Skip to main content
Log in

A Parametric Analysis of a Protocol to Induce Bidirectional Naming: Effects of Protocol Intensity

  • ORIGINAL ARTICLE
  • Published:
The Psychological Record Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Researchers have identified protocols to induce verbal behavior cusps and capabilities that put an individual’s repertoires in contact with new contingencies and allows the individual to learn in new ways; however, little is known about the relationship between different intensities (i.e., doses) of the protocols and inducing important repertoires. The Intensive Tact Instruction (ITI) has been found to reliably induce Bidirectional Naming (BiN), a capability whereby an individual is able to function simultaneously as a speaker and a listener, to acquire language incidentally after exposure to word–object relations in the environment. We matched eight participants into four dyads and participants in dyads experienced different intensities of the ITI protocol. The full intensity protocol included 100 additional tact learning opportunities each day and the accelerated intensity included 50 daily learning opportunities. We found varied results dependent on a participant’s degree of BiN at the onset of the intervention. Participants with Unidirectional Naming (UniN) acquired BiN under both intensities, but participants in the accelerated condition did so with fewer learning opportunities, suggesting the efficiency of the accelerated protocol. Students that demonstrated No-incidental Naming (NiN) demonstrated mixed results from the intervention, regardless of intervention intensity. The outcomes are discussed in terms of efficiency and cost effectiveness.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Albers, A, & Greer, R. D. (1991). Is the three-term contingency trial a predictor of effective instruction? Journal of Behavioral Education, 1, 337–354.

  • Arntzen, E. (2004). Probability of equivalence formation: Familiar stimuli and training sequence. The Psychological Record, 54, 275–291.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bijou, S. W., & Baer, D. M. (1961). The Century psychology series. Child development, Vol. 1. A systematic and empirical theory. East Norwalk, CT: Appleton-Century-Crofts.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brand, D., Henley, A. J., DiGennaro Reed, F. D., Gray, E., & Crabbs, B. (2019). A review of published studies involving parametric manipulations of treatment integrity. Journal of Behavioral Education, 1, 1–26.

    Google Scholar 

  • Codding, S. R., & Lane, K. L. (2015). A spotlight on treatment intensity: An important and often overlooked component of intervention inquiry. Journal of Behavioral Education, 24, 1–10.

    Google Scholar 

  • Costa, A., & Pelaez, M. (2014). Implementing intensive tact instruction to increase frequency of spontaneous mands and tacts in typically developing children. Behavioral Development Bulletin, 19, 19–24.

    Google Scholar 

  • Delgado, J. A. P., & Oblak, M. (2007). The effects of daily intensive tact instruction on the emission of pure mands and tacts in non-instructional settings by three preschool children with developmental delays. Journal of Early & Intensive Behavior Intervention, 4, 392–411.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dinsmoor, J. (1983). Observing and conditioned reinforcement. The Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 6, 693–704.

    Google Scholar 

  • Du, L., Broto, J., & Greer, R. D. (2015). The effects of establishment of conditioned reinforcement for observing responses for 3D stimuli on generalized visual match-to-sample in children with autism spectrum disorders. European Journal of Behavior Analysis, 16, 82–98.

    Google Scholar 

  • Du, L., & Greer, R. D. (2014). Validation of adult generalized imitation topographies and the emergence of generalized imitation in young children with autism as a function of mirror training. The Psychological Record, 64, 161–177.

    Google Scholar 

  • Erjavec, M., & Horne, P. J. (2008). Determinants of imitation of hand-to-body gestures in 2- and 3-year old children. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 89, 183–207.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Fiorile, C. A., & Greer, R. D. (2007). The induction of Naming in children with no echoic-to-tact responses as a function of multiple exemplar instruction. Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 23, 71–88.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fryling, M. J., Johnston, C., & Hayes, L. J. (2011). Understanding observational learning: an interbehavioral approach. Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 27(1), 191–203.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fuller, J. L., & Fienup, D. M. (2018). A preliminary analysis of mastery criterion level: Effects on response maintenance. Behavior Analysis in Practice, 11, 1–8.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Greer, R. D. (2002). Designing teaching strategies: An applied behavior analysis system approach. San Diego, CA: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Greer, R. D. (2004). Opinion: The two subject matters of behavior analysis and early intervention. Journal of Early & Intensive Behavior Intervention, 1, 239–245.

    Google Scholar 

  • Greer, R. D., & Du, L. (2010). Generic instruction versus intensive tact instruction and the emission of spontaneous speech. Journal of Speech & Language Pathology—Applied Behavior Analysis, 5, 1–19.

    Google Scholar 

  • Greer, R. D., Dudek-Singer, J., & Gautreaux, G. (2006). Observational learning. International Journal of Psychology, 41, 486–499.

    Google Scholar 

  • Greer, R. D., & Han, H. S. A. (2015). Establishment of conditioned reinforcement for visual observing and the emergence of generalized visual-identity matching. Behavioral Development Bulletin, 20, 227–252.

    Google Scholar 

  • Greer, R. D., & Longano, J. (2010). A rose by naming: how we may learn how to do it. Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 26, 73–106.

    Google Scholar 

  • Greer, R. D., Pistoljevic, N., Cahill, C., & Du, L. (2011). Effects of conditioning voices as reinforcers for listener responses on rate of learning, awareness, and preference for listening to stories in preschoolers with autism. Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 27, 103–124.

    Google Scholar 

  • Greer, R. D., Pohl, P., Du, L., & Moschella, J. L. (2017). The separate development of children’s listener and speaker behavior and the intercept as behavioral metamorphosis. Journal of Behavioral & Brain Science, 7, 674–704.

    Google Scholar 

  • Greer, R. D., & Ross, D. E. (2008). Verbal behavior analysis: Inducing and expanding new verbal capabilities in children with language delays. Boston, MA: Person Education.

    Google Scholar 

  • Greer, R. D., & Speckman, J. (2009). The integration of speaker and listener responses: A theory of verbal development. The Psychological Record, 59, 449–488.

    Google Scholar 

  • Greer, R. D., Stolfi, L., Chavez-Brown, M., & Rivera-Valdez, C. (2005). The emergence of the listener to speaker component of naming in children as a function of multiple exemplar instruction. Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 21, 123–134.

    Google Scholar 

  • Haq, S. S., Kodak, T., Kurtz-Nelson, E., Porritt, M., Rush, K., & Cariveau, T. (2015). Comparing the effects of massed and distributed practice on skill acquisition for children with autism: Comparison of massed and distributed practice. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 48, 454–459.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hart, B., & Risley, T. R. (1995). Meaningful differences in the everyday experience of young American children. Baltimore, MD: Paul H. Brookes.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hawkins, E., Gautreaux, G., & Chiesa, M. (2018). Deconstructing common bidirectional naming: A proposed classification framework. Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 34, 44–61.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hawkins, E., Kingsdorf, S., Charnock, J., Szabo, M., & Gautreauz, G. (2009). Effects of multiple exemplar instruction on naming. European Journal of Behavior Analysis, 10, 95–103.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hayes, S. C., Rincover, A., & Solnick, J. V. (1980). The technical drift of applied behavior analysis. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 13, 275–285.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Horne, P. J., & Lowe, C. F. (1996). On the origins of naming and other symbolic behavior. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 65, 185–241.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Ingham, P., & Greer, R. D. (1992). Changes in student and teacher responses in observed and generalized settings as a function of supervisor observations. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 25, 153–164.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Jennings, A. M., & Miguel, C. F. (2017). Training intraverbal bidirectional naming to establish generalized equivalence class performances. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 108, 269–289.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Johnston, J. M., & Pennypacker, H. S. (1993). Strategies and tactics for human behavioral research (2nd ed.). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Keohane, D., & Greer, R. D. (2005). Teachers’ use of a verbally governed algorithm and student learning. International Journal of Behavioral Consultation and Therapy, 3, 252–271.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lotfizadeh, A. D., Kazemi, E., Pompa-Craven, P., & Eldevik, S. (2018). Moderate effects of low-intensity behavioral intervention. Behavioral Interventions. Advanced Online Publication. doi:https://doi.org/10.1177/0145445518796204

  • Lovaas, O. I. (1987). Behavioral treatment and normal educational and intellectual functioning in young autistic children. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 55, 3–9.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lydon, H., Healy, O., Leader, G., & Keohane, D.-D. (2009). The effects of intensive tact instruction on three verbal operants in non-instructional settings for two children with autism. Journal of Speech & Language Pathology—Applied Behavior Analysis, 3, 173–184.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ma, M. L., Miguel, C. F., & Jennings, A. M. (2016). Training intraverbal naming to establish equivalence class performances. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 105, 409–426.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Majdalany, L., Wilder, D. A., Smeltz, L., & Lipschultz, J. L. (2016). The effects of brief delays to reinforcement on the acquisition of tacts in children with autism. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 49, 411–415.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Miguel, C. F. (2016). Common and intraverbal bidirectional naming. Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 32, 125–138.

    Google Scholar 

  • Miguel, C. F., & Petursdottir, A. I. (2009). Naming and frames of coordination. In R. A. Rehfeldt & Y. Barnes-Holmes (Eds.), Derived relational responding: Applications for learners with autism and other developmental disabilities (pp. 129–148). Oakland, CA: New Harbinger.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pistoljevic, N., & Greer, R. D. (2006). The effects of daily intensive tact instruction on preschool students’ Emission of pure tacts and mands in non-instructional setting. Journal of Early & Intensive Behavior Intervention, 1, 103–120.

    Google Scholar 

  • Poulson, C. L., & Kymissis, E. (1988). Generalized imitation in infants. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 46, 324–336.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rosales-Ruiz, J., & Baer, D. M. (1997). Behavioral cusps: A developmental and pragmatic concept for behavior analysis. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 30, 533–544.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Ross, D. E., Singer-Dudek, J., & Greer, R. D. (2005). The teacher performance rate and accuracy scale: Training as evaluation. Education and Training in Developmental Disabilities, 40, 411–423.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schmelzkopf, J., Greer, R. D., Singer-Dudek, J., & Du, L. (2017). Experiences that establish preschoolers’ interest in speaking and listening to others. Behavioral Development Bulletin, 22, 44–66.

    Google Scholar 

  • Silverman, K., Anderson, S. R., Marshall, A. M., & Baer, D. M. (1986). Establishing and generalizing audience control of new language repertoires. Analysis and Intervention in Developmental Disabilities, 6, 21–40.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stromer, R., & Mackay, H. A. (1996). Naming and formation of stimulus classes. In T. R. Zentall & P. M. Smeets (Eds.), Stimulus class formation in humans and animals (pp. 221–252). Amsterdam, The Netherlands: Elsevier.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sy, J. R., & Borrero, J. C. (2009). Parametric analysis of presession exposure to edible and nonedible stimuli. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 42, 833–837. https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.2009.42-833.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Taylor, B. A., & DeQuinzio, J. A. (2012). Observational learning and children with autism. Behavior Modification, 3, 341–360.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Availability of Data and Materials

Data and materials can be obtained by contacting the corresponding author.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Daniel M. Fienup.

Ethics declarations

The authors declare that s/he has no conflict of interests. This study was approved by the Institutional Review Board of the respective university. We obtained parent permission for each participant described in this manuscript.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

This article is based on a dissertation submitted by Rebecca M. Hotchkiss as partial fulfillment of the requirement for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy in Applied Behavior Analysis at Teachers College, Columbia University. We thank Dr. R. Douglas Greer, Dr. Laudan Jahromi, Dr. Kathleen O’Connell, and Dr. Anna Petursdottir for their support of this research and suggestions on an earlier version of this manuscript. We also thank Genevieve Karanian and Michaela Dunham for their assistance in conducting this research.

Appendix

Appendix

ITI Intervention Data

Fig. 4
figure 4

Detailed correct responses to the ITI intervention for Dyad 1 across 10-learn unit (Participant A1) or 20-learn unit (Participant A2) instructional sessions, per data path (category)

Fig. 5
figure 5

Detailed correct responses to the ITI intervention for Dyad 2 across 10-learn unit (Participant A2) or 20-learn unit (Participant F2) sessions, per data path (category).

Fig. 6
figure 6

Detailed correct responses to the ITI intervention for Dyad 3 across 10-learn unit (Participant A3) or 20-learn unit (Participant F3) instructional sessions, per data path (category).

Fig. 7
figure 7

Detailed correct responses to the ITI intervention for Dyad 4 across 10-learn unit (Participant A4) or 20-learn unit (Participant F4) instructional sessions, per data path (category).

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Hotchkiss, R.M., Fienup, D.M. A Parametric Analysis of a Protocol to Induce Bidirectional Naming: Effects of Protocol Intensity. Psychol Rec 70, 481–497 (2020). https://doi.org/10.1007/s40732-020-00383-5

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s40732-020-00383-5

Keywords

Navigation