Skip to main content
Log in

Spelling Instruction by Stimulus Pairing in Japanese Students with Autism Spectrum Disorders: Effects of Stimulus Presentation Order

  • Original Article
  • Published:
The Psychological Record Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Students with autism spectrum disorder (ASD) often have difficulties in learning stimulus relations in spelling. Using the two-stimulus pairing procedure, we examined the emergence of stimulus relations between Japanese and English words by comparing the spelling performance of five students with ASD with that of five typically developing students. In the Japanese-English pairing procedure, a Japanese word was presented first, followed by its English translation, and in the English-Japanese pairing, an English word was presented first, followed by its Japanese translation. Training effects were evaluated with a sign test and analysis of variance. All the students correctly spelled the English words in both procedures. The Japanese-English pairing procedure required fewer training blocks than the English-Japanese pairing procedure. In the Japanese-English pairing, students with ASD required fewer training blocks than typically developing students. These results suggest that presenting already established words (i.e., Japanese) first might better facilitate the emergence of stimulus relations in a stimulus pairing procedure.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • American Psychiatric Association. (2000). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (4th ed., text rev). Washington, DC: Author

  • Clayton, M., & Hayes, L. (2004). A comparison of match-to-sample and respondent-type training of equivalence classes. The Psychological Record, 54, 579–602. Retrieved from http://opensiuc.lib.siu.edu/tpr/vol54/iss4/6/.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, J. (1968). Weighted kappa: Nominal scale agreement with provisions for scale disagreement or partial credit. Psychological Bulletin, 70, 313–320. doi:10.1037/h0026256.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fields, L., Reeve, K. F., Rosen, D., Varelas, A., Adams, B. J., Belanich, J., et al. (1997). Using the simultaneous protocol to study equivalence class formation: The facilitating effects of nodal number and size of previously established equivalence classes. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 67, 367–389. doi:10.1901/jeab. 1997.67-367.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hayashi, Y., & Vaidya, M. (2008). Effects of stimulus discriminability on the acquisition of conditional discriminations in adult humans. European Journal of Behavior Analysis, 9, 173–183. Retrieved from http://www.ejoba.org/PDF/2008_2/Hayashi_Vaidya_2008.pdf.

    Google Scholar 

  • Holth, P., & Arntzen, E. (1998). Stimulus familiarity and the delayed emergence of stimulus equivalence or consistent nonequivalence. The Psychological Record, 48, 81–110. Retrieved from http://opensiuc.lib.siu.edu/tpr/vol48/iss1/6/.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kazdin, A. E. (2011). Single-case research designs: Methods for clinical and applied settings (2nd ed.). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Leader, G., & Barnes-Holmes, D. (2001). Matching-to-sample and respondent-type training as methods for producing equivalence relations: Isolating the critical variable. The Psychological Record, 51, 429–444. Retrieved from http://opensiuc.lib.siu.edu/tpr/vol51/iss3/5/.

    Google Scholar 

  • O’Connor, J., Rafferty, A., Barnes-Holmes, D., & Barnes-Holmes, Y. (2009). The role of verbal behavior, stimulus nameability, and familiarity on the equivalence performances of autistic and normally developing children. The Psychological Record, 59, 53–74. Retrieved from http://opensiuc.lib.siu.edu/tpr/vol59/iss1/4/.

    Google Scholar 

  • Omori, M., & Yamamoto, J. (2013a). Stimulus pairing training for Kanji reading skills in students with developmental disabilities. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 34, 1109–1118. doi:10.1016/j.ridd.2012.12.016.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Omori, M., & Yamamoto, J. (2013b). Sequential stimulus pairing procedure for the students with intellectual disabilities. Psychology, 4, 238–245. Retrieved from http://dx.doi.org/10.4236/psych.2013.43A036.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Petursdottir, A. I., & Hafliđadóttir, R. D. (2009). A comparison of four strategies for teaching small foreign-language vocabulary. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 42, 685–690. doi:10.1901/jaba. 2009.42-685.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rayner, K. (2009). Eye movements and attention in reading, scene perception, and visual search. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 62, 1457–1506. doi:10.1080/17470210902816461.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rosales, R., Rehfeldt, R. A., & Huffman, N. (2012). Examining the utility of the stimulus pairing observation procedure with preschool children learning a second language. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 45, 173–177. doi:10.1901/jaba. 2012.45-173.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Serna, R. W., Dube, W. V., & McIlvane, W. J. (1997). Assessing same/different judgments in individuals with severe intellectual disabilities: A status report. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 18, 343–368. doi:10.1016/S0891-4222(97)00015-2.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sherer, M., Pierce, K. L., Paredes, S., Kisacky, K. L., Ingersoll, B., & Schreibman, L. (2001). Enhancing conversation skills in children with autism via video technology: Which is better, “self” or “other” as a model? Behavior Modification, 25, 140–158. doi:10.1177/0145445501251008.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sidman, M. (2000). Equivalence relations and the reinforcement contingency. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 74, 127–146. doi:10.1901/jeab. 2000.74-127.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Smyth, S., Barnes-Holmes, D., & Forsyth, J. P. (2006). A derived transfer of simple discrimination and self-reported arousal functions in spider fearful and non-spider fearful participants. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 85, 223–246. doi:10.1901/jeab. 2006.02-05.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Smyth, S., Barnes-Holmes, D., & Barnes-Holmes, Y. (2008). Acquired equivalence in human discrimination learning: The role of propositional knowledge. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Animal Behavior Processes, 34, 167–177. doi:10.1037/0097-7403.34.1.167.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Stromer, R., Mackay, H. A., Howell, S. R., McVay, A. A., & Flusser, D. (1996). Teaching computer-based spelling to individuals with developmental and hearing disabilities: Transfer of stimulus control to writing tasks. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 29, 25–42. doi:10.1901/jaba. 1996.29-25.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Takahashi, K., Yamamoto, J., & Noro, F. (2011). Stimulus pairing training in children with autism spectrum disorder. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 5, 547–553. doi:10.1016/j.rasd.2010.06.021.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tonneau, F., & González, C. (2004). Function transfer in human operant experiments: The role of stimulus pairings. Journal of Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 81, 239–255. doi:10.1901/jeab. 2004.81-239.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wang, L., Mottron, L., Peng, D., Berthiaume, C., & Dawson, M. (2007). Local bias and local-to-global interference without global deficit: A robust finding in autism under various conditions of attention, exposure time, and visual angle. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 24, 550–574. doi:10.1080/13546800701417096.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wechsler, D. (1998). Wechsler Intelligence Scales for Children (3rd ed.). San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wolf, M., & Denckla, M. B. (2005). RAN/RAS: Rapid automatized naming and rapid alternating stimulus tests. Austin, TX: Pro-Ed.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

This research was supported in part by grants from the Japan Society for the Promotion of Science and the Research Center for Thinking and Behavioral Judgment Proceedings at Keio University. The Keio University Institutional Review Board of the Faculty of Letters approved this study.

Author Note

Mikimasa Omori, Department of Psychology, Graduate School of Human Relations, Keio University; Jun-ichi Yamamoto, Department of Psychology, Faculty of Letter, Keio University.

Mikimasa Omori is a Research Fellow of the Japan Society for the Promotion of Science and the Department of Developmental Disorders, National Institute of Mental Health, National Center of Neurology and Psychiatry.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Mikimasa Omori.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Omori, M., Yamamoto, Ji. Spelling Instruction by Stimulus Pairing in Japanese Students with Autism Spectrum Disorders: Effects of Stimulus Presentation Order. Psychol Rec 65, 401–410 (2015). https://doi.org/10.1007/s40732-014-0114-z

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s40732-014-0114-z

Keywords

Navigation