Introduction

Postural control in sitting or standing is essential for performing the functional activities that underpin all activities of daily living. Standing has been traditionally considered an unconscious, automatic task, relying on feedback control from somatosensory and visual input, while attentional requirements are minimal.1 However, evidence from dual-task paradigms, where postural and suprapostural (secondary) activities are performed concurrently suggest that there are significant attentional requirements in postural control.2, 3

Dual task studies have revealed that increased dependency on attentional processes for postural control seems more apparent with changes in the central nervous system, such as with aging and disease2, 4, 5, 6, 7, 8 in people with spinal cord injury (SCI), where information to and from the brain has been disrupted, weakness, spasticity, sensory deficits and/or pain can impact balance.9 Indeed, unsteadiness during standing is exacerbated in conditions where somatosensory or visual challenges are presented.9, 10 Postural control is thought to become more attentionally demanding as the postural environment is made more challenging and when a secondary task is required.11 Under these dual-task conditions, there may be more interference or competition between processes involved in cognition and postural control, leading to a disruption of the automaticity of movements (as in quiet standing) to a more conscious form of movement control.12 Such a shift from an automatic, unconscious level of control to one relying more on conscious control is termed reinvestment12 and may have detrimental effects on function.13

Populations with decreased balance ability due to deficits in either their somatosensory, visual or vestibular systems compensate by allocating greater cognitive resources to remaining systems to maintain postural stability.5 Such reallocation of attentional resources to an apparently simple task, such as quiet standing, that forms the basis of many daily tasks has implications for understanding functional outcomes following an SCI. Thus, the overall objective of this study was to explore the effects of attentional requirements of postural control in people with SCI by comparing standing time (STime), movement reinvestment, postural sway and math-task performance during standing between SCI and able-bodied (AB) controls in a dual-task paradigm. We chose a mathematical task which has been shown to effectively degrade postural stability in healthy older and young adults.11, 14, 15, 16

Materials and methods

Participants

Adult subjects with motor-incomplete SCI and age- and sex-matched AB controls were recruited. Inclusion criteria were: being medically stable and able to stand for at least 30 s unassisted without the use of hand-held aids, though the use of orthoses was permitted. Exclusion criteria were: presence of cognitive or visual impairments, inability to follow instructions, orthopedic or neurological problems (other than SCI) or cardiovascular dysregulation (for example, orthostatic hypotension) that could interfere with postural control. All procedures were approved by the university research ethics board and all participants provided written informed consent before any experimental procedures. We certify that all applicable institutional and governmental regulations concerning the ethical use of human volunteers were followed during the course of this research.

Postural conditions

Three 2-min trials of two postural conditions (sitting and standing) were performed. For the sitting condition, subjects sat supported in a chair, eyes focused on a target 2 m in front with hands in their lap. For the standing condition, subjects were asked to stand barefoot (unless wearing orthoses, where shoes were permitted) between parallel bars in their preferred position on two Bertec force platforms (Bertec FP 4550, Bertec Corporation, Columbus, OH, USA), looking straight ahead at a target 2 m away and with their arms by their sides. Analog signals from the force plates were streamed to a data acquisition system at 100 Hz using custom-written software in Labview 8.6 (National Instruments, Austin, TX, USA). Subjects’ performance was videotaped for offline analysis to measure the STime. For all standing trials, the subject was asked to focus on balance. To ensure consistent foot placement between trials, outlines of the feet were traced on the force platform. Commencement of data collection occurred ~10–20 s after achieving standing balance in order to account for transient effects seen in the first 20 s of quiet stance.17

Suprapostural task and vision conditions

Single-task and dual-task performance were assessed under two vision conditions: eyes open and eyes closed (Table 1). For the dual-task conditions, subjects were asked to count out loud and backwards by 3 s starting from a random number provided by the researcher. Data were collected until the subject opened their eyes (in the eyes closed condition) or until their hands left their sides or touched the parallel bars. Rest intervals of 10 min were provided between trials to decrease the effects of fatigue while providing the time to complete the perceptual and functional measures. The order of the trials was randomized.

Table 1 Experimental conditions

Outcome measures

Our primary measures were STime, math-task performance (error ratio (ER) and total words uttered), movement reinvestment and postural control. Our secondary measures were subjective mental workload, fear and confidence.

STime was defined by the start of the trial until the subject was seen to lose their balance or open their eyes for both single- and dual-task conditions.

ERs were calculated as the ratio of the number of incorrect responses to the total number of words uttered (TOT) including any corrections made.16 Average ER and TOT were calculated for each subject and then averaged across all participants. Note that for the standing eyes closed condition, not all the SCI participants were able to complete the allotted 2-min counting time. Therefore, for this condition, the first 60 s of counting were used for the analysis.

Signals from the force plates were processed offline using a dual-pass Butterworth filter with a low pass cut off frequency of 5 Hz to smooth the data and calculate center of pressure (CoP) during each trial using custom-written routines in Matlab (Mathworks, Natick, MA, USA). Root mean squared and mean velocity of the CoP in the mediolateral and anteroposterior directions were calculated. No CoP measures were analyzed if the trial lasted less than 60 s since important low-frequency components of the signal may be missed.17, 18

The tendency to switch from automatic to conscious control of movement was measured by asking subjects to complete the Movement Specific Reinvestment Scale (MSRS)12 after each single-task standing trial during eyes open and eyes closed conditions. The MSRS consists of 10 items that relate to concerns about movement self-consciousness, and conscious motor processing. We modified the questionnaire to reflect standing balance state-specific changes by substituting the words ‘movement’ with ‘standing’. Increased reinvestment corresponds to a higher score out of a total possible score of 36.

Subjective mental workload was assessed after each trial using the online National Aeronautics and Space Administration Task Load Index (NASA-TLX).19 An overall workload score was calculated based on a weighted average of six subscales. Scores range from 0 (least) to 100 (highest amount of workload).19

Although postural control is normally attributed to physiological factors, psychological perceptual factors such as fear of falling and balance self-confidence have also been attributed to postural control changes.20, 21 Balance self-confidence was assessed prior to each standing trial, while fear was assessed after each trial on a scale of 0–100% (0 least confident/fearful, 100 most confident/fearful).21

To assess the effects of the postural and suprapostural conditions during dual tasking, a relative measure of change, the dual-task cost (DTC),22, 23 was calculated for all primary measures (except MSRS):

For our outcome measures, with the exception of TOT, a positive value represents a decrease in performance under the dual-task condition, whereas a negative value corresponds to an improvement in performance in the dual-task condition.

Statistical analysis

All data were analyzed using SPSS (v.21) statistical software (SPSS Inc, Chicago, IL, USA). The critical value for significance for all statistical tests was set at an alpha value of 0.05. Reported values are mean±s.d. The mean difference between groups was calculated with 95% confidence interval (CI). Subject characteristics were summarized using descriptive statistics, and differences between groups were assessed using the independent t-test.

Primary outcomes

A two-way repeated measures analysis of variance (group: SCI, AB × task: single-task, dual-task) was performed on the primary measures: STime, ER and TOT. Normality of distribution was checked using the Shapiro–Wilk test. When the sphericity assumption was violated (Mauchly’s Test), a Greenhouse–Geisser correction was used. Differences in standing performance between the two groups for MSRS was assessed using independent t-tests. Levene’s test was used to assess equality of variances. A two-way repeated measures multivariate analysis of variance was performed on the CoP measures. A log transform was performed on the data that were not normal. We used the data for a 120 s bout for eyes open since all SCI subjects could stand for the maximum amount of time, and 60 s for eyes closed.

Secondary outcomes

Differences in standing performance between the two groups for fear, confidence, and NASA-TLX was assessed using independent t-tests.

Results

Subject characteristics

Seventeen subjects (nine SCI and eight AB controls) participated in the study. Individual characteristics of the SCI subjects are presented in Table 2. Parametric tests revealed no statistically significant difference between the two groups (mean±s.d.) for age (SCI: 51.8±11.3; AB: 49±11.3 years), height (SCI: 174.3±8.9; AB 177.6±10.4 cm) or weight (SCI: 78.1±9.0; AB: 78.5±13.6 kg), P>0.05 for all comparisons.

Table 2 Subject characteristics and clinical outcomes of individual SCI subjects (n=9)

Primary outcomes

Standing time

All SCI subjects were able to stand for the maximum trial duration of 2 min during the eyes open condition for both single- and dual-task conditions. However, for total STime with eyes closed, significant interactions between task and group were observed (F1,15=5.4, P=0.034, partial η2 =0.266). On average, SCI subjects stood for 26% shorter duration in the dual-task with respect to the single-task condition compared to AB controls (Figure 1), with two of the SCI subjects unable to stand at all with eyes closed. The mean difference (95% CI) between SCI and AB controls for the DTC of STime was −26.15% (−45.54 to −6.76%).

Figure 1
figure 1

Standing time eyes closed between SCI and AB groups. Comparison between SCI and AB groups for the single task of standing and the dual task of standing and counting with eyes closed. Total standing duration was shorter among the SCI subjects in the dual-task condition compared to the AB controls (P<0.05). Error bars represent 95% confidence interval.

Math task

Four of the nine SCI subjects could not complete the dual-task condition of standing and counting with eyes closed for the allotted minimum 60 s of STime, therefore their data could not be included in eyes closed math-task performance analysis. There were no statistically significant main or interaction effects in math-task performance (ER or TOT) between the SCI and AB group, and between single- and dual-task conditions for either eyes open or eyes closed. The mean difference (95% CI) for eyes open ER was 5.49% (−8.98 to 19.96%), and TOT was −15.41% (−98.76 to 67.94). The mean difference for eyes closed ER was 7.76% (−11.94 to 27.46%) and TOT was −72.57 (−322.49 to 177.35%). (Figure 2).

Figure 2
figure 2

Math task performance between SCI and AB groups. Comparison between math-task performance (error ratio (left) and total words uttered (right)) during the single task of count and sit (white bars) and the dual task of count and stand (gray bars) for (a) eyes open, counting for 120 s and (b) eyes closed, counting for 60 s. The SCI group performed equally as well as the AB group in all conditions (P>0.05). Error bars represent 95% confidence interval.

Movement reinvestment

SCI subjects reinvested significantly more conscious attention to postural control compared to AB subjects when standing quietly in both vision conditions (eyes open: t15 =7.33, P<0.001); (eyes closed: t13=6.86, P<0.001) (Figure 3).

Figure 3
figure 3

Movement reinvestment between SCI and AB groups. Comparison between SCI and AB groups for the amount of conscious control of standing posture (single task) during eyes open and eyes closed conditions. SCI subjects reinvested more conscious control to posture than AB subjects in both vision conditions (P<0.05). Higher MSRS scores indicate higher reinvestment. Error bars represent 95% confidence interval.

Center of pressure

Figure 4 illustrates representative single-subject data of CoP displacement plots during single-task (standing) and dual-task (standing and counting) from an SCI and AB participant. For the CoP measures, we observed a statistically significant multivariate main effect of group in both vision conditions (eyes open: Wilks’ λ=0.24, F4,12=9.4, P=0.001, partial η2=0.758; eyes closed: Wilks’ λ=0.17, F4,9=11.4, P=0.001, partial η2=0.835), and task for eyes closed (Wilks’ λ=0.34, F4,9=4.3, P=0.032, partial η2=0.658). Significant univariate main effects for group and task were obtained for all CoP measures for eyes open. For eyes closed, significant univariate main effects for group and task for Root mean squared-mediolateral were observed. Generally, SCI subjects’ mean velocity and sway area in anteroposterior and mediolateral directions were higher than those of controls in both vision conditions (Table 3).

Figure 4
figure 4

CoP displacement for an SCI and AB subject. Representative CoP path plots for the single task of standing (left) and the dual task of standing and counting (right) from (a) SCI subject 5 and (b) an age-matched AB subject.

Table 3 Center of pressure postural performance eyes open and closed

Subjective mental workload

The subjective mental workload (NASA-TLX) of the SCI subjects during standing was significantly higher compared to AB controls (eyes open: t10 =3.56, P=0.005; CI 95% (11.27, 47.42); (eyes closed: t13 =3.67, P=0.003; CI 95% (12.95, 50.18). The mean difference (95% CI) was −29.34% (−47.57 to −11.11%) for eyes open and −31.53% (−49.33 to −13.73%) for eyes closed.

Fear and confidence

We observed statistically significant differences between groups for fear (t6 =2.98, P=0.024, CI 95% (6.20, 60.87)) and confidence (t6 =−4.58, P=0.003, CI 95% (−65.67,−20.20)) in the eyes closed condition only. SCI subjects were 34% more fearful (mean difference −33.54% (−56.03 to −11.05%) and 43% less confident (mean difference 42.93% (24.18–61.68%) than AB controls during quiet standing with their eyes closed.

Discussion

We found a significant DTC in total STime with eyes closed. We did not observe a significant dual-task effect on math-task performance, but we did find that SCI subjects had larger sway velocities and amplitudes and reinvested significantly more conscious control to posture than AB controls. These results appear to correspond to the higher ratings for fear, lower confidence and higher subjective workload in people with SCI during standing. However, even though we attempted to limit confounding variables by matching the groups, our small sample size limits us from confirming that the observed differences can be completely attributed to the SCI itself. Nevertheless, this data provide some insights into the attentional requirements of standing in this population and some considerations for understanding the recovery of postural control following SCI.

SCI subjects require greater attention to postural control than AB subjects in quiet standing

Standing balance in people with SCI may be compromised due to various levels of sensory loss, muscle weakness and spasticity,24 which could lead to greater movement reinvestment and postural sway. Our results are consistent with a study that compared the mean velocity and amplitude between SCI and AB controls24 and a study that compared the amount of conscious reinvestment of stroke subjects with matched controls.25, Increased conscious control to posture could be viewed as an adaptive strategy by people with reduced postural control in order to maintain quiet standing.

Perceptual measures may influence the amount of resources devoted to processing tasks with regards to postural control. Psychological factors such as increased fear and decreased confidence have been reported to be associated with increased reinvestment.21 Our SCI subjects found standing more effortful and they were significantly more fearful and less confident than our AB controls, which could explain why we observed significant group effects between SCI and AB subjects for movement reinvestment and sway for both vision conditions.

Dual task effect for standing time only

We detected a dual-task effect for STime but not for the math task or postural sway and this could be explained in a number of ways. First, our SCI cohort was relatively high functioning. All could stand for at least 2 min with their eyes open and 7/9 participants could stand for at least 60 s with eyes closed. In addition, the median Berg Balance Scale score for our SCI group was 43/56, indicating that they would be considered as low falls risk.26

Second, the math task might not have been difficult enough. However, there have been many studies that have reported a dual-task effect when combining a backwards counting task with quiet standing.14, 16 It is possible that our lack of significant DT effects on math performance and CoP parameters may have been hampered by our attempt to normalize the timeframe by only selecting those subjects who could stand for at least 60 s, thereby inadvertently limiting our SCI group to those who were high functioning. This is underscored by the fact that our STime results, which included the two subjects who could not stand at all with their eyes closed, revealed a significant DTC between our SCI and AB groups. Therefore, our protocol revealed that the math task may not have been challenging enough for subjects who could stand for at least 60 s eyes closed, yet was too difficult for those who could not stand for the minimum 60 s.

A key limitation in this study was the small sample size and use of multiple outcomes and comparisons. Moreover, with the exception of STime with eyes closed, the differences in DTC between groups of our primary performance measures were small, with large amounts of variability, consistent with the idea that our postural and secondary tasks may not have been challenging enough. Consideration must also be made when involving verbal responses, since depending on the level of the lesion, SCI can lead to impairment of the respiratory system27 and changes in respiration and speech have been shown to have an effect on postural control.28 Our SCI group was living and working in the community and physically active, and so generalizability of our findings may be limited to only the higher-functioning strata of the SCI population.

Clinical message

Adding a suprapostural task to balance training may help reveal the attentional resources devoted to postural control in people with SCI, but the dual task must be sufficiently difficult to challenge balance especially in high-functioning people with SCI.

Conclusions

Our study has demonstrated that total STime during eyes closed is adversely affected with the addition of a math task. Threat-related factors, such as increased fear, decreased confidence and increased perceived mental workload were observed in parallel with increases in postural sway and conscious control of standing in subjects with SCI. However, it appears that for people with SCI with good balance function, standing on a firm surface with eyes open and performing a concurrent suprapostural task of minimal mental workload would not greatly affect their postural control any more than the single task of standing quietly.