Introduction

In modern Western societies, it has been seen that women usually prefer men who are significantly taller than average1,2,3, while men are more tolerant in choosing women who are taller or shorter than average4. This is consistent with the idea that male height can be adaptive5 and sexual selection favours taller men, possibly because height may represent an honest signal of individual quality, providing hereditary advantages, such as genetic quality for the offspring6,7, or direct benefits, provisioning resources and protection for women and their children8. Following these last possible benefits, height has been also proposed as an indicator of resource holding potential (RHP), in terms of social dominance and deference9,10 and socioeconomic status6,11.

This idea is supported by evidence that the male height is directly correlated with reproductive success, which is not applicable to women, suggesting an unrestricted directional selection that favours very tall men but not to very tall women12. In fact, it has been reported that taller men (but not extremely tall men) are more likely to find a long-term partner and have several different long-term partners13, while the maximum reproductive success of women is below the female average height14. Furthermore, heterosexual men and women tend to adjust their preferred height of hypothetical partners according to their own stature15. In general, heterosexual men and women prefer couples in which the man is taller than the woman, and women show a preference for facial cues that denote a taller man16.

Although previous evidence has supported the idea that human height could be a sexually selected trait, little is known regarding whether this role is based on the honest signalling of individual quality6. To test this idea, it is important to consider that height needs to face a trade-off with other life-history components17, such as reproduction18 or immunity19, and its expression should involve certain costs that not all individuals could equally afford, such that there would be an important phenotypic variation on this trait. Both aspects are present in human height. First, growth in body height is a life-history component1,20 that involves important costs for its expression and maintenance. The costs in height can be measured in terms of survival and physiological expenditure19. For example, according to the Hayflick limit theory of ageing21, our cells have a limited number of cell replications available in a lifetime. A minimal increment in body height involves more cells, maybe trillions, and large numbers of cell replications. These large numbers of cell replications demand a large pool of proteins to maintain taller, larger bodies19, which together, with an increase of free radicals generated by normal cellular metabolism, may lead to a greater likelihood of DNA damage22, thus increasing the incidence of cancer and reducing longevity19.

Secondly, reproduction and immunity could face a trade-off with height because the effect of sex hormones on these life-history components; the trade-off with reproduction occurs when the notorious increment of sex steroids, particularly testosterone and oestrogens, induce an accelerated growth period in puberty for both sexes23, but also a reallocation of physiological resources for reproduction posterior to this period (i.e. spermatogenesis, follicular maturation, etc.), which results in a growth cessation. Both steroids stimulate mineral deposition in the growth plates at the ends of the long bones, thus terminating cell proliferation and resulting in the fusion of the growth plates to the shaft of the bone24,25.

In turn, the increment of sexual steroids at sexual maturity triggers another trade-off for individuals that is particularly associated with the effect of testosterone on immunity26. Usually, testosterone exerts suppressive effects on several, but not all, immune components27. For example, it may negatively affect the activity and cellular proliferation of several adaptive and innate immune responses, such as Macrophages, Natural Killers, production of cytokines28 and helper T cells and lymphocyte activation (i.e. Th2 and Th1729). In consequence, it has been documented that testosterone may influence general health patterns, in relation to the severity of certain infections such as malaria, leishmaniasis, amoebiasis30, and tuberculosis31,32

Therefore, as a consequence of these life-history trade-offs, height could be considered as a reliable indicator of an individual’s condition in terms of (1) the amount and quality of nutritional resources acquired until sexual maturity, (2) the RHP to obtain resources for the somatic maintenance in the adult stage, and (3) the general health condition, given the possibly costly immunosuppressive effect of testosterone. Thus, height can be used for potential partners to receive information about the quality of potential mates; only high-quality individuals could afford to allocate resources to this attractive secondary sexual trait33, which would result in an increased sexual preference towards taller individuals.

Despite extensive studies on the association between height and attractiveness, the role of height as a signal of biological quality has been largely studied but results are still controversial. For example, one aspect that has been particularly studied is the relationship between height and various indicators of health. Generally, height is positively related to measures of health in men, such as coronary heart disease morbidity and mortality34, limiting long-standing illness6 and perceived health6,35. Nevertheless, the implementation of sex and other indicators of health has led to conflicting results, demonstrating the complexity of the question. For instance, mortality by cancer diseases has been associated more to taller than shorter people of both sexes36, and height somewhat predicts general health in women but following a curvilinear trend6.

Moreover, most studies have been done using high-income developed populations such as Western, Educated, Industrialised, Rich and Democratic (WEIRD) societies37, which has led to a lack of information on what is occurring in other populations with important socio-ecological differences35. These ecological pressures are important because although genetic allelic expression could be the main factor that determines individual height differences35, height is also the most sensible human anatomical feature that responds to environmental and socioeconomic conditions18,38. For instance, variation in height across social classes is known to be greater in poorer countries39 but is much reduced in countries with higher standards of living40. Economic inequality not only affects the population’s nutritional patterns, which are especially important during childhood to establish adult height, but also the presence of infectious diseases41. Childhood disease is known to adversely affect growth. For instance, mounting an immune response to fight against the infection requires concomitant increases in metabolic rate, which could affect the net nutrition, and hence reduces productivity. Disease also prevents food intake, impairs nutrient absorption and causes nutrient loss42,43. Therefore, compared with high-income and developed populations, habitants from locations with stronger ecological pressures imposed by pathogens or greater nutritional deficiencies would face greater costs to robustly express this trait, thereby showing stronger sexual selective pressure over height, as it signals growth rates, life-history trajectories, and health status more accurately. This phenotypic variation is described as developmental plasticity, which is a part of the phenotypic plasticity related to growth and development, in response to social, nutritional and demographic conditions, among others44. During the last century, given a general improvement in nutrition, human height has steadily increased across the globe45, but the level of dimorphism in favour of men is maintained.

Colombia and Mexico are two of the most socioeconomically heterogeneous countries in the world with a high Human Development Index46. Colombia and Mexico attain respective scores of 68 and 66 in the Healthcare Access and Quality Index47, indicating that the populations are in relatively good health compared to global standards. Yet, Colombia and Mexico have GINI coefficients of 50.8 and 43.4, respectively, making them the 12th and 43rd most unequal countries in the world (GINI index – World Bank estimate; https://data.worldbank.org/indicator/SI.POV.GINI). These national-level statistics, however, hide important within-country differences. In particular, Latin-American people in rural areas tend to be poorer and have less access to basic services such as health and education than people in urban areas.

According to data from the World Bank and the Colombian National Administrative Department of Statistics, in 2017 Colombia was the second most unequal country in Latin-America after Brazil. In rural areas, 36% of people were living in poverty and 15.4% in extreme poverty, while in urban areas, these values were only 15.7% and 2.7%, respectively48.

In addition to rural communities, in Latin-America indigenous people tend to have high rates of poverty and extreme poverty49, and poorer health50, which is less susceptible to improvement by national income growth51. In Mexico, there are at least 56 independent indigenous peoples whose lifestyle practices differ in varying degrees from the typical ‘urbanised’ lifestyle. Among these groups, the Me’Phaa people, from an isolated region known as ‘Montaña Alta’ of the state of Guerrero, is one of the groups whose lifestyle most dramatically differs from the typical Westernised lifestyle of more urbanised areas52. Me’Phaa communities are small groups of indigenous people, composed of 50 to 80 families, each with five to ten family members. Most communities are based largely on subsistence farming of legumes such as beans and lentils, and the only grain cultivated is corn. Animal protein is acquired by hunting and raising some fowls, and meat is only consumed during special occasions but not as part of the daily diet. There is almost no access to allopathic medications, and there is no health service, plumbing or water purification system. Water for washing and drinking is obtained from small wells. Most of the Me’Phaa speak only their native language53. In consequence, these communities have the lowest income and economic development in the country, and the highest child morbidity and mortality due to chronic infectious diseases52.

These three Latin-American populations can provide an interesting indication about how the regional socioeconomic conditions and intensity of ecological pressures by pathogens may modulate the function of height as an informative sexually selected trait of health and individual condition in each sex. Therefore, the aim of the present study was to evaluate whether human height is related to health measured by self-perception, and relevant nutritional and health anthropometric indicators in three Latin-American populations that widely differ in socioeconomic and ecological conditions: two urbanised populations from Bogota (Colombia) and Mexico City (Mexico), and one isolated indigenous population (Me´Phaa, Mexico). In addition, given the possible immunological effects of testosterone, and that men present higher levels than women, we predicted this relation to be different between sexes in all studied populations. Therefore, we propose that height would be a stronger signal of self-reported health condition in men compared to women.

Methods

All procedures for testing and recruitment were approved by Universidad El Bosque Institutional Committee on Research Ethics (PCI.2017–9444) and National Autonomous University of Mexico Committee on Research Ethics (FPSI/CE/01/2016), and run in accordance with the ethical principles and guidelines of the Colombian College of Psychologists (COLPSIC) and the Official Mexican Law (NOM-012-SSA3-2012). All participants read and signed a written informed consent.

Participants

A total of 477 adults (238 women and 239 men) participated in this study. They were from three different samples: (1) Mexican indigenous population, (2) Mexican urban population and (3) Colombian urban population. In Mexico, Me’Phaa indigenous participants from ‘La Montaña Alta’ were recruited and participated in this study between January and March 2017, while data from participants from Mexico City was collected between May and June 2017. In Colombia, data collection was carried out between October 2018 and May 2019.

The first sample consisted of 63 subjects (mean age ± standard deviation [SD] = 33.63 ± 9.69 years old) from the small Me’Phaa community – ‘Plan de Gatica’ from a region known as ‘Montaña Alta’ of the state of Guerrero in Southwest Mexico. In this sample, 24 participants were women (33.46 ± 8.61 years old) and 39 participants were men (33.74 ± 10.41 years old), who participated in a larger study on immunocompetence. Both sexes were aged above 18 years old. In Mexico, people above 18 years old are considered adults. All measurements were collected in the participants’ own community. Me’Phaa communities are about 20 km apart, and it takes about three hours of travel on rural dirt roads to reach the nearest large town, about 80 km away. Mexico City is about 850 km away, and the trip takes about twelve hours by road. This community has the lowest income in Mexico, the highest index of child morbidity and mortality by gastrointestinal and respiratory diseases (children aged 0 to 8 years had the highest vulnerability and death risk52), and the lowest access to health services. These conditions were recorded in the National Health Information System 201652.

The second sample consisted of 60 subjects of over 18 years old (30.27 ± 8.56 years old) from the general community in Mexico City, of whom 30 were women (37.47 ± 5.61 years old) and 30 were men (23.07 ± 3.22 years old). Finally, the third sample consisted of 354 undergraduate students with ages ranging from 18 to 30 years old (20.39 ± 2.10 years old), 184 were women (20.16 ± 2.08 years old), and 170 were men (20.64 ± 2.10 years old) from Bogota, Colombia. All urban participants were recruited through public advertisements.

Participants from both urban population samples were taking part in two separate, larger studies in each country. In Colombia, all data were collected in the morning, between 7 and 11 am, because saliva samples (for hormonal analysis), as well as voice recordings, body odour samples, and facial photographs, were also collected as part of a separate project. Additionally, women in the Colombian and Mexican samples were not hormonal contraception users, and all data were collected within the first three days of their menses.

Participants who were under allopathic treatment and hormonal contraception users from both countries were excluded from data collection. All participants completed a sociodemographic data questionnaire, which included medical and psychiatric history. No women were users of hormonal contraception. Although no participant reported any endocrinological or chronic disease, these health issues were also considered as exclusion criteria.

Given that the indigenous community of ‘Plan de Gatica’ consists of 60–80 families, each with five to seven members, the final sample for this study could be considered as semi-representative of a larger Me’Phaa population inhabiting in the same community. Nevertheless, the total population of Me’Phaa people inhabiting the ‘Montaña Alta’ is comprised of 20–30 communities with almost the same number of families as ‘Plan de Gatica’. Therefore, it is important to mention that our sample size cannot be considered representative of the total Me’Phaa people inhabiting the ‘Montaña Alta’ region, but from the specific ‘Plan de Gatica’ community. Similar condition occurs for participants from the Mexico City and Bogota samples. These participants were recruited at the National Autonomous University of México and Universidad El Bosque campuses, respectively. Therefore, these samples are comprised mostly of bachelor and graduate students, and cannot be considered as representative of a large population of the whole city, which is comprised of about 12 million adult persons in Mexico and about 5 million adults in Bogota.

Procedure

All participants signed the informed consent and completed the health and background questionnaires. For participants from the indigenous population, the whole procedure was carried out within their own communities, and participants from the Mexican and Colombian urban population attended a laboratory at either the National Autonomous University of México or Universidad El Bosque respectively, on individual appointments.

Participants from Mexico City and Bogota were recruited through public advertisements on social media and poster boards located along the central campus of the National Autonomous University and Universidad El Bosque. While in Mexico City, participants received either one partial course credit or a payment equivalent to $5 dollars as compensation for their participation, all participants in Bogota were given academic credits for their participation.

For the indigenous groups, recruitment was done through the Xuajin Me’Phaa non-governmental organisation, which is dedicated to the social, environmental and economic development for the indigenous communities of the region (see video from this organisation, https://youtu.be/WOEcGUHjR9Q). Xuajin Me’Phaa has extensive experience in community-based fieldwork and has built a close working relationship with the community authorities. The trust and familiarity with the community customs and protocols have previously led to successful academic collaborations53,54. Therefore, Xuajin Me’Phaa served as a liaison between the Mexican research group and the communities for the present study, offering mainly two important factors in data collection: the informed consent of community members and participants, and two trained interpreters of Me’Phaa and Spanish language of both sexes.

First, participants were asked to complete the health and sociodemographic data questionnaires. Subsequently, the anthropometric measurements were taken.

Self-reported health

In order to obtain a standardised value of self-perception of health, we implemented in all three populations the Short Form (36) health survey (SF-36; RAND Corp.; https://www.rand.org/health-care/surveys_tools/mos/36-item-short-form/survey-instrument.html). The SF-36 produces eight dimensions, of which we only used the las one (8) General health. Each factor is calculated by averaging the recoded scores of individual items: (1) Physical functioning (items 3 to 12), (2) Role limitations due to physical health (items 13 to 16), (3) Role limitations due to emotional problems (items 17 to 19), (4) Energy/fatigue (items 23, 27, 29 and 31), (5) Emotional well-being (items 24, 25, 26, 28 and 30), (6) Social functioning (items 20 and 32), (7) Pain (items 21 and 22) and (8) General health (items 1, 33, 34, 35 and 36).

The interpreters provided by the Xuajin Me’Phaa organisation administered the SF-36 Health survey in Me’Phaa language. Interpreters used Spanish as the second language and are thoroughly proficient in speaking and reading Spanish. We used the validated SF-36 survey for urban and rural Mexican populations55 for interpreters to translate Spanish to Me’Phaa language. Given the ethnical customs of Me’Phaa culture, the participants were always interviewed by an interpreter of the same sex to avoid bias in participant responses; for instance, men were interviewed by a male interpreter and women by a female interpreter. The same interpreter interviewed all participants of his/her corresponding sex.

For the present study, both urban and indigenous participants only answered items corresponding to the dimension defined as general health (i.e. Item numbers 1, 33, 34, 35 and 36), except for item 35. This item informs about the expectation for future health. Since the grammatical compositions of Me’Phaa language do not consider ‘infinitive’ and ‘future’ as verbal tenses56, an interpretation of this question was not possible for the Me’Phaa people, therefore, this item was excluded. In Colombia, we used a Spanish version of the SF-36 questionnaire57, that was previously validated in the same country58.

To obtain the self-reported health rate, all items were recoded following the instructions on how to score SF-3657. We calculated the final factor by averaging the recoded items. To make this data compatible with the Mexican database, item 35 was excluded because it cannot be answered by the Mexican Indigenous population, and the general health dimension was calculated by averaging only items 1, 33, 34 and 36.

Anthropometric measurements

All anthropometric measurements were measured thrice and subsequently averaged to obtain the mean value (for agreement statistics between the three measurements of each characteristic, see section 1.3 in the Supplementary Material). All participants wore light clothing and had their shoes removed. The same observer repeated the measurements thrice.

We measured the body height in cm, to the nearest mm, by using a 220 cm Zaude stadiometer, with the participant’s head aligned according to the Frankfurt horizontal plane, and feet together against the wall.

Anthropomorphic measurements also included waist circumference (cm), weight (kg), fat percentage, visceral fat level, muscle percentage and body mass index (BMI). The waist circumference was measured midway between the lowest rib and the iliac crest in cm by using a flexible tape and was recorded to the nearest mm. These anthropomorphic measurements have been used as an accurate index of nutritional status and health, especially waist circumference. Metabolic syndrome is associated with visceral adiposity, blood lipid disorders, inflammation, insulin resistance or full-blown diabetes and increased risk of developing cardiovascular disease59,60,61, amongst Latin-American populations62. Waist circumference has been proposed as a crude anthropometric correlate of abdominal and visceral adiposity, and it is the simplest and accurate screening variable used to identify people with the features of metabolic syndrome63,64. Hence, in the presence of the clinical criteria of metabolic syndrome, increased waist circumference provides relevant pathophysiological information insofar as it defines the prevalent form of the syndrome resulting from abdominal obesity60.

Weight, fat percentage, visceral fat level, muscle percentage, and BMI were obtained using an Omron Healthcare HBF-510 body composition analyser, which was calibrated before each participant’s measurements were obtained.

Statistical analysis

We used linear models (LM) to test the association between height and self-reported health. The dependent variable in this model was the health factor and predictor variables included participant sex, age, sample (Bogota, Mexico City, Me’Phaa), height and waist and anthropometric measurements (hip, weight, fat percentage, BMI and muscle percentage) as fixed, main effects, as well as all possible interactions between height, waist, sample, and sex. For all models, the continuous regressors involved in interactions (waist and height) were centred.

Although sample could be thought as a random factor (i.e. fitting linear mixed models instead), we treated it as a fixed-effects categorical predictor in the models because there were only three levels (Bogota, Mexico City, Me’Phaa), and a minimum of five levels is recommended. To test the residual distribution, generalised linear models (GLM) were fitted, but in all cases, residuals were closer to a normal or gamma (inverse link) distribution, for each sample. Models here included were fitted using the lm function in R, version 3.6.165.

The most parameterised initial model (Model 1) was then reduced, by excluding the main effects of hip, weight, fat percentage, visceral fat, BMI and muscle percentage (as these are phenotypic markers associated either with height or waist circumference), and keeping the main effects of age, as well as the main effects and all possible interactions between any combination of height, waist, sample, and sex, consistent with our predictions. This, still highly parameterised model (Model 2), was further reduced using the functions dredge (https://www.rdocumentation.org/packages/MuMIn/versions/1.43.6/topics/dredge) and model.sel (https://www.rdocumentation.org/packages/MuMIn/versions/1.43.6/topics/model.sel) from the package MuMIn: Multi-Model Inference66. The dredge function fitted a set of 334 models with combinations (subsets) of fixed-effect terms from the second model, that were then compared using the function model.sel based on the Akaike Information Criterion (AICc) and Akaike weights, allowing us to select the best model (Model 3). This, best-supported model (i.e. the model with the lowest AICc with a ΔAICc higher than two units from the second most adequate model), is reported67.

Finally, we compared the three models selected model (Models 1, 2 and 3) using the ICtab function from the bbmle package68. Once a final model was selected, model diagnostics were performed (collinearity, residual distribution, and linearity of residuals in each single term effect; see section 3.3 in the Supplementary Material).

Interactions in the final model were explored and via simple slopes analysis and Johnson-Neyman intervals69,70, using the R package interactions: Comprehensive, User-Friendly Toolkit for Probing Interactions71. For this purpose, we implemented the functions sim_slopes (https://www.rdocumentation.org/packages/interactions/versions/1.1.1/topics/sim_slopes), interact_plot (https://www.rdocumentation.org/packages/interactions/versions/1.1.1/topics/interact_plot) and johnson_neyman (https://www.rdocumentation.org/packages/interactions/versions/1.1.1/topics/johnson_neyman).

Results

All data and code used to perform these analyses are openly available from the Open Science Framework (OSF) project for this study (https://osf.io/5rzfs/).

Descriptives

Descriptive statistics of age, waist circumference, hip, height, weight, fat percentage, visceral fat, BMI, muscle percentage and self-reported health and reported in Table 1.

Table 1 Descriptive statistics of measured variables of all participants.

The distribution of all measured variables is shown in Fig. 1. Age, waist, height, visceral fat, and self-reported health strongly varied in both women (Fig. 1a) and men (Fig. 1b) between samples.

Figure 1
figure 1

Distribution of all measured variables by sex and sample. (a) Women. (b) Men. Samples are represented as colours (blue = Bogota; purple = Mexico City; red = Me’Phaa). For a comparison of sex differences in height, waist circumference and self-reported health in the three samples, see Supplementary Fig. S1.

Age, waist circumference, height, fat percentage, visceral fat, BMI and muscle percentage, were significantly correlated with self-reported health (r > 0.10, in all cases) in both men and women (bivariate Pearson correlations between all measured variables for all participants combined are shown in the Supplementary Table S2, for women in Supplementary Table S3, and for men in Supplementary Table S4).

Models to predict self-reported health

To establish the relationship between height and self-reported health, we fitted three linear models (Table 2). For all models, the continuous regressors involved in interactions (waist and height) were mean-centred.

Table 2 Results of separate LMs testing effects of independent variables on self-reported health.

In the first model (Model 1), we included as predictors all measured variables as main effects, as well as all interactions between height, waist circumference, sample, and sex. The first model was initially reduced by excluding hip, weight, fat percentage, BMI and muscle percentage. We decided to include waist circumference instead of visceral fat or fat percentage for two reasons: first, because these three variables are strongly correlated in women and men (r > 0.79 in all cases; see Supplementary Tables S3 and S4, for women and men, respectively). And second, because unlike visceral fat or fat percentage, waist circumference can be directly perceived by others, and hence could have a direct effect on mate-choice; fat percentage and visceral fat, on the other hand, are likely perceived and assessed in social contexts through other variables, including relative waist size.

In the second model (Model 2), we therefore included age, height, sample, sex, waist circumference, and all possible interactions between combinations of height, waist circumference, sample, and sex. This second model was further reduced by the implementation of the functions dredge and model.sel from the package MuMIn66 (for details, see the Statistical analysis section in the Methods). These functions fitted and compared a total of 334 models with different combinations of fixed terms from Model 2; these compared models and their relative probability to be the best model are shown according to their relative Akaike weights (wi(AICc)) in Fig. 2.

Figure 2
figure 2

Model selection plot. Rows represent each of the 334 compared models. Cells coloured in blue represent included terms in each model, according to their Akaike weight (wi(AICc)), represented as the height of each row/model. Given the important age differences between samples, we selected the second-best model (labelled 160), because it had the same structure as the best model (labelled 159), but also included Age as a regressor. Furthermore, this second-best model had a ΔAICc of less than 2 units (≈0.98) compared to the best model. For model terms: WC(c) = Waist circumference (centred); H(c) = Height (centred); S = Sample.

This analysis revealed that the best model (labelled 159 in Fig. 2), included height (centred), sample, sex, waist circumference (centred) and the interaction between height (centred) and waist (centred). However, to account for the age differences between samples, we selected the second-best model (labelled 160 in Fig. 2), because it also included age as a regressor, and had a ΔAICc of less than 2 units (≈0.98) compared to the best model. This model, including age, was therefore selected as our final model (Model 3).

The three selected models were compared using the AICc, Akaike weights (wi(AICc)) and ΔAICc (Table 3). The analyses revealed that Model 3 is not only the most parsimonious of the three selected models, but has higher R2adjusted and F values (Table 2), as well as a lower AIC and higher Akaike weight67 (Table 3) than the previous two models. In fact, Model 3 is close to 464,686 times more likely to be the best model compared to Model 2, and about 35,141,683 times compared to Model 1 (Model 2, was around 76 times more likely compared to Model 1).

Table 3 Performance criteria of the three selected models.

Furthermore, for Model 3 (the final, minimum adequate model), Generalised Variance Inflation Factors (GVIF)66 revealed no concerning cases of collinearity for any of the predictor terms (GVIF ≤3, and a GVIF1/(2 × Df) ≤1.6 in all cases; for details, see Supplementary Table S8; residual distribution by sample and linearity in each single term factor are shown in Supplementary Fig. S2).

The final model (Model 3: Table 4; Fig. 3) showed a significant, negative main effect of waist circumference (t = −3.20, p = 0.001), and significant main effects of sex (men rated their health 6.01 points higher than women; t = 2.66, p = 0.008), and sample (Mexico City and Me’Phaa individuals rated their health 9.32 and 19.78 points lower than participants from Bogota; t = −3.25, p = 0.001 and; t = −4.75, p < 0.001, respectively).

Table 4 Results of the final LMM testing effects of independent variables on self-reported health.
Figure 3
figure 3

Model 3 estimates and interaction between Height and Waist. Values of Height and Waist were centred: for Height, mean ± SD = 163.83 ± 9.85; for Waist circumference, mean ± SD = 78.01 ± 10.59. (a) Estimates and 95% CI for each model term. For categorical predictors, women and Bogota were used as reference levels. For model terms, WC(c) = Waist circumference (centred); H(c) = Height (centred); S = Sample. (b) Interaction between Height and Waist. As waist reference, −1 SD (67.43 cm), mean (78.01 cm), and +1 SD (88.6 cm) values were used, showed on a blue to red colour scale. (c) Johnson-Neyman plot, showing for which values of Waist (centred), the slope of Height (centred) is significant as a predictor of Self-reported health; these slopes are predicted to be significant for centred Waists circumferences below −4.51 (73.51 cm), or above 68.73 (146.74 cm; not shown as it is a prediction for extreme values, beyond the ones found in any of our samples).

Moreover, a significant interaction between waist and height (Table 4; t = −2.30, p = 0.022) was revealed, indicating that the negative association of waist circumference with self-reported health was height-dependent (Fig. 3b); the best predicted self-reported health was for tall participants with small waists, and while the association between height and self-reported health is positive for people with small waist circumferences, it decreases for people with increasingly large waists. Furthermore, the Johnson-Neyman procedure69,70 (Fig. 3c), indicated that height is only a significant, always positive, predictor of self-reported health for people with relatively small waist circumferences of less than 73.51 cm (centred: −4.51).

This interaction was replicated when fitting an alternative version of Model 3 (Model 3 A), replacing waist circumference for visceral fat, by following the same method to select it (i.e. fitting an alternative Model 2, and repeating the same selection process; see section 4.3 in the Supplementary Material). Similar to Model 3, this alternative Final Model, also included an interaction between height and, in this case, visceral fat, in which height was found to be a positive significant predictor of self-reported health, only for people with low levels of visceral fat (see Table S9 and Fig. S3 in the Supplementary Material). Furthermore, in this model the interaction was more extreme than when using waist circumference, and height becomes a significant, negative predictor of self-reported health for people with high visceral fat (see Fig. S3c in the Supplementary Material).

Discussion

The present study provides new insights into the relationship between height and health in men and women by studying three Latin-American populations, which included urban and indigenous populations with marked differences in access to basic needs and services like food and health.

Contrary to our initial hypothesis, height was not a significant predictor of self-perceived health but interacted with waist circumference. Most results in favour of a direct relationship between height itself and health were carried out in small modern populations and specific Western ethnic groups more than twenty years ago. New studies with non-traditional population groups have failed to verify the positive relationship between height and health, especially associated with cardiovascular and autoimmune diseases72,73. For example, studies on Native Americans, Japanese, Indians, and Pakistanis showed that shorter people had a lower prevalence of cardiovascular disease than the tallest people in each population73. These findings were similar in Sardinian inhabitants, a European population with the lowest physical stature recorded in Europe in recent years72.

Interestingly, our results suggest that there is a main negative effect of waist circumference on self-perceived health. This is congruent with a broad range of studies done in different human populations74. In fact, waist circumference has been proposed as one of the most important biomarkers of metabolic syndrome that predicts health condition in terms of cardiovascular diseases60. Nevertheless, we found that the positive association between height and self-reported health was waist-dependent in our studied samples. This is, height predicted self-reported health differently for people of different waist circumferences: while being taller predicts better self-reported health for individuals with relatively small waists, this association was not found in people with larger waist circumferences. Furthermore, while there is a cost of abdominal and visceral adiposity for tall people, there is no predicted cost for shorter persons. Interestingly, epidemiological studies have widely implemented an integration of both phenotypical components in the form of waist to height ratio (WtHR). In general, waist circumference has a stronger negative impact on the health of short individuals than on tall ones75, contrasting with our results. These differences might be due to WtHR has been mainly used to predict health in terms of metabolic and cardiovascular diseases (CVD), while our study used a general status of health, which could include more than metabolic and CVD. In addition, we use these phenotypic variables as continuous and independent predictors because the aim of our study argued that human height by itself would be an honest indicator of general health, which we would not be able to evaluate with WtHR as predictor. Therefore, our results argue for the importance of considering a phenotypic independent integration of different human features that could be involved in health or physiological conditions, when a possible sexually selected trait is being evaluated as a signal of individual condition.

On the other hand, given that height is the human anatomical feature most sensitive to environmental and socioeconomic conditions18,38, we expected a stronger association between health and height for the indigenous population where the cost to produce and maintain this costly trait is greater than for inhabitants from urbanised areas. Nevertheless, we did not find inter-population differences in the magnitude of this relation. Urban populations reported better health than the indigenous population, and the shortest participants tended to be from the indigenous Me’Phaa sample. These results could, in fact, suggest different life-history strategies. Compared with modern Western societies, different life strategies could take place in harsh environments76, for instance, investing relatively less energy in growth and reallocating it towards reproduction18. In addition, a relative increase in the intensity or number of infectious diseases (including paediatric diseases in Me’Phaa) and higher tendency to early sexual maturity could negatively impact growth, resulting in a lower average height77,78. These trends could be compensations between life-history components35. Finally, fast and prolonged growth implies high costs for the organism1. Rapid growth may influence mortality risk79 and growing for a longer time delays the onset of reproduction, increasing the risk of death and producing fewer offspring1. This perspective of life strategies allows us to understand the relationship between height, health, and reproduction. This suggests the importance of addressing factors such as ethnicity, socioeconomic status, level of urbanisation in populations where there is great heterogeneity in access to food, health, and pressure from pathogens, for instance, in Latin-American populations in which this relationship has barely been directly explored.

Although our results show that height and waist circumference are important predictors of self-perceived health, we did not evaluate any immunological mechanism that may underlie the self-perception responses of the participants. This limitation makes it hard to directly evaluate human height as an honest signal of individual condition. In the present study, the questionnaire used to evaluate general health (SF-36) is far from being a direct indicator of immune condition, since the participant´s perception responses could be influenced by components different than the individual’s ability to deal or resist to infectious pathogens, such as skeletal disorders, cancers, cardiovascular or metabolic abnormalities. Nevertheless, studies that have evaluated a more direct approximation of immune condition have led to controversial results. For instance, antibody response to a hepatitis-B vaccine as a marker of immune condition has been positively associated with height in men (but not in women) up to a height of 185 cm, but negatively in taller men80. Furthermore, height is uncorrelated with components of innate and adaptive immune system functioning, such as lysozyme activity, neutrophil function, IgA and IgG81. One possible explanation for this disparity is that, while participants of the former study80 lacked any previous exposure to the hepatitis B virus, participants of the latter study81 were previously exposed to flu and tetanus.

Finally, in relation to sex differences, women reported lower average health than men in all communities, which is concordant with reports and normative SF-36 data in other populations, especially in younger people82,83. These results could consolidate the idea that height is a reliable signal of health in men35, while it could reflect reproductive success in women84 in terms of labour and birth, and to a lesser extent, function as an indicator of health85. It has been seen that taller women experience fewer problems during the labour process due to a lower risk of mismatch between foetal head size and size of the birth canal85. Nevertheless, this speculative idea warrants further studies on comparing health, reproductive success and female height.

It is important to consider that the mode of survey administration may be another limitation in our study, and it could have led to confounding effects. For example, it is possible that indigenous people have different understanding and thresholds about their general health perception, which we were unable to evaluate without previous validation of translated items, and it could have explained the lowest values of general health reported by indigenous people. Nevertheless, it could also reflect the real health conditions in Me’Phaa communities and not a misunderstanding of the survey. Other national indicators of health, such as morbidity and mortality by gastrointestinal and nasopharyngeal infectious diseases, have reported that Me’Phaa communities also present the poorest health in Mexico52, which is consistent with our results. In fact, items for the dimension of general health perception have the lowest standard deviation and coefficient of variation in the entire SF-36 survey, in both validated Spanish55,58 and English versions86, which makes this dimension the most understandable one.

In addition, in order to consider obvious differences in language and perception of health, statistical models in this study assumed these inter-population variations a priori. The effects of the sample were considered in all performed LMs. We found that although samples differ considerably, the associations between height, waist circumference and self-perceptions of health were predicted to be in the same direction for all populations (i.e. not interacting with the sample).

Finally, we did not have any information regarding potential pregnancy history in women. This is important because each pregnancy can affect waist circumference, so future studies should collect and control or include this variable in all fitted models.

The present study contributes information that could be important in the framework of human sexual selection. If health and genetic quality cues play an important role in human mate-choice87, and height and waist interact to signal health, its evolutionary consequences, including cognitive and behavioural effects, should be addressed in future research. This could be done by studying the interaction between waist circumference and height, in relation to reproductive and/or mating success, as well as mate preferences and perceived attractiveness, in populations with both Westernised and non-Westernised lifestyles.