Introduction

In developing countries, indoor air pollution (IAP) is one of the leading causes of several diseases and premature death1. As the World Health Organization (WHO) reports, there are 3.8 million premature deaths annually in low- and middle-income countries that may be attributed to IAP from cooking fires2. Even so, approximately 2.7 billion individuals who lacked access to clean cooking facilities used solid fuel (coal, firewood, animals dung) to meet the most basic energy needs in developing countries, mainly referring to cooking in 20183. In these households, solid fuels are usually burned in inefficient or poorly ventilated combustion devices (traditional stoves or open fires)4. The incomplete combustion of solid fuels can exhaust many potentially toxic pollutants, including particulate matter (PM), carbon monoxide (CO), nitrogen dioxide and other air pollutants5. While using solid fuels in cooking, a high-level dosage of PM is exhausted (1181.4–5891.7 µg/m3), which is higher than that of other fuels6. Once PM is inhaled by the body, it can travel through our circulatory system and eventually damage organs, such as the lungs, heart, and even the brain, which may cause brain inflammation and neuronal dysfunction7,8.

In numerous studies, the mixture of pollutants from burning solid fuel was certified as the cause of several diseases, such as cardiovascular disease9, acute respiratory infections10, low birth weight11, chronic obstructive pulmonary disease, lung cancer, tuberculosis12, asthma13, and diseases of eye, but few studies have investigated its role in cognitive decline. Currently, most studies have primarily paid attention to the association between outdoor air pollution and cognitive decline, such as ambient air pollution14,15 and traffic-related air pollution16,17. Only a few studies of IAP have suggested that household incense burning causes cognitive decline18,19 and one cross-sectional study showed that there is an association between solid fuel use and cognitive function20.

China is one of the largest developing countries with a population of 1.4 billion. Most populations are threatened by IAP, with widespread use of solid fuel3. In China, fuels for household use contributed only 7.5% of the total Chinese energy consumption but contributed 71% and 27% of the indoor and outdoor PM2.5 concentrations, respectively, and 67% of PM2.5-related premature deaths in 201421. Meanwhile, China is also one of the largest aging countries, and the aging problem will reach its peak after a few decades22. The cognitive decline of middle-aged and elderly people is one of the most important public health problems. There is a growing amount of evidence showing that cognitive decline can be affected by many factors, and cognitive decline affects both elderly individuals and middle-aged individuals23,24 because they all face the constant effects of aging, other diseases and environmental threats. Under this background, IAP from burning solid fuel may have more long-lasting and serious effects on cognitive decline among these people25,26. Taking into account the rapid growth of the middle-aged and elderly populations and the prevalence of cognitive deficits, further evidence about the association between solid fuel use and cognitive function is invaluable in China.

However, previous studies linking solid fuel use to cognitive decline lacked enough evidence, especially longitudinal studies19,20. In this study, we used a nationally representative sample of Chinese middle-aged and elderly participants to explore the hypothesis that solid fuel use for cooking is associated with worse cognitive function and a faster cognitive decline rate and to assess the moderation effect of gender and residential area on cognitive function among solid fuel users.

Methods and measurements

Study sample

The China Health and Retirement Longitudinal Study (CHARLS) is a nationally representative longitudinal survey of persons in China 45 years of age or older that was conducted by Peking University. The baseline survey was selected from 23 Chinese provinces using multistage sampling in 2011, and 3 follow-up surveys were conducted in 2013, 2015 and 2018. Details of the design and methodology regarding this program have been described elsewhere27. The CHARLS datasets from the 4 survey waves were used in the study. There were 17,705 respondents at baseline, and 11,981 of them continued to take the survey from 2011 to 2018 after excluding those lost to follow-up and the deceased population. Furthermore, we removed the records that failed to meet the research requirements, which led to 5140 eligible respondents in the study. Figure 1 shows the process of exclusion of research participants in the study.

Figure 1
figure 1

Flowchart of participant eligibility in the current study.

Cognitive assessment

Cognitive function was assessed by four composite measurements, including episodic memory, time orientation, numerical ability and picture drawing27. First, to measure episodic memory, interviewers read 10 Chinese words and asked participants to repeat the words that they remembered (immediate recall). In addition, they were asked to recall the 10 words 5 min later (delayed recall) and were given 1 point for each word they recalled correctly. The score for episodic memory was equal to the average of the sum of immediate recall and delayed recall, ranging from 0 to 10. Second, the measurement of time orientation required respondents to recall today’s date (year, month, day), the day of the week, and the current season. The time orientation score was equal to the number of correct answers to the 5 questions, ranging from 0 to 5. Third, the numerical ability required respondents to perform 7 serial subtractions starting from 100 (up to 5 times), and the total score was equal to the number of correct calculations, ranging from 0 to 5. However, this total score was reduced by half if participants used paper, pen or other aid to complete this assessment. Fourth, to measure the ability to draw a picture, interviewers showed a picture of 2 pentagons overlapping with participants and asked them to draw this picture on a white paper. The respondents who successfully drew this picture received 1 point, but those who failed to draw it received no score. Finally, the total cognitive function score was defined as the sum of all 4 parts, ranging from 0 to 2128.

Solid fuel exposure

Participants’ exposure to cooking fuels was assessed by an item of the questionnaire. The interviewer asked the respondents the question “what is the main source of cooking fuel?”, and the response options included coal, crop residue, wood, natural gas, marsh gas, liquefied petroleum gas, electric, never cook and others. Because we were not sure what the other fuel was, we excluded the users of other fuels and those that answered that they never cook. Considering a previous study on the health effects of fuel burning21, we defined cooking fuel as solid fuel (coal, crop residue and wood) and clean fuel (natural gas, marsh gas, liquefied petroleum gas and utilizing electricity).

Covariates

We controlled for 4 sets of potential covariates associated with cognition and solid fuel use in the study, including sociodemographic characteristics, health behaviors, health status and time (years since baseline). The sociodemographic characteristics included gender (male/female), age (continuous), residential area (rural/urban), marital status (married/unmarried), and education (no finished primary school/primary/middle school/high school and above). The health behaviors included smoking status (never smoker/current smoker/former smoker), drinking status (never drinker/a little/frequency), and sleep time (< 6 h/6–8 h/> 8 h). Health status comprised depressive symptoms (no/yes, assessed by CESD-10)28, number of chronic diseases (0/1/≧ 2, diagnosed by a doctor), and self-rated health (good/fair/poor, assessed by themselves).

Statistical analysis

First, we used multiple imputation (MI) to estimate the missing values for depressive symptoms. We used the linear regression model based on gender, age, residential area, education level, marital status, chronic diseases, self-rated health and cognitive score as predictors to estimate 20 replications to account for missing depressive scores per year. Second, baseline characteristics of CHARLS are described as the mean ± standard deviation (SD) for the continuous variables or percentages (%) for categorical variables according to cooking fuel use. T tests (for continuous variables) and Pearson’s chi-square (for categorical variables) were applied to compare the differences in basic characteristics between the solid fuel users and clean fuel users. Third, considering the variations in participant characteristics over time and the lack of independence of repeated measurement data, we used a linear mixed model to verify the hypotheses of the study. After adjusting for gender, age, residential area, marital status, education, smoking status, drinking status, sleep time, depressive symptoms, chronic diseases, self-rated health, cooking fuel use × time and time (years since baseline), we used a linear mixed model to assess the effect of solid fuel use on cognitive function. Fourth, to assess the moderation effect of gender and residential area, we added interaction terms to the linear mixed models. Finally, to examine the difference in the decline rate of the cognitive score among the different groups, the study, based on gender and age stratification, added an interaction term for cooking fuel use and time to the linear mixed models with adjustment for all covariates. All data processing and analyses were performed in STATA version 14.0 (StataCorp LLC, College Station, Texas, USA). All tests were two-sided, and P < 0.05 was considered statistically significant. The analysis results for the linear mixed models are reported using standardized coefficients (β) and their 95% confidence intervals (CIs).

Results

Baseline characteristics

A total of 5140 participants were included in the final analyses; the mean age was 58.18, and 52.2% of the participants were men. A total of 2420 (47.1%) participants used solid fuel for cooking, and 1618 (34.5%) respondents still used it in 2018. Most of the participants (77.4%) lived in rural areas, and 2565 (64.5%) reported using solid fuel for cooking. Approximately one-third of the participants had not finished primary school, and 66.6% of these participants used solid fuel. Furthermore, compared with clean fuel users, solid fuel users were more likely to be women, older, live in rural areas, not have finished primary school, current smoker, have a sleep time less than 6 h, depressive symptoms, 2 or more chronic diseases, and a poor health status and lower cognitive scores (P < 0.01 for all). Table 1 shows more of the baseline characteristics according to different cooking fuel uses.

Table 1 Baseline characteristics of study participants.

Association between cooking fuel use and cognitive function

In the multivariate analysis of the linear mixed model, compared with clean fuel users, solid fuel users had lower cognitive scores (β =  − 0.17, P = 0.001). Moreover, gender, age, residential area, marital status, educational level, drinking status, sleep time, depressive symptoms, chronic diseases, self-rated health, time, the interaction of time and cooking fuel use all had a significant relationship with cognitive function (P < 0.05 for all). Table 2 describes more details about the factors influencing cognitive function.

Table 2 Association of cognitive function with cooking fuel use.

To analyze the moderation effect of gender, an interaction term was added to the linear mixed model. Compared with the reference, female users of solid fuel had lower cognitive scores (β =  − 0.96, P < 0.001), but there was no significant interaction between male gender and solid fuel users. In addition, in the model assessing the moderation effect of residential area, solid fuel users in rural areas had lower cognitive scores (β =  − 1.01, P < 0.001), but no interaction was not found for solid fuel users in urban areas (Table 3).

Table 3 The interaction between cooking fuel use and socio-demographic characteristics.

The decline of cognitive function from 2011 to 2018

During the 7-year follow-up period, the cognitive scores of the different groups continued to drop. Compared with clean fuel users, participants who used solid fuel for cooking had a faster decline rate in cognitive scores (from 11.7 to 10.3, annual decline rate: 1.8%). In addition, in the follow-up period, the cognitive scores of male users of clean fuel fell slower than those of the other groups (from 13.7 to 12.6, annual decline rate: 1.2%), and female users of solid fuel had the fastest decline rate (from 10.7 to 9.2, annual decline rate: 2.1%) (Fig. 2).

Figure 2
figure 2

Cognitive change in the participants during the follow-up period.

Association between solid fuel use and the rates of cognitive decline

In the linear mixed models, there were different decline rates of cognitive function in the different subgroups. In the total sample, female sample and rural samples, solid fuel users had faster decline rates of cognitive scores (P < 0.05 for all), but the same effect was not found in the male (P = 0.147) or middle-aged samples (P = 0.719). Compared with the reference groups, the decline rate of the cognitive scores increased by 0.04 units per year (β =  − 0.04; 95% CI − 0.09, 0.01) among female users and 0.06 units among the elderly sample (β =  − 0.06; 95% CI − 0.11, 0.01) (Table 4).

Table 4 Association between solid fuel use and the rate of cognitive decline.

Discussion

As one of the largest prospective cohort studies in Chinese adults, this study found that solid fuel users had poorer cognitive function, especially female users and rural users. In addition, compared with clean fuel users, solid fuel users had a faster decline rate of cognitive function, especially among the female and older samples. Therefore, gender inequality and rural–urban differences deserve more attention, especially in the elderly.

The study is in line with several previous studies. A cross-sectional study that included 13,023 older (age 50+) Mexican adults examined whether exposure to indoor air pollution from cooking fuel (coal or wood) was associated with poorer cognitive function20. In addition, Qiu et al. found a significant adverse impact of IAP use for cooking on cognitive abilities in Chinese middle-aged and elderly people, specifically short-term memory and mathematical reasoning19. However, these previous studies used a cross-sectional study design, and therefore our study supplemented the results from a longitudinal study and found a significant association between solid fuel use and cognitive decline (especially in the female and elderly samples). Our study may help develop intervention strategies for the rapidly increasing aging population and environmental pollution in China.

Although we found that solid fuel use was related to slight cognitive decline, the continuous adverse health impacts cannot be ignored for middle-aged and elderly people29. The evidence about the mechanism of solid fuel use exposure-related cognitive decline is not extensive, but it may be linked to the fine PM, CO or other pollutants released by solid fuel combustion30. The burning of solid fuels produces high concentrations of PM and other pollutants, which may increase brain inflammation and the accumulation of β-amyloid, a marker of neuronal dysfunction8. Therefore, individuals who suffer from PM pollution may be more likely to develop cognitive deficits, structural brain aging and even neurodegenerative diseases31,32. The elderly are especially more susceptible to various environmental risk factors, such as PM-burning solid fuel23. If cognitive function continues to decline, it will influence the normal life of people and cause Alzheimer’s disease25. Even so, in parts of the Chinese countryside, where rapid economic growth and infrastructure expansion have contributed to universal access to electricity, solid fuel use has persisted3, which may cause additional health hazards to the rural population. In summary, the use of solid fuel for cooking not only causes disease suffering and cognitive decline but also exacerbates aging and environmental pollution problems in China.

Solid fuel use not only brings about severe health risks to users but also exacerbates gender inequality in a negative way33,34. In low- and middle-income regions such as rural China, women have lower cognitive function than men because families may only emphasize the development of male ability traditionally but ignore women’s demands35. Just so, it is difficult for women to improve their cognitive ability in the course of personal development36. In addition to the lower cognitive function, female users of solid fuel for cooking had a faster decline rate of cognitive function than male users. Women usually take care of their family and cooking, whereas men traditionally work away from home during the day37. The Chinese tradition leads to women being particularly exposed to IAP from solid fuel burning and having a higher risk of developing IAP-associated adverse health conditions38,39,40. Therefore, solid fuel use and Chinese tradition bring about health impacts and gender inequality that are too significant to be ignored among the middle-aged and elderly.

Overall, necessary steps should be taken in China to alleviate the impact of solid fuel use on health. Promoting cleaner fuels, using stoves with chimneys and improving ventilation efficiency may be the best way to reduce the exposure of IAP within households, but it is a challenge that clean fuels are widely used for cooking in remote rural areas41. Poorer families are likely to stick with solid fuels for cooking in rural areas because alternative clean fuels cannot be affordable for them, and some biomass fuels are everywhere in the countryside, such as straw, animal dung and others. Governments must consider the personalized needs of these people and work with community leaders to implement programs about reducing exposure to IAP caused by solid fuel burning20. In addition, other developmental objectives, such as promoting entrepreneurship, providing jobs and women’s empowerment, also reduce the exposure of burning solid fuel, especially for women42. After all, women benefit more from fruits of economic development—it may less female disadvantage in cognitive function35.

This study has several limitations. First, our study results can only be generalized to middle-aged and elderly people in China, and the impacts of solid fuels may vary among different populations. Second, we assessed whether solid fuel use was based on self-reported main fuel use for cooking rather than the participant’s actual exposure dose, and pollutant exposure dose could vary by the level and efficiency of ventilation, climate, and fuel properties; therefore, we could not find an association of fine PM or other pollutant compositions of solid fuel smoke with cognitive decline43. Third, we could not assess the exposure time of each participant44. Fourth, cognitive function in the study was assessed by self-rating scales, which are less than clinical diagnoses. Finally, interviewers collected information by a questionnaire so that recall bias of this study was possible.

Conclusions

This study suggests that solid fuel use for cooking is associated with lower cognitive scores and that these scores were lower among female and rural users of solid fuel in the Chinese middle-aged and elderly populations. In addition, solid fuel use was related to accelerated decline in cognitive function, especially among female and older users. To our knowledge, our study supplemented evidence from a longitudinal study about the effects of solid fuel use on cognitive decline. Further studies including a more objective assessment of individual exposure to IAP from solid fuel burning are needed to confirm our findings.