1903/1923). Grundlegung der Ästhetik [The Foundation of Aesthetics] (vol. 1). Leipzig: Leopold Voss.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lipps, Th. (1903/1960). Empathy, Inner Imitation, and Sense Feelings. In M. Rader (Ed.). A Modern Book of Esthetics. New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lipps, Th. (1906/1920). Aesthetik: Psychologie des Schönen und der Kunst [Aesthetics. Psychology of the beautiful and the arts] (vol. 2). Leipzig: Leopold Voss.

    Google Scholar 

  • Makkreel, R. (2000). From Simulation to Structural Transposition: A Diltheyean Critique of Empathy and Defense of Verstehen. In H. H. Kögler (Ed.). Empathy and Agency: The Problem of Understanding in the Human Sciences (pp. 181–93). Boulder, CO: Westview Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mallgrave, H. F. & Ikonomou, E. (1994). Introduction. In H. F. Mallgrave & E. Ikonomou (Eds.). Empathy, Form and Space: Problems in German Aesthetics, 1873–1893 (pp. 1–66). Santa Monica, CA: Getty Center.

    Google Scholar 

  • Myers, C.S. (1909). Text-Book of Experimental Psychology. London: Edward Arnold.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Ogden, R. (1909). Reports and Discussion, Sixth International Congress of Psychology. Psychological Bulletin, 6(11), 389–397.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ogden, R. (1938). The Psychology of Art. New York: Charles Scribner’s Sons.

    Google Scholar 

  • Okabe, T. (1910). Experimental Study of Belief. The American Journal of Psychology, 21(4), 563–596.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Perky, C. (1910). An Experimental Study of Imagination. The American Journal of Psychology, 21(3), 422–452.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pinotti, A. (2010).Empathy.In H. R. Sepp & L. Embree (Eds.).Handbook of Phenomenological Aesthetics. Dordrecht: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Richards, R. J. (1987). James Mark Baldwin: Evolutionary Biopsychology and the Politics of Scientific Ideas. In R. J. Richards. Darwin and the Emergence of Evolutionary Theories of Mind and Behavior (pp. 480–496), Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ringer, F. (1969). The Decline of the German Mandarins. Hanover: Wesleyan University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rizzolatti, G., Fadiga, L., Gallese, V., & Fogassi, L (1996). Premotor cortex and the recognition of motor actions. Cognitive Brain Research, 3(2), 131–141.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rogers, A. (1917). An Analytic Study of Visual Perceptions. American Journal of Psychology, 28(4), 519–577.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schaub, A. D. V. (1911). On the Intensity of Images. American Journal of Psychology, 22, 346–368.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Skrbina, D. (2005). Panpsychism in the West. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sokal, M. M. (1997). Baldwin, Cattell and the Psychological Review: a collaboration and its discontents. History of the Human Sciences, 10(1), 57–89.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sorley, W. R. (1925). James Ward. Mind, New Series, 34(135), 273–279.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Starr, G. G. (2013). Feeling Beauty: The Neuroscience of Aesthetic Experience. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Tallon, H. J. (1939). The Concept of Self in British and American Idealism: A Dissertation. Washington, D.C.: The Catholic University of America Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Titchener, E. B. (1904). Organic Images. The Journal of Philosophy, Psychology and Scientific Methods, 1(2), 36–40.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Titchener, E.B. (1908). Lectures on the Elementary Psychology of Feeling and Attention. New York: Macmillan.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Titchener, E. B. (1909a). Experimental Psychology of the Thought-Processes. New York: Macmillan.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Titchener, E.B. (1909b). A Text-book of Psychology. Part 1. New York: Macmillan.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Titchener, E.B. (1910). A Text-Book of Psychology. New York: Macmillan.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Titchener, E.B. (1915). A Beginner’s Psychology. New York: Macmillan.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Titchener, E.B. (1899/1916). An Outline of Psychology (new Edition with Additions). New York: Macmillan.

    Google Scholar 

  • Turner, F. M. (1974). Between Science and Religion: The Reaction to Naturalism in Late Victorian England. New Haven: Yale University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Urban, W. M. (1909). Valuation: Its Nature and Laws, Being an Introduction to a General Theory of Value. London: Sonnenschein & Co.

    Google Scholar 

  • Urban, W. M. (1917). The Knowledge of Other Minds and the Problem of Meaning and Value. The Philosophical Review, 26(3), 274–296.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Valentine, C. W. (1912). Psychological Theories of the Horizontal-Vertical Illusion. The British Journal of Psychology, 5(1), 8–35.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vischer, R. (1873/1994). On the Optical Sense of Form. In H. F. Mallgrave & E. Ikonomou (Eds). Empathy, Form and Space: Problems in German Aesthetics, 1873–1893 (pp. 89–123). Santa Monica, CA: Getty Center.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ward, J. (1886). Psychology. In Encyclopedia Britannica: A Dictionary of Arts, Sciences and General Literature (9th ed., vol. 20, pp. 37–85).

    Google Scholar 

  • Ward, J. (1906). The Present Problems of General Psychology. In H. J. Rogers (Ed.). International Congress of Arts and Sciences (vol. 5). London, New York: University Alliance.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ward, J. (1911). The Realm of Ends or Pluralism and Theism: The Gifford Lectures, St. Andrews, 1907–1911. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ward, J. (1919). Psychological Principles. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ward, J. (1927). Essays in Philosophy with a Memoir of the Author by Olwen Ward Campbell. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wispé, L. (1987). History of the concept of empathy. In N. Eisenberg & J. Strayer (Eds.). Empathy and its D evelopment (pp. 17–37). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wozniak, R. H. (1998). Thought and Things: James Mark Baldwin and the Biosocial Origins of Mind. In R. W. Rieber & K. D. Salzinger (Eds). Psychology: Theoretical and Historical Perspectives (2nd ed., pp. 429–453). Washington, D.C.: American Psychological Association.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Wozniak, R. H. (2004). Lost Classics and Forgotten Contributors: James Mark Baldwin as a Case Study in the Disappearance and Rediscovery of Ideas. In T. C. Dalton & R. B. Evans (Eds.). The Life Cycle of Psychological Ideas (pp. 33–58). New York: Kluwer Academic/Plenum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wozniak, R. H. (2009a). Consciousness, Social Heredity and Development. American Psychologist, 64(2), 93–101.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wozniak, R. H. (2009b). James Mark Baldwin, professional disaster, and the European connection. Rassegna di Psicologia, 26, 111–128.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wozniak, R. H. & Santiago-Blay, J. A. (2013). Trouble at Tyson Alley: James Mark Baldwin’s Arrest in a Baltimore Bordello. History of Psychology, 16(2), 227–248.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

I would like to thank the Master and Fellows of Trinity College Cambridge for permission to reproduce the image of James Ward’s letter of 1915, and the archivist Adam Green for his assistance. I would also like to thank members of my writing group, IWSS, the anonymous reviewers of my manuscript, as well as the editors of this volume for helpful feedback.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Susan Lanzoni .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Copyright information

© 2017 The Author(s)

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Lanzoni, S. (2017). Empathy’s Translations: Three Paths from Einfühlung into Anglo-American Psychology. In: Lux, V., Weigel, S. (eds) Empathy. Palgrave Studies in the Theory and History of Psychology. Palgrave Macmillan, London. https://doi.org/10.1057/978-1-137-51299-4_12

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics