Introduction

Stochastic resonance (SR) is a phenomenon resulting from the effect of a random or unpredictable interference (“noise” hereafter) on information processing in nonlinear threshold systems. Noise added in optimal amounts enhances the information transfer and improves the detection of sub-threshold signals; further increases in the amount of noise degrade the signal-to-noise ratio and reduce signal detectability; the system is said to resonate at a particular noise level1,2,3,4,5,6,7.

SR phenomena are fully coded in mathematical terms, occur in both biological and artificial systems, and have proved conspicuously compatible with theoretical models of neural systems, experimental neuroscience and sensory processing across many levels of neuronal organization1,2,3,4,5,6,7. Nonlinearity is a common characteristic of neurons and neural networks. Noise is ubiquitous in the nervous system8,9,10. It originates e.g. from fluctuations in the neurotransmitter release, number of activated postsynaptic receptors, ion concentrations, membrane conductance, effects of previous action potentials, etc. Synaptic transmission is non-stationary, nonlinear and noisy because of the varying contributions from depolarizing and hyperpolarizing currents. Synaptic noise affects relatively simple neuronal systems and small amounts of synaptic noise from dendritic synapses improve the response to independent, sub-threshold synaptic stimuli in agreement with the SR theory10,11,12,13,14,15,16,17,18,19,20.

Investigation on SR has been extensive in physiology, neuroscience, and medical science. SR phenomena are documented in a variety of processes ranging in complexity from neuronal membrane properties to neural coding to higher brain functions such as behavior and sensory processing5,11,21. In humans, SR paradigms enhance the sensitivity to weak visual signals and improve visual processing in the normally sighted. Optimally added random noise transfers undetectable images above threshold in the perception of sub-threshold gratings, ambiguous figures or letters, in the three-dimensional perception of autostereograms, and in binocular rivalry and improves the discrimination of motion directions4,5,6,22,23,24,25,26,27,28,29,30. In a magnetoencephalographic (MEG) study, the improvement in the recognition rate of meaningfull words when gaussian noise was added in proper amounts in a SR paradigm was paralleled by increased activation (with reduced latencies) of the response neuronal sources in visual cortices29.

Evidence of any effect of signal-noise interaction in optimazing visual processing in the visually impaired is still lacking; the purpose of this study was to verify in a pilot test whether a SR phenomenon can be induced in these subjects by applying experimental paradigms comparable to those validated in the normally sighted4,5,6,22,23,24,25,26,27,28.

Material and Methods

Subjects

Fourteen subjects (age range: 22–53 yrs.; 9 females) with severe visual impairment due to retinitis pigmentosa (RP; 7 subjects) or disorders of other etiologies (degenerative myopia in 3 cases, optic atrophy in 3, one macular degeneration) were admitted to the study. Exclusion criteria were concurrent neurological or systemic disorders, disabilities other than visual, treatment with (neuro)active drugs, communication problems, or poor collaboration. Demographics, clinical conditions and residual visual acuity (Snellen) and field are summarized in Table 1. All subjects were informed in full detail about the recording procedures and gave their consent. The ethical principles of the Declaration of Helsinki (1964) by the World Medical Association concerning human experimentation were followed.

Table 1 Summary demographics and clinical conditions.

Visual Stimuli

The paradigm validated in previous studies on sighted volunteers4,5,6,22,23,24,25,26,28,29 was replicated with a reduced number of noise levels to adapt to the patients’ conditions and collaboration. Most SR studies on healthy volunteers have applied an artificial contrast threshold to set a lower limit to the subjects’ sensitivity at small contrast differences4,5,6,23,24,25,26,27,28,29. In this study, the subjects’ disabled vision was treated as equivalent to a higher threshold than normal31. The visual paradigm was individually modeled as the threshold each pixel needed to cross to become distinguishable from the background. Stimuli were in 200 × 200 pixel squares, each one containing an alphabetic character (C, D, H, K, N, O, R, S, V, Z) with the Sloan typeset commonly used in the Pelli-Robson test32; pair of similar letters, such as O-C, H-N, or R-K, were never presented in close sequence to avoid uncontrolled ambiguity or possible guessing. The background gray scale value (GSV) was set at 127 to be halfway between extremes (0–255); the foreground (letter) GSV ranged from 129 to 145 depending on the subject’s contrast sensitivity. Purpose of this limitation was to take advantage of the most linear portion of the range and to obtain only images with bright letters against a dark background, while avoiding saturation and changes in luminance depending on, and correlated with, the intensity of added noise. The grey level distribution of the frames composing the final videos was controlled by means of imageJ built-in grey histogram functions and the contrast was kept constant across noise levels. A set of 1000 images was generated for each letter by independently adding zero-mean Gaussian noise to each pixel, at each contrast and noise intensity level. Images were saved in a PNG format in order to avoid image distortion or impaird quality and subsequently used to create a video at 20 fps, thus obtaining dynamic noise. For each contrast level, videos were made for each of the ten letters and six noise levels differing by their gray level sigma (s1 = 6, s2 = 12, s3 = 18, s4 = 30, s5 = 60, s6 = 90) (example in Fig. 1).

Figure 1
figure 1

Examples of stimuli without (first left) and with added noise at the indicated intensity levels. Only the central letters were reported by subjects.

Experimental Paradigm

Sets of letters were displayed by a VLC media player on a Mitsubishi Diamond monitor for visual electrophysiology testing, with contrast and mean luminance automatically calibrated by way of a photometric system (mean luminance during the test: 30 cd·m−2). For each subject, the first contrast level below threshold, as defined in agreement with the Pelli-Robson test criteria32, or the lower contrast level allowed for the subject with the available gray range was identified. For this contrast level, sets of letters were displayed (on the same monitor and in comparable conditions) in sequence without or with noise added (6 levels of noise). Images with different noise levels were presented in random sequences to normalize for the effects over time of adaptation, learning processes, changing attention, or fatigue. The ten-letters set was presented twice to each subject for a total of 20 letters per noise level and subjects were instructed to report the letter presented on the screen. Each letter remained displayed long enough to comply with the subject’s adaptation to the task and allow recognition, but to a maximum of 50 seconds; multiple guessing were not permitted. The letter was then removed and a 1-min rest was allowed between presentations. The distance at which the subject could comfortably read the best contrasted letters was defined at the beginning of the experimental session and remained unchanged during the test session (70 to 85 cm; central 7.0° of monitor). The room was dimly illuminated.

Three subjects accepted to repeat the experimental session after a 2-wks. interval and are compared in Fig. 2.

Figure 2
figure 2

Fraction of recognized letters versus added noise (sigma); TOP: individual curves. The first [dots] and second [squares) sessions of the three subjects who repeated the test are compared in inset. BOTTOM left: mean across subiects and SE. BOTTOM right: average curves and SE of subjects with peak improvement at sigmas 6 (n = 3), 12 (n = 5), 18 (n = 3) or 30 (n = 3) shown separately. The chance level is indicated. In all cases, the curves of the recognition rate vs added noise level were assimilable to a SR phenomenon.

Results

Under the stimulus conditions, the fraction of recognized letters ranged among subjects between 0 and 0.3 at baseline (with zero noise); it increased in all subjects when noise was added. Maximum recognition ranged between 0.2 and 0.8 at noise levels between 6 and 30 sigmas. The average Χ2 was 13.183; the improvement observed at the optimal noise levels was significant at the p < 0.05 level compared to baseline in 10 subjects when tested individually, at the p = 0.90 level in one, and not significant in three subjects (Table 2). Pooling together all subjects at all noise levels resulted in a Χ2 of 24.3111 (p = 8.1965e-07). The recognition rate decreased in all subjects at noise levels above 30 sigmas (Fig. 2). There was an inverse trend between the recognition rate at baseline and the difference between baseline and the peak values (R2 = 0.5604) due to the larger improvement in the subjects with lower recognition level at baseline; individual differences and a possible ceiling effect are to be investigated in larger subject samples (Fig. 3). The optimal noise level was not predicted by the subject’s contrast sensitivity at the Pelli-Robson testing. No correlation with age, sex, residual visual field, or time from diagnosis of the disease was observed. The subjects with RP (n = 7) did not differ from those with other retinal disorders by improvement in the recognition rate (0.36 ± 0.19 and 0.42 ± 0.25, respectively) or optimal noise level. The fraction of recognized letters vs. added noise featured a SR-like curve also in the second experimental sessions of the three subjects who volunteered to be re-tested; the individual curves of each subjects were comparable (Fig. 2).

Table 2 Χ2 computed for each subject’s maximum recognition fraction versus baseline.
Figure 3
figure 3

difference between baseline and the peak increase in recognition versus baseline.

The mean luminance of letters and background was kept constant during the experimental session and across noise levels. For each threshold level, the numbers of pixel crossing threshold were predicted from the portion of the tails of a Gaussian distribution centered on the gray area (either letter or background) that would fall beyond this threshold. The numbers of pixels crossing the perceptive threshold in the letters area (true positives) or in the background (false positives) were estimated for each threshold level. The fractions of true and false positives both increased with sigma for any fixed threshold, eventually reaching a plateau at sigma around 50; the improvement in letter recognition was to a substantial extent accounted for by their difference. In the example shown in Fig. 4, the difference reaches a maximum at a noise level with sigma = 12 in agreement with the subjects’ improvement in the fraction of recognize letters and the requirements for a SR phenomenon.

Figure 4
figure 4

TOP: example of true (green) and false (blue) positives, i.e. pixels crossing threshold in the areas of letters and in the background, respectively. BOTTOM: estimated numbers of pixels crossing threshold in the letters or background areas vs. the noise level; the difference (thick black) is consistent with a SR model function with a peak effect at sigma = 12.

Discussion

Gaussian noise added in optimal amounts increased the fraction of recognized subtreshold letters by subjects with disorders causing severe visual impairment. The effect proved a function of the noise level1,2,3,4,5,6 and depended on the higher percentage of pixels crossing threshold in the area of displayed letters than in the background. This statistical condition is assimilable to a stochastic resonance phenomenon, with peak effects at noise levels compatible with those that have proven optimal in normally sighted subjects4,5,6,22,23,24,25,26,27,28.

Additional research by functional neuroimaging or electrophysiological/MEG techniques is needed to replicate previous observations in healthy subjects29 and correlate psychophysics to the neuronal mechanims possibly underlying (residual) vision in the visually impaired. The mechanims serving visual information processing are nonetheless known to depend in several instances on signal/noise interaction consistent with the SR paradigm. The sensitivity of the retina bipolar ON cells is enhanced in vertebrates by sub-threshold, otherwise undetectable light stimulation; this effect is mediated by cGMP-activated channels and facilitating feedback mechanisms that transfer signals above background33. Neuronal membrane noise promotes spiking and contributes to the contrast invariance of orientation tuning in V1 34. Also consistent with a SR paradigm are the processes improving detection of weak signals in the context-dependent response of activated cortical cells34. Noise-induced linearization in the visual system is thought to result from neuronal membrane characteristics associated with weak modulation of membrane voltage or to originate from low-pass phenomena34,35,36. SR promoted accuracy and efficiency in online brain-control tasks by decreasing the contributions from the threshold non-linearity and increased coherence37. SR phenomena have been observed in a variety of processes ranging in complexity from neuronal membrane properties to to higher brain functions5,11,21. A role of the (spared) neuroretina cannot be excluded in our subjects but remains inferential, while the retinal or optic nerve damage suggests the SR facilitation to occur at brain level. A suggested neurophysiological mechanism is increased synchronization that SR may mediate in, or result of neural (phase) synchronization within the visual system29,32,36,37,38,39,40. In this regard, our findings are congruent with the identification of the visual cortices as the locus of SR phenomena in binocular and multisensory study paradigms30,41,42,43,44 and with studies making use of transcranial magnetic stimulation to inject white noise directly into neuronal processing at cortical level45,46. Images with dynamic Gaussian noise at spatial frequency (>45 cycle/degrees) incompatible with the human contrast sensitivity function activate the visual cortex47. Some general compatibility of visual processing with, or intrinsic adaptation to the SR paradigm seems conceivable; it would be in line with the SR modeling brain functions48,49, as well as with the observation that the individual subject’s contrast sensitivity did not predict the optimal noise level in this study.

Some degree of residual modulation of the sensory input throughout the visual system is also known to be possible in the visually impaired. RP is a progressive dystrophic disorder of retinal photoreceptors that triggers loss of function in these structures while leaving the neuroretina and visual pathways unaffected and with residual capability to process and transfer visual information4,5,6,22,23,24,25,26,27,28,50,51,52,53,54. Higher-level visual function is reportedly possible55 and passive viewing is known to recruit relatively large networks in the occipital and temporal lobes also in the visually impaired adult56. Electrophysiological responses originating from the retinal ganglion cells and the visual cortex have been recorded from subjects with RP severely impairing visual acuity; miscoding in visual information processing has been suggested57.

The improved recognition of alphabet letters in a SR paradigm fits in this scenario. It suggests that adding noise into the visual sensory channel in optimal amounts can result in improved signal transmission and optimized neuronal synchronization at the single neuron level and/or large-scale synchronization of cortical neurons30,58,59 also in subjects with visual disorders. Further, systematic research is mandatory. However, the SR paradigm proved efficient in this study with image manipulations at low level of complexity. SR has been applied in the processing of heavily degraded images60 and appears readily applicable in order to improve efficacy of electronic low vision aids currently used to support the visually impaired and the rehabilitation procedures. Application in developing the retinal prostheses hopefully available in the future is also conceivable61,62,63,64,65,66.