Introduction

What makes “spot the difference” puzzles so challenging and why are some people better at these puzzles than others? The deceptively simple task of identifying the differences between two similar visual scenes highlights the complexity of human visual cognition1.

Actively discriminating between two similar images engages a cascade of steps from low-level processing of stimulus features to high-level object recognition. At the perceptual level, exposure to an object may generate expectations of similar, contextually-related objects2,3. For example, consider a scenario in which a person is asked to visually inspect two slightly different images, image A and image B, and decide whether they match or not. The more subtle the differences between the two images, the more uncertain the brain may be about the “true” state of the environment. After the initial visual processing,3 the overall “discriminability” of features in image A may lead to an expectation violation in image B, thus facilitating a perceptual decision4,5,6. However, what if image B is identical to image A? In this scenario, the lack of discernable differences may contribute to internal noise during perceptual inference leading to conflict or uncertainty during the decision process7,8,9. While performance on such tasks may boil down to inter-individual differences across various factors such as motivation, working memory, fluid intelligence, and visual attention10,11,12, it may also be modulated by differences in perceptual inference and decision-making as seen in autism spectrum conditions13,14,15,16.

In this article, we use the preferred identity-first language to describe people on the autism spectrum17. Autism spectrum conditions (henceforth autism) are a set of neurodevelopmental conditions characterized by difficulties in communication and relationships, alongside unusually narrow interests, repetitive, restricted patterns of behaviour, and sensory-perceptual differences18. Visual cognition is a prominent area of interest in autism research. Autistic people have been described as not “seeing the wood for the trees” due to their more “veridical” perception19,20,21,22. For example, autistic individuals have been found to consistently outperform typical participants in identifying hidden figures in complex scenes and in classic visual search paradigms23,24. However, it is important to note that autistic individuals have been found to be faster, but not necessarily more accurate in these tasks23,25,26,27,28. It is unclear how autistic participants make two-alternative perceptual decisions in such target detection tasks, particularly in trials where there is no target or “signal” present.

Optimal performance on a perceptual decision task requires filtering out of external noise and a reduction in internal noise29,30,31,32,33. While external noise encompasses environmental factors, such as task-relevant or task-irrelevant distractors, internal noise refers to variability in neuronal signals or random neuronal fluctuations that pose a challenge during perceptual inference and decision-making34. Neural models of autism suggest that, due to a possible imbalance of excitatory and inhibitory neurotransmitters, variable levels of internal noise may contribute to the cognitive features characteristic of the condition35,36,37,38,39.

In this investigation, we aimed to expand upon previous findings of figure disembedding in autism by investigating how autistic and typical individuals make perceptual decisions about two similar or differing figures.

Methods

Participants

Participants with normal or corrected-to-normal vision were recruited online via an email notification sent to individuals registered to two University of Cambridge databases: (1) the Autism Research Centre database (accessible at www.autismresearchcentre.com) was used to recruit autistic adults and (2) a second database (accessible at www.cambridgepsychology.com) was used to recruit the non-autistic adult controls. The first database collects information on formal autism diagnoses by asking participants to choose their diagnoses from a drop-down menu. This is followed up by questions about the year of diagnosis, the professional who diagnosed them, and the facility where they were diagnosed. Participants were entered into a prize draw for the chance to win £50. After excluding participants with missing/incomplete data, the dataset contained 140 autistic (82 females) and 147 non-autistic (118 females) adults aged 18–60 years. There were no significance group differences in age (t(283) = −0.55, p = 0.579) for autism (Mean = 35.1, SD = 9.85) and controls (Mean = 35.8, SD = 9.85).

Procedure

This study was approved by and conducted in accordance with the regulations of the Psychology Research Ethics Committee in Cambridge (PREC. 2015.018). Written informed consent was obtained from all participants. Participants completed behavioural tasks probing working memory and visual perception via Cambridge Brain Sciences (www.cambridgebrainsciences.com), a web-based platform for cognitive assessments. Participants were instructed to complete the tasks on a desktop computer while seated comfortably and with a clear view of the screen. Verbal and visuospatial working memory were assessed using the standardised Digit Span test, which measures the ability to recall a sequence of digits, and the Monkey Ladder test, which measures the ability to recall the location of digits40,41. Stimuli were scaled to size to account for differences in browsers, devices, and screen size. All tasks were adapted for online computerized testing and validated in large samples42.

For each task, participants were given 90 s to complete as many trials as possible, with a timer and the score displayed on one side of the screen. The stimulus presentation was pseudo-randomized such that equal numbers of “match” and “mismatch” trials were administered over 90 s. The difficulty level of each trial increased or decreased based on the participant’s performance on the previous trial. More specifically, there were two trials at each level of difficulty. If the participant got both trials right, the difficulty level increased by one and if they got both wrong, it reduced by one. The following visual scene discrimination tasks were implemented:

Task 1: Interlocking polygons

The Interlocking Polygons task is based on pen-and-paper tasks used in clinical neuropsychological tests43. In this task, a pair of interlocked polygons is displayed on one side of the screen. Participants were instructed to indicate whether a polygon displayed on the other side of the screen is identical (“match”) or not identical (“mismatch”) to one of the interlocking polygons (Fig. 1A). Difficulty on each trial corresponded to more subtle differences in the polygons.

Figure 1
figure 1

Overview of Cambridge Brain Sciences visual perception tasks. (A) Task 1: Interlocking polygons. (B) Task 2: Feature match. Participants were instructed to indicate whether a scene displayed on the other side of the screen is identical (“match”) or not identical (“mismatch”) to the other scene. Participants were given 90 s to complete as many trials as possible, with a timer and the score displayed on one side of the screen.

Task 2: Feature match

The Feature Match task is a visual search task based on the feature integration theory of visual attention5. Arrays of abstract shapes were displayed on either side of the screen. Participants were instructed to indicate whether the arrays’ contents were identical (“match”) or differed by a single shape (“mismatch”) (Fig. 1B). Difficulty on each trial corresponded to an increase in the number of shapes in the array.

Data analysis

Data were analysed in R version 4.0.3 (R Core Team, 2020) and RStudio (RStudio Team, 2020) with the help of the “tidyverse” package44. For Bayesian statistics, we used the “Bayes Factor” R package and report Bayes factors (BF) which quantify the strength of evidence for the alternative hypothesis (BF10) over the null (BF01)45,46,47. The magnitude of this strength increases with deviation from 1, with BF10 > 3 considered as moderate evidence and BF10 > 10 as strong evidence for the alternative hypothesis, while BF10 < 3 is insufficient evidence for or against the alternative hypothesis48,49,50. For t-tests, we report t-statistics, p-values, 95% confidence interval (CI) values, and effect sizes in addition to the Bayes factors. The R package “psycho” was used for the signal detection theory analyses51.

To help address the heterogeneity within our online sample, we first excluded participants whose working memory scores were less than 2 standard deviations from the overall mean. We then conducted exploratory t-tests to measure the extent to which the Autism and Control groups differed in working memory abilities.

As accuracy rates do not adequately capture the participants’ decision criteria, we employed a Signal Detection Theory (SDT) approach to examine the response biases/strategies used by the groups52,53. In this approach, we considered the “mismatch” trials as the signal and the “match” trials as noise.

We calculated the sensitivity/discriminability index (d′) of signal from noise using the following formula:

$${d}^{{\prime}}=Z\left(hit \,rate\right)-Z (false \,alarm \,rate)$$
(1)

The response criterion (C) which measures participant bias in choosing one response was calculated using the following:

$$C=\frac{-\left[Z\left(hit\, rate\right)+Z\left(false \,alarm\, rate\right)\right]}{2}$$
(2)

where hits and false alarms are expressed as the proportion of responses in each category, and Z(.) is the inverse of the cumulative distribution function of the given Gaussian distribution. We then assessed group differences in sensitivity indices (d´) and response criteria (C) by means of t-tests. Additional analyses of group differences in accuracy rates on each condition are reported in the Supplementary Material.

Results

Working memory

After excluding participants whose working memory performance was below the cut-off, 276 participants remained: 129 Autism (75 female, 54 male) and 147 Control (118 female, 29 male). The exploratory t-test on verbal working memory as assessed by the Digit Span test showed evidence in favour of group differences (BF10 = 27, t(273) = 3.40, p < 0.001, d = 0.40, 95% CI [0.14, 0.56]) between the Autism (Mean = 5.44, SD = 0.82) and Control (Mean = 5.84, SD = 0.91) groups. Meanwhile, between-group results for the visuospatial working memory test yielded a BF10 smaller than 1 (BF10 = 0.69), with evidence leaning towards a lack of group differences (t(273) = 1.87, p = 0.06, d = 0.22, 95% CI [−0.006, 0.27]) between the Autism (Mean = 5.07, SD = 0.57) and Control (Mean = 5.21, SD = 0.62) groups. The distribution of working memory scores can be seen in Supplementary Figs. 1 & 2.

Task 1: Interlocking polygons

The independent samples t-test on the total number of trials attempted by each group yielded BF10 = 1.44, suggesting no evidence in favour of group differences (t(540) = 2.36, p = 0.018, d = 0.20, 95% CI [0.19, 2.1]) between the Autism (Mean = 26.65, SD = 3.63) and Control (Mean = 25.51, SD = 3.60) groups. The mean number of trials completed by both groups in each condition are reported in Supplementary Table 1. The SDT analyses showed no evidence of group differences in the sensitivity index (d′) (BF10 = 0.16, t(232) = −0.68, p = 0.49, d = 0.08, 95% CI [−0.20, 0.09]) between Autism (Mean = 1.52, SD = 0.55) and Control (Mean = 1.4, SD = 0.61) (Fig. 2A). At the same time, we found moderate evidence of group differences in the decision criterion (C) (BF10 = 1.55, t(265) = 2.38, p = 0.02, d = 0.27, 95% CI [0.22, 0.30]) used by Autism (Mean = −0.14, SD = 0.55) and Control (Mean = 0.02, SD = 0.61) groups (Fig. 2B). This suggests a response bias by the Autism group in choosing the “mismatch” option when uncertain, thus leading to more false alarms on the “match” trials (Fig. 2B).

Figure 2
figure 2

Task 1: Interlocking polygons. (A) Signal detection sensitivity index for autism and control groups. (B) Signal detection decision criterion values for autism and control groups. Autism group is displayed in orange and control in purple. Dots indicate individual participant results. Error bars show the standard error of the mean.

Task 2: Feature match

The independent samples t-test on the total number of trials attempted by each group yielded BF10 < 1 (BF10 = 0.09) suggesting no evidence of group differences (t(539) = 0.22, p = 0.82, d = 0.02, 95% CI [−0.53, 0.67]) between the Autism (Mean = 25.5, SD = 3.63) and Control (Mean = 25.4, SD = 3.60) groups (Supplementary Table 1). The SDT analyses showed no evidence of group differences in the sensitivity index (d′) (BF10 = 0.45, t(258) = 1.60, p = 0.10, d = 0.19, 95% CI [−0.02, 0.22]) between Autism (Mean = 2.69, SD = 0.50) and Control (Mean = 2.79, SD = 0.49) (Fig. 3A). At the same time, we found substantial evidence of group differences in the decision criterion (C) (BF10 = 8.01, t(258) = 2.94, p = 0.003, d = 0.36, 95% CI [0.04, 0.22]) used by Autism (Mean = −0.10, SD = 0.37) and Control (Mean = 0.03, SD = 0.36) groups (Fig. 3B). This suggests more false alarms by the autism group during “match” trials.

Figure 3
figure 3

Task 2: Feature match. (A) Signal detection sensitivity index for autism and control groups. (B) Signal detection decision criterion values for autism and control groups. Autism group displayed in orange and control in purple. Dots indicate individual participant results. Error bars show the standard error of the mean.

Discussion

Using a large sample of autistic and typical participants, we conducted two visual cognition tasks to test figure discrimination ability and perceptual decision-making. In Task 1: Interlocking Polygons, participants indicated whether a target polygon was present in the comparison figure of interlocking polygons. In Task 2: Feature Match, participants indicated whether two arrays of shapes differed by a single item. Investigations using Signal Detection Theory indicated no group differences in visual perceptual sensitivity. At the same time, we found group differences in the decision criterion used by the groups. Specifically, autistic participants on average tended to choose the “mismatch” option when faced with uncertainty during “match” trials.

We found no group differences in the sensitivity index (d′), suggesting no clear differences in visual perception between the groups (Figs. 2A, 3A). This contradicts our initial hypotheses of differential visual perception in autism. At the same time, we found a notable response bias by the autism group while making decisions. More specifically, autistic participants were more likely to choose the “mismatch” option, thus leading to more hits on the “mismatch” condition, but also more false alarms on the “match” condition (Figs. 2B, 3B). To measure the extent of this response bias, additional one-sample t-tests on the response criterion (C) were computed separately for each group, which indicated that the autism group tended to choose “mismatch” more often than the ideal observer (Supplementary Material). It has been hypothesized that the perceptual features of autism may be due to variable levels of internal noise, thus leading to difficulties in signal-to-noise separation39,54,55,56,57,58,59. Internal noise in autism has been attributed to atypical neural connectivity of sensory brain networks35,37,60,61 and an imbalance in excitatory and inhibitory neurotransmitters38,62,63,64. However, in this study, we found no differences between autistic and control groups in their discriminability indices in both tasks (Figs. 2A, 3A). While our findings indicate group differences in the decision criteria used by the groups (Figs. 2B, 3B), it is unclear why autistic individuals use a lower decision criterion while choosing between signal and noise responses. Future research using computational models and neuroimaging methods may shed more light on these findings.

Our study has its limitations: the less-controlled nature of the online task set-up, the sampling bias of participants with access to computers and internet, and the unbalanced sex ratio within our study sample. A greater percentage of female participants reflects what is the norm with online research65. However, due to possible sex differences in autism and visual cognition, we acknowledge this as an important caveat. Our findings may be more generalizable to females, however it is ultimately unclear whether the same effects would be observed in a sex-balanced or male-only sample. Future research taking these caveats into account may answer more questions about perceptual inference and decision-making in autism.

In conclusion, contrary to findings from previous research, when compared to typical people, autistic individuals show no differences in visual perceptual sensitivity on two variations of figure disembedding tasks. On the other hand, autistic individuals show a response bias when faced with uncertainty during these tasks, suggesting differences in perceptual decision-making. Taken together, our findings shed light on how autistic individuals make perceptual choices on similarity judgement tasks and provide clear directions for future research.