Skip to main content
Log in

Gender differences in giving in the Dictator Game: the role of reluctant altruism

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
Journal of the Economic Science Association Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

The Dictator Game has been a helpful tool to study whether men or women are more generous. But recent work suggests that motivations other than generosity also influence behavior in this game. Image concerns and expectations management may cause dictators to “give reluctantly”; that is, to share money with the recipient if asked to, but to renege on their gifts if they can do so without being detected. We provide evidence from two separate experiments that females are more likely than males to give reluctantly in the Dictator Game. After accounting for retraction of gifts, males and females transfer similar amounts to the recipient in expectation. The results suggest that gender differences in non-payoff-related motivations may play a role in producing gender differences in giving in the Dictator Game.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Endowments are fairly similar in purchasing power across locations: at the time of sessions in Santiago, 5000 CLP exchange for approximately 8 USD.

  2. Our design is inspired by Dana et al. (2006), who run a Dictator Game with dictators and recipients sitting in separate rooms. After deciding how to allocate $10, dictators face a binary choice between implementing their allocation, or taking $9 and leaving $0 for the recipient while also leaving the recipient unaware that the game was played. In our design, we ask dictators to choose a retraction probability between 10 and 90 percent, rather than giving them a binary choice, to obtain a more granular measure of preferences over retraction, and to be able to implement the retraction quietly while dictators and recipients sitting in the same room. This also implies that instructions are common knowledge at all times in our experiment.

  3. Of these, two male subjects participated twice in Santiago. We dropped their second participation (two observations) from the data.

  4. This is not to say that transferring 0 ECU and then selecting a retraction probability larger than 10 percent is a mistake or noise in the data. This behavior can also be rationalized by non-payoff-related motivations, such that the dictator is willing to give up 1 ECU to hide from the recipient the fact that they acted selfishly in the Dictator Game, or that the dictator is engaging in “moral cleansing” or “conscience accounting” after having acted selfishly in the Dictator Game (Sachdeva et al. 2009; Gneezy et al. 2014).

  5. Thus, we fail to replicate Broberg et al.’s (2007) finding that subjects who make larger donations in the Dictator Game are more prone to exit. In fact, participants who transfer 5 or more ECU in our Dictator Game are 13 percentage points less likely to select a probability larger than 10 percent than participants who transfer 2–4 ECU (p = 0.003, from regressions analogous to Table 3b with the pooled data). This result seems to be more in line with findings of within-subject positive correlation in prosocial behavior across tasks, such as in Dariel and Nikiforakis (2014).

  6. Although Niederle (2016) suggests a different interpretation to the findings in the literature.

References

  • Andreoni, J., & Bernheim, D. (2009). Social image and the 50–50 norm: A theoretical and experimental analysis of audience effects. Econometrica, 77(5), 1607–1636.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Andreoni, J., & Vesterlund, L. (2001). Which is the fair sex: Gender differences in altruism. Quarterly Journal of Economics, 116(1), 293–312.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bardsley, N. (2008). Dictator game giving: Altruism or artefact? Experimental Economics, 11(2), 122–133.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bertrand, M. (2011). New perspectives on gender. In Handbook of labor economics (Vol. 4, pp. 1543–1590). Amsterdam: Elsevier.

    Google Scholar 

  • Broberg, T., Ellingsen, T., & Johannesson, M. (2007). Is generosity involuntary? Economics Letters, 94(1), 32–37.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cain, D. M., Dana, J., & Newman, G. E. (2014). Giving versus giving in. The Academy of Management Annals, 8(1), 505–533.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Camerer, C. F. (2003). Behavioral game theory: Experiments in strategic interactions. Princeton: Princeton University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Camerer, C. F., & Thaler, R. H. (1995). Anomalies: Ultimatums, dictators, and manners. Journal of Economic Perspectives, 9(2), 209–219.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chen, D. L., Schonger, M., & Wickens, C. (2016). oTree: An open-source platform for laboratory, online, and field experiments. Journal of Behavioral and Experimental Finance, 9(1), 88–97.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Croson, R., & Gneezy, U. (2009). Gender differences in preferences. Journal of Economic Literature, 47(2), 448–474.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dana, J., Cain, D. M., & Dawes, R. M. (2006). What you don’t know won’t hurt me: Costly (but Quiet) exit in dictator games. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 100(2), 193–201.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dariel, A., & Nikiforakis, N. (2014). Cooperators and reciprocators: A within-subject analysis of prosocial behavior. Economics Letters, 122(2), 163–166.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • De Wit, A., & Bekkers, R. (2016). Exploring gender differences in charitable giving. Nonprofit and Voluntary Sector Quarterly, 45(4), 741–761.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • DellaVigna, S., List, J. A., Malmendier, U., & Rao, G. (2013). The importance of being marginal: Gender differences in generosity. American Economic Review: Papers and Proceedings, 103(3), 586–590.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Exley, C. L. (2018). Incentives for prosocial behavior: The role of reputations. Management Science., 64, 2460–2471. (forthcoming).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fehr, E., & Schmidt, K. M. (1999). A theory of fairness, competition, and cooperation. Quarterly Journal of Economics, 114(3), 817–868.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fischbacher, U. (2007). Z-Tree: Zurich toolbox for ready-made economic experiments. Experimental Economics, 10(2), 171–178.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Forsythe, R., Horowitz, J. L., Savin, N. E., & Sefton, M. (1994). Fairness in simple bargaining experiments. Games and Economic Behavior, 6(3), 347–369.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gill, D., & Prowse, V. (2012). A structural analysis of disappointment aversion in a real effort competition. American Economic Review, 102(1), 469–503.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gino, F., Norton, M. I., & Weber, R. A. (2016). Motivated Bayesians: Feeling moral while acting egoistically. Journal of Economic Perspectives, 30(3), 189–212.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gneezy, U., Imas, A., & Madarász, K. (2014). Conscience accounting: Emotion dynamics and social behavior. Management Science, 60(11), 2645–2658.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Greiner, B. (2015). Subject pool recruitment procedures: organizing experiments with ORSEE. Journal of the Economic Science Association, 1(1), 114–125.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jones, D., & Linardi, S. (2014). Wallflowers: Experimental evidence of an aversion to standing out. Management Science, 60(7), 1757–1771.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Klinowski, D. (2016). Reluctant donors and their reactions to social information. Working Paper.

  • Lazear, E. P., Malmendier, U., & Weber, R. A. (2012). Sorting in experiments with application to social preferences. American Economic Journal: Applied Economics, 4(1), 136–163.

    Google Scholar 

  • List, J. A. (2007). On the interpretation of giving in dictator games. Journal of Political Economy, 115(3), 482–493.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Niederle, M. (2016). Gender. In Handbook of experimental economics (Vol. 2, pp. 481–562). Princeton: Princeton University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sachdeva, S., Iliev, R., & Medin, D. L. (2009). Sinning saints and saintly sinners: The paradox of moral self-regulation. Psychological Science, 20(4), 523–528.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to David Klinowski.

Additional information

This research was supported in part by a grant from the Science of Philanthropy Initiative. I also would like to thank two anonymous referees and the editor, Nikos Nikiforakis, for comments that greatly improved this paper.

Electronic supplementary material

Below is the link to the electronic supplementary material.

Supplementary material 1 (DOCX 68662 kb)

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Klinowski, D. Gender differences in giving in the Dictator Game: the role of reluctant altruism. J Econ Sci Assoc 4, 110–122 (2018). https://doi.org/10.1007/s40881-018-0058-1

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Revised:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s40881-018-0058-1

Keywords

JEL codes

Navigation