Skip to main content
Log in

The Simple Memory Span Experiment: A Behavioral Analysis

  • Theoretical Article
  • Published:
The Psychological Record Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

The purpose of this article is to present a behavioral theory of performance in the simple (classic) memory span experiment. This experiment has been used to investigate hypothetical mental and neurological processes and intelligent function, but behavioral span theory takes the standpoint of event-based constructs. Established empirical limits on memory span are described in terms of span ability, namely, the ability to function under span load. Span load is a count of the number of discriminative stimuli that function concurrently to occasion responding. Span ability is assessed via standard memory span procedures. The theory distinguishes response string from response chain on the basis of the locus of stimulus control; the response string is controlled by the multiple stimuli in a stimulus string or stimulus complex. Response chains are emitted intraverbals. There is no known limit to the length of a chain that can be attained through extensive experiential history, but the memory span experiment assesses the length of response strings that are severely limited at around 7 items (the span limit) and are quite resistant to change. Span limits increase during the developmental period and covary with intelligent function. The values of span ability and span load are potent tools for the prediction and control of developmental and individual differences in diverse experimental paradigms drawn from both behavior analytic and general experimental traditions.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Ackerman, P. L., Beier, M. E., & Boyle, M. O. (2005). Working memory and intelligence: The same or different constructs? Psychological Bulletin, 131, 30–60. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.131.1.30.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Asch, S. E. (1968). The doctrinal tyranny of associationism: Or what is wrong with rote learning. In T. R. Dixon & D. L. Horton (Eds.), Verbal behavior and general behavior theory (pp. 214–228). Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Atkinson, R. C., & Shiffrin, R. M. (1968). Human memory: A proposed system and its control processes. In K. W. Spence & J. T. Spence (Eds.), The psychology of learning and motivation: Advances in research and theory (Vol. 2, pp. 89–195). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bachelder, B. L. (1970). The memory span paradigm: Its use for the analysis of mental retardation. (Unpublished doctoral dissertation). Michigan State University, East Lansing, MI.

  • Bachelder, B. L., & Denny, M. R. (1977a). A theory of intelligence: I. Span and the complexity of stimulus control. Intelligence, 1, 127–150. doi:10.1016/0160-2896(77)90001-0.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bachelder, B. L., & Denny, M. R. (1977b). A theory of intelligence: II. The role of span in a variety of intellectual tasks. Intelligence, 1, 237–256. doi:10.1016/0160-2896(77)90008-3.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baddeley, A. D. (1976). The psychology of memory. New York, NY: Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bartlett, F. C. (1932). Remembering: A study in experimental and social psychology. London, England: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bijou, S. W. (1966). A functional analysis of retarded development. International Review of Research in Mental Retardation, 1, 1–19. doi:10.1016/S0074-7750(08)60159-7.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Binet, A., & Simon, T. (1916). The development of intelligence in children (The Binet-Simon Scale) (E. S. Kite, Trans.). Baltimore, MD: Williams & Wilkins. (Original work published 1905)

  • Blankenship, A. B. (1938). Memory span: A review of the literature. Psychological Bulletin, 35, 1–25.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bolton, T. L. (1892). The growth of memory in school children. American Journal of Psychology, 4, 362–380.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Boswell, S. L., Sanders, B., & Young, S. J. (1972). The effects of exposure duration and practice on the immediate memory spans of children and adults. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 17, 167–176.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Broadbent, D. E. (1958). Perception and communication. New York, NY: Pergamon.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Brown, J. (1958). Some tests of the decay theory of immediate memory. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 10, 12–21.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chase, W. G., & Ericsson, K. A. (1982). Skill and working memory. In G. H. Bower (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation (Vol. 16, pp. 1–58). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Conway, A. R. A., Cowan, N., Bunting, M. F., Therriault, D. J., & Minkoff, S. R. B. (2002). A latent variable analysis of working memory capacity, short-term memory capacity, processing speed, and general fluid intelligence. Intelligence, 30, 163–184. doi:10.1016/S0160-2896(01)00096-4.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Conway, A. R. A., & Engle, R. W. (1996). Individual differences in working memory capacity: More evidence for a general capacity theory. Memory, 4, 577–590.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Conway, A. R. A., Kane, M. J., Bunting, M. F., Hambrick, D. Z., Wilhelm, O., & Engle, R. W. (2005). Working memory span tasks: A methodological review and user’s guide. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 12, 769–786.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cowan, N. (2001). The magical number 4 in short-term memory: A reconsideration of mental storage capacity. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 24, 87–185. doi:10.1017/S0140525X01003922.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cowan, N. (2010). The magical mystery four: How is working memory capacity limited, and why? Psychological Science, 19, 51–57. doi:10.1177/0963721409359277.

    Google Scholar 

  • Crowder, R. G. (1982). The demise of short-term memory. Acta Psychologica, 5, 291–323.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Crowder, R. G. (1993). Short-term memory: where do we stand? Memory & Cognition, 21, 142–145.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Davelaar, E. J., Goshen-Gottstein, Y., Ashkenazi, A., Haarmann, H. J., & Usher, M. (2005). The demise of short-term memory revisited: Empirical and computational investigations of recency effects. Psychological Review, 112, 3–42. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.112.1.3.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Delprato, D. J. (1986). Response patterns. In H. W. Reese & L. J. Parrott (Eds.), Behavior science: Philosophical, methodological, and empirical advances (pp. 61–113). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dempster, F. N. (1981). Memory span: sources of individual and developmental differences. Psychological Bulletin, 89, 63–100. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.89.1.63.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dixon, T. R., & Horton, D. L. (Eds.). (1968). Verbal behavior and general behavior theory. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ebbinghaus, H. (1885/1964). Memory: A contribution to experimental psychology. New York, NY: Teachers College, Columbia University.

  • Engle, R. W., Tuholski, S. W., Laughlin, J. E., & Conway, A. R. A. (1999). Working memory, short-term memory, and general fluid intelligence: A latent-variable approach. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 128, 309–331.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ericsson, K. A., & Chase, W. G. (1982). Exceptional memory. American Scientist, 70, 607–615.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Estes, W. K. (1974). Learning theory and intelligence. American Psychologist, 29, 740–749. doi:10.1037/h0037458.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Galton, F. (1887). Supplementary notes on “prehension” in idiots. Mind, 12, 79–82.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gertner, J. (2012). The idea factory: Bell Labs and the great age of American innovation. New York, NY: Penguin.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gignac, G. E. (2015). The magical numbers 7 and 4 are resistant to the Flynn effect: No evidence for increases in forward or backward recall across 85 years of data. Intelligence, 48, 85–95. doi:10.1016/j.intell.2014.11.001.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gignac, G. E., & Weiss, L. G. (2015). Digit span is (mostly) related linearly to general intelligence: Every extra bit of span counts. Psychological Assessment, 27, 1312–1323. doi:10.1037/pas0000105.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gray, S., Green, S., Alt, M., Hogan, T., Kuo, T., Brinkley, S., & Cowan, N. (2017). The structure of working memory in children and its relation to intelligence. Journal of Memory and Language, 92, 183–201. doi:10.1016/j.jml.2016.06.004.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Guilford, J. P., & Dallenbach, K. M. (1925). The determination of memory span by the method of constant stimuli. American Journal of Psychology, 36, 621–628. doi:10.2307/1413916.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Holmes, O. W. (1871). Mechanism in thought and morals. Boston, MA: James R. Osgood.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Humphreys, L. G. (1994). Intelligence from the standpoint of a (pragmatic) behaviorist. Psychological Inquiry, 5, 179–192. doi:10.1207/s15327965pli0503_1.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jacobs, J. (1887). Experiments on “prehension.”. Mind, 12, 75–79.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jensen, A. R. (1970). A theory of primary and secondary familial mental retardation. In N. R. Ellis (Ed.), International review of research in mental (pp. 33–105). New York, NY: Academic Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Kantor, J. R. (1924). Principles of psychology (Vol. 1). New York, NY: Alfred A. Knopf.

  • Kantor, J. R. (1959). Interbehavioral psychology. Granville, OH: Principia Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lyon, D. R. (1977). Individual differences in immediate serial recall: a matter of mnemonics? Cognitive Psychology, 9, 403–411.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mahadevan, R., Malone, J. C., & Bailey, J. (2002). Radical behaviorism and exceptional memory phenomena. Behavior and Philosophy, 30, 1–13.

    Google Scholar 

  • Melton, A. W. (1963). Implications of short-term memory for a general theory of memory. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 2, 1–21.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Michael, J., Palmer, D. C., & Sundberg, M. L. (2011). The multiple control of verbal behavior. Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 27, 3–22.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Miller, G. A. (1956). The magical number seven, plus or minus two: some limits on our capacity for processing information. Psychological Review, 63, 81–97.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Neath, I., & Surprenant, A. M. (2003). Human memory (2nd ed.). Belmont, CA: Wadsworth.

    Google Scholar 

  • Peterson, L. R., & Peterson, M. J. (1959). Short-term retention of individual verbal items. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 58, 193–198.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ryan, J. J., Lopez, S. J., & Paolo, A. M. (1996). Digit span performance of persons 75–96 years of age: Base rates and associations with selected demographic variables. Psychological Assessment, 8, 324–327. doi:10.1037/1040-3590.8.3.324.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F. (1938). The behavior of organisms. New York, NY: Appleton-Century-Crofts.

    Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F. (1957). Verbal behavior. New York, NY: Appleton-Century-Crofts.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Staats, A. W. (1996). Behavior and personality: Psychological behaviorism. New York, NY: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Takahashi, M., Shimizu, H., Saito, S., & Tomoyori, H. (2006). One percent ability and ninety-nine percent perspiration: A study of a Japanese memorist. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 32, 1195–1200. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.32.5.1195.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tillman, C. M. (2011). Developmental change in the relation between simple and complex spans: A meta-analysis. Developmental Psychology, 47, 1012–1025. doi:10.1037/a0021794.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Unsworth, N., & Engel, R. W. (2007). The nature of individual differences in working memory capacity: Active maintenance in primary memory and controlled search from secondary memory. Psychological Review, 114, 104–132. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.114.1.104.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Winch, W. H. (1904). Immediate memory in school children. British Journal of Psychology, 1, 127–134.

    Google Scholar 

  • Woods, D. L., Herron, T. J., Yund, E. W., Hink, R. F., Kishiyama, M. M., & Reed, B. (2011). Computerized analysis of error patterns in digit span recall. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 33, 721–734. doi:10.1080/13803395.2010.550602.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Dennis J. Delprato.

Ethics declarations

Funding

Preparation of the manuscript was self-funded.

Conflict of Interest

There is no conflict of interest.

Ethical Approval

All span testing procedures were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional research committees and with the 1964 Helsinki declaration and its later amendments. Informed consent was obtained for all children and adults and participation was always voluntary.

Additional information

The final version of this paper was prepared by Dennis J. Delprato after Dr. Bachelder’s death.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Bachelder, B.L., Delprato, D.J. The Simple Memory Span Experiment: A Behavioral Analysis. Psychol Rec 67, 423–433 (2017). https://doi.org/10.1007/s40732-017-0222-7

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s40732-017-0222-7

Keywords

Navigation