Introduction

Habitat loss is one of the main causes of biodiversity loss around the world as humans continue to modify and destroy natural habitats (Rands et al. 2010). Approximately a third of the world’s forests have been cleared, with around 5 million ha cleared annually (Ritchie and Roser 2021). Habitat loss generally causes fragmentation, whereby habitat becomes divided into smaller and more isolated fragments surrounded by a matrix of human-modified land. Species within these habitat fragments are typically vulnerable to isolation and exposed to anthropogenic impacts (Lindenmayer and Fischer 2013).

Mammalian predators are particularly susceptible to habitat fragmentation as they often require relatively large home ranges, occur at naturally low population densities, and have specialized niche requirements (Crooks et al. 2011; Gittleman et al. 2001). Globally, native predator populations are declining as a result of the associated impacts of habitat loss and fragmentation (Chapron et al. 2014; Crooks et al. 2011; Crooks 2002) and subsequent increased conflict with other predators and humans (Farris et al. 2015; Remonti et al. 2012; Ripple et al. 2014). For example, habitat fragmentation has altered habitat use by long-tailed weasels (Mustela frenata) in America (Gehring and Swihart 2003, 2004) as well as the güiña (Leopardus guigna) in Chile (Schüttler et al. 2017). As landscapes continue to be modified and degraded, survival of predators will depend on their ability to persist in human-modified landscapes (Kremen and Merenlender 2018). Some native predator species are able to persist within or even benefit from modified habitats, particularly where resource availability is increased or supplemented (Bateman and Fleming 2012; Crooks 2002; Swihart et al. 2003). For example, pine martens (Martes martes) can exist in agricultural landscapes in Europe (Mergey et al. 2012; Pereboom et al. 2008; Weber et al. 2018) utilizing supplemental prey and den resources (Caryl et al. 2012). Similarly, raccoons (Procyon lotor) in North America are well adapted to human-modified landscapes, utilizing croplands and urban habitats for foraging and denning (Beasley et al. 2007b; Henner et al. 2004).

The spotted-tailed quoll (Dasyurus maculatus, hereafter referred to as “quoll”) is a medium-sized (1.2–4.2 kg, Belcher 2003) marsupial carnivore which occurs throughout eastern Australia, including the island state of Tasmania (Jones et al. 2001). It is known to occur in a range of habitats including wet and dry sclerophyll forests, woodlands, and rainforests (Belcher and Darrant 2004; Claridge et al. 2005; Glen and Dickman 2006b). Quolls are generally considered a forest-dependent species (Belcher 2004), typically associated with structurally complex contiguous forests that offer an abundance of hollow-bearing trees, logs, and burrows, which provide suitable den sites and support high densities of prey (Belcher and Darrant 2006; Claridge et al. 2005; Glen et al. 2011).

Currently, quolls are listed as “near threatened” on the IUCN Red List of Threatened Species, and Australia’s mainland population is listed as endangered under the Commonwealth Environment Protection and Biodiversity Conservation Act 1999. Since European colonization in the late eighteenth century, quolls have suffered a 50–90% decline in their range (Maxwell et al. 1996), with habitat loss being considered the greatest threat to quoll survival (Long and Nelson 2010). Additionally, introduced predators such as feral cats (Felis catus) and red foxes (Vulpes vulpes) threaten quolls through resource competition and predation (Jones et al. 2014). Foxes, in particular, are known to exhibit strong dietary overlap with quolls (Glen and Dickman 2008; Glen et al. 2011), and there is some evidence of interspecific killing of quolls by foxes (Körtner et al. 2004). In southeastern mainland Australia, potential competitive interactions between sympatric quolls and foxes (Henderson et al., 2021) may exacerbate the impacts of habitat loss on quoll populations in fragmented landscapes, potentially restricting quolls to more protected intact forested habitats.

Recent studies in Tasmania (Andersen et al. 2017; Hamer et al. 2021; Jones et al. 2021), as well as in New South Wales (Henderson et al. 2022; Henderson et al. 2021), indicate that quolls are able to persist in fragmented landscapes. However, information on how quolls use these fragmented landscapes is limited to research in Tasmania. Troy (2014) suggested that habitat preferences of quolls in Tasmania are more flexible than in mainland Australia likely due to the absence of interspecific competition with foxes. In mainland Australia, quoll research has predominately been conducted in contiguous forested landscapes. Consequently, their dependence on intact forests is only assumed and could instead reflect sampling bias. Henderson et al. (2021) recently found that quolls and foxes coexisted spatially and temporally within a fragmented landscape in mainland Australia, which was possibly facilitated by low fox density. Therefore, mainland quolls living in some fragmented landscapes could demonstrate similar habitat flexibility to quolls in Tasmania, although this is currently unknown.

In our study, we investigated the habitat use by quolls in mainland Australia in a landscape comprising forested habitat fragments interspersed with open grassland habitat. By identifying essential habitat features to be retained and protected at the landscape level, information on how quolls use fragmented landscapes can help inform relevant conservation and management strategies. We specifically sought to determine whether quoll habitat use within a fragmented landscape was similar across four broad vegetation types (open grassland, grassy woodland, dry sclerophyll forest, and wet sclerophyll forest) or whether quolls demonstrated a preference for forested vegetation types and avoided open grassland. We hypothesized that quolls would prefer dry and wet sclerophyll forests, as mainland quoll occurrence is typically associated with these forest types (Belcher 2004; Catling et al. 2002; McLean et al. 2015) and avoid open grasslands due to the increased risk of interspecific competition and exposure to predators.

Materials and methods

Study site and design

The study was conducted in a biodiversity offset area and adjacent private land (approximately 30 km2) located in the Hunter Valley region of New South Wales, Australia (Fig. 1). The study site was defined by private property and biodiversity site boundaries, and comprised a mix of sclerophyll forests and grassy eucalypt woodlands fragmented by mining and agriculture, and interspersed with open grassland paddocks (Fig. 1). The study site was divided into four broad vegetation types (open grassland, grassy woodland, dry sclerophyll forest, and wet sclerophyll forest; Table 1). The proportion of each vegetation type available within the study site was calculated using ArcGIS version 10.4.1 (ESRI 2015). The study site was then divided into 2 km2 hexagonal cells (with some cells excluded due to access constraints), resulting in 42 accessible cells. Each accessible cell was systematically assigned a vegetation type based on the dominant vegetation within that cell while also ensuring that the proportion of cells per vegetation type was similar to the proportion of available vegetation within the study site (Table 1).

Fig. 1
figure 1

Location of 42 camera traps and the associated vegetation types within the study site. Insert shows the location of the site within New South Wales, Australia

Table 1 Description of each vegetation type, its available proportion (%) within the study site, and associated number of camera traps allocated to each vegetation type (total cameras = 42)

Camera trapping

To assess quoll habitat preferences, we deployed Reconyx HC600 camera traps (Reconyx, Holmen, USA) across all accessible sampling units (n = 42, Fig. 1), and camera placement targeted suitable habitat features required for installation (e.g., a fallen log or rock pile). Each camera was positioned ~ 1.0 m above the ground and attached to a metal post, which faced a large horizontal log (or an equivalent rock pile) located between 1.5 and 3.0 m away from the camera. A lure canister containing ~ 500 g of raw chicken necks was pegged into the ground in front of the log. Cameras were aligned so that the camera’s upper detection zone covered the top of the log and the lower detection zone covered the lure canister as described in Henderson et al. (2021). Camera traps were deployed continuously from May to December 2020, and were serviced twice (August and November) to replace camera batteries, SD cards, and lures. The study was separated into three survey periods (May–June, August–September, and November–December), with each period commencing on the day that camera traps were serviced and lures refreshed, thus ensuring that the potential effect of declining lure age on quoll detectability remained consistent across survey periods (Henderson et al. 2022). To ensure temporal independence between survey periods and account for declining lure age, only the first 28 camera trap nights were included in data analysis.

Data analysis

All statistical analyses were performed in R version 4.2.0 (R Core Team 2022).

Naive occupancy

Raw detection data was used to determine naive occupancy for each vegetation type in each survey period. Naive occupancy expresses the proportion of camera sites where at least one quoll was detected, stratified by vegetation type. As not all the cameras operated for the full four weeks (due to battery depletion or camera malfunction), we only included detection data from cameras that were operational for at least 2 weeks.

Quoll activity across vegetation types

For each survey period, we used quoll activity (the number of independent quoll detections) in each vegetation type to infer habitat preference. Consecutive quoll detections on the same camera were considered independent detection events if image sequences were separated by more than 10 min, as described in Henderson et al. (2021). We used a chi-square goodness-of-fit test to compare the observed and expected a number of independent quoll detections across the four vegetation types. We then calculated the number of available camera nights for each vegetation type as a proportion of the total available camera nights across all vegetation types to determine the expected number of detections for each vegetation type. Available camera trap nights varied slightly between survey periods due to battery depletion or camera malfunction. Habitat preference was inferred where quolls were detected in vegetation types more frequently than expected.

Probability of occupancy across vegetation types

As raw detection data does not account for imperfect detection (MacKenzie et al. 2002), we also sought to infer habitat preference during each survey period using the probability of occupancy for each vegetation type. Occupancy probability was estimated using the single-species, single-season occupancy model in the “unmarked” package version 1.1.0 (Fiske and Chandler 2011). We created camera-specific detection histories by determining quoll presence (1) or absence (0) for each camera night using the package “camtrapR” version 2.0.3 (Niedballa et al. 2016). We defined a sampling “occasion” as a one-night period (24-h duration beginning at 12:00). Nights when cameras were inoperable (due to battery depletion or camera malfunction) for part or all of the 24-h period were recorded as “NA” in the detection history. We included vegetation type as a covariate on occupancy and lure age as a covariate on detection probability to account for the potential impact of lure efficiency over time (Henderson et al. 2022).

Temporal activity across vegetation types

To determine if habitat use differed temporally, we compared quoll temporal activity across vegetation types for each survey using the “overlap” package Version 0.3.2 (Ridout and Linkie 2009). To determine the extent of temporal activity overlap, we calculated the coefficient of overlap (Δ) whereby values range from 0 (no overlap) to 1 (complete overlap). Following Meredith and Ridout (2018), we used the Δ1 estimator because all sample sizes had < 5 detection events. We obtained 95% confidence intervals using 10,000 smoothed bootstrap samples (Rovero and Zimmermann 2016). We then used nonparametric kernel density curves to plot temporal activity profiles and tested for differences in temporal activity peaks using the nonparametric Mardia–Watson–Wheeler test for homogeneity within the ‘circular’ package Version 0.4–93 (Lund et al. 2017).

Results

Quoll activity across vegetation types

We recorded a total of 267 independent quoll detections from 3384 camera nights across the three survey periods. There was clear habitat flexibility, with quolls detected in all four vegetation types during each of the three survey periods. Naïve occupancy remained consistent between the May–June and August–September survey periods for all vegetation types, ranging from 50% in open grassland up to 89% in grassy woodland (Table 2a). However, naïve occupancy was low across all vegetation types during November–December, ranging from 20% in open grassland and up to 50% in both grassy woodland and wet sclerophyll forest (Table 2a).

Table 2 Summary of spotted-tailed quoll (Dasyurus maculatus): (a) naive occupancy and (b) observed and expected independent detections with percentage difference for each vegetation type during each survey

Quoll activity differed significantly between vegetation types in all the three survey periods (all P < 0.05), although different habitats were preferred in different survey periods. During May–June, quolls were detected in dry sclerophyll forest more frequently than expected (X2 = 16.61, df = 3, P < 0.01) while grassy woodlands were preferred in both the August–September (X2 = 8.28, df = 3, P < 0.05) and November–December (X2 = 18.82, df = 3, P < 0.01) survey periods (Table 2b). In contrast, quolls were detected in wet sclerophyll forest less frequently than expected during both the May–June and November–December surveys while use of open grasslands was lower than expected in both the August–September and November–December survey periods (Table 2b).

Probability of occupancy across vegetation types

The probability of quoll occupancy did not reveal any significant difference in habitat use across vegetation types or between surveys as indicated by wide, overlapping confidence intervals (Fig. 2).

Fig. 2
figure 2

Probability of occupancy of spotted-tailed quolls (Dasyurus maculatus) for each vegetation type (OG, open grassland; GW, grassy woodland; DS, dry sclerophyll; WS, wet sclerophyll) during a May–June 2020, b August–September 2020, and c November–December 2020. Error bars show 95% confidence intervals

Temporal activity

There were no significant differences in activity peaks between each of the three forested vegetation types (grassy woodland, dry sclerophyll, and wet sclerophyll) for each survey (all P > 0.05). Therefore, we subsequently pooled these three vegetation types into a combined “Forest” category to compare temporal activity across the vegetation types. The quolls were primarily nocturnal (1800–0600 h) and exhibited a similar temporal activity between forest and open grassland during both May–June (Δ1 = 0.74, 95% CI: 0.53–0.83, Fig. 3a) and August–September (Δ1 = 0.83, 95% CI: 0.73–0.97, Fig. 3b). The nonparametric Mardia–Watson–Wheeler tests also showed no significant difference in activity peaks between the forest and open grassland habitats for both May–June (W = 4.30, P = 0.12) and August–September (W = 2.61, P = 0.27). The November–December survey was not compared due to the low number of quoll detections (n = 5) in the open grassland.

Fig. 3
figure 3

Spotted–tailed quoll (Dasyurus maculatus) temporal activity overlap between forest (solid line) and open grassland (dashed line) for a May–June 2020 and b August–September 2020. The shaded areas denote the extent of temporal activity overlap across vegetation types

Discussion

Our study is the first to investigate habitat use by quolls in a fragmented landscape in mainland Australia. The raw detection data revealed some seasonal variations in habitat preferences, although quolls generally avoided open grassland and wet sclerophyll forest in preference for grassy woodland and dry sclerophyll forest. In contrast, occupancy modelling revealed broad habitat flexibility by quolls, with no specific habitat preference. Furthermore, quoll temporal activity remained similar between vegetation types and survey periods. Our results suggest that quoll habitat use in mainland Australia is likely more flexible than previously assumed and highlights the potential value of human-modified landscapes for quoll conservation. However, the conflicting results also highlight the inherent difficulties of reliably determining the habitat preference of cryptic carnivorous species such as quolls.

Our findings are consistent with those reported by Troy (2014) in Tasmania where quoll habitat use was flexible within fragmented agricultural landscapes. Troy (2014) suggested that an absence of foxes in Tasmania potentially explains the wide habitat tolerances of quolls observed in their study. In contrast, the quoll research in mainland Australia has predominately been conducted in intact forested landscapes, leading to an established notion that quolls are dependent on large, complex contiguous forests (Belcher 2000; Belcher and Darrant 2006; Mansergh 1983) because of the threat posed by foxes in more open landscapes (Long and Nelson 2010). However, Henderson et al. (2021) found that the presence of foxes at our study site did not appear to influence quoll activity, possibly due to low fox density. The broad habitat use by quolls observed in our current study possibly reflects a flexibility in habitat use under low fox densities, similar to the flexibility observed in the absence of foxes (Troy 2014). This suggests that quoll habitat use where foxes are absent or rare (e.g., in Tasmania and in our current study) might reflect the species’ preferred niche, while higher fox densities might restrict quoll habitat use to a realized niche of intact contiguous forest where the probability of survival is increased (Troy 2014). Further research is required to compare quoll habitat use in fragmented landscapes under different fox densities.

The observed habitat flexibility of quolls in our study might reflect prey availability across vegetation types as suggested by studies on other mesocarnivores in fragmented landscapes. For example, pine martens can benefit from supplemental food resources in fragmented habitats, and this can likely facilitate the tolerance of habitat loss up to a certain threshold (Caryl et al. 2012; Mortelliti and Boitani 2008). Similarly, forest-dependent leopard cats are able to utilize open agricultural plantations in Southeast Asia (Chua et al. 2016) due to the better prey catchability of abundant rodent pests (Lorica and Heaney 2013; Rajaratnam et al. 2007; Silmi et al. 2013). This could be similar for quolls in the open grassland, because the abundant prey species such as hares (Lepus europaeus) and northern brown bandicoots (Isoodon macrourus) (Glen and Dickman 2006a; Jarman et al. 2007) at our site were not limited to forested habitats but were also frequently detected on cameras in the open grassland where the catchability for these species might be greater than in forested areas. Further research is needed to investigate whether prey availability and catchability influence the habitat flexibility of quolls within fragmented landscapes.

The avoidance of open grasslands and preference for forest fragments were also consistent with observations of quolls in Tasmania (Troy 2014). Troy (2014) found a positive correlation between female quoll home range size and the proportion of pasture, and a negative correlation between home range size and eucalypt forests, suggesting that pastures contain insufficient resources to meet quoll resource demands while forests provide disproportionately more resources. In our study, the habitat preference might also reflect the resource availability for quolls within each vegetation type with the open grassland providing insufficient resources to induce preference. Within forested vegetation types, the strong preference for grassy woodland was surprising considering quolls in mainland Australia are typically associated with structurally complex contiguous forests such as sclerophyll forests or rainforests (Belcher 2000; Belcher and Darrant 2006; Mansergh 1983). At our site, the grassy woodlands support a mix of semi-arboreal prey such as brush-tailed possums (Trichosurus vulpecula) as well as ground-dwelling grassland species such as bandicoots and hares while also providing hollow-bearing trees or logs for shelter. These grassy woodlands could potentially act as a “buffer zone” between open grasslands and resource-rich sclerophyll forests to facilitate quoll foraging and movement while maintaining a low risk from predation. In contrast, quoll detections were less common in the wet sclerophyll forest, which includes a more structurally complex vegetation offering greater protective cover from other predators. It is possible that prey availability is lower in wet sclerophyll forest as it may provide prey with more cover from quolls compared to more open forests. Therefore, grassy woodlands and dry sclerophyll forests may provide a balance of sufficient den sites, cover from other predators, and increased functional availability of prey. However, further research is needed to understand the specific features that influence quoll preference within these vegetation types.

The observed differences in habitat preference between the survey periods suggest that seasonal variations in habitat use might be associated with the quoll's reproductive cycle. For example, the May–June survey coincides with the quoll’s annual breeding season (Belcher 2003) when transient males roam across large areas in search of females (Belcher and Darrant 2004). This possibly explains why quolls were detected more frequently than expected in the open grassland during this survey compared to the other survey periods when the open grassland was avoided. The aversion for the open grassland during August–September could reflect the need for females to remain within the protection of forested habitats, where they are less vulnerable to predation while they are carrying pouch young during this time of the year (Körtner et al. 2019). A similar seasonal variation in habitat use has been observed for raccoons in Canada, which are known to forage in open agricultural environments but prefer forested habitats when rearing young (Beasley et al. 2007a). In addition, the November–December survey period coincides with juvenile quolls initially emerging from their natal dens and becoming independent (Andrew 2005), potentially avoiding the open grassland where they are more exposed to other predators. Surprisingly, quolls did not prefer wet sclerophyll forest, which is likely to provide more cover from other predators. In fact, quoll activity was relatively lower in wet sclerophyll in both the May–June and November–December survey periods compared to the other forested vegetation types. This may instead reflect seasonal variations in prey availability or catchability within wet sclerophyll forests. Future studies should investigate quoll habitat use during different times of the year and across multiple years to determine if seasonal variation in habitat use is consistent with our findings and hypotheses.

The broad habitat use of quolls and a strong preference for forest fragments suggest that quolls are likely traversing open grasslands to access these forest fragments. In Tasmania, quolls used small, isolated vegetation patches as “stepping stones” between larger forest patches (Troy 2014). Our study site similarly comprised many small clusters of paddock trees or isolated exotic shrubs, which potentially act as refugia within the open grasslands that stretch between the larger forested vegetation types. These vegetation patches may be important for facilitating connectivity within the landscape and provide additional resources for quolls such as den sites or prey. Further research is needed to ascertain fine-scale movements of quolls throughout this fragmented landscape to understand the importance of these isolated vegetation patches so that conservation strategies can ensure the protection of critical vegetation within the fragmented matrix.

Our occupancy modelling analysis was unable to discern any significant difference in habitat use across vegetation types. Estimates derived using an occupancy modelling approach are generally considered more robust as they account for imperfect detection data (MacKenzie et al. 2006). Accordingly, it is possible that our occupancy modelling estimates accurately reflect the habitat use by quolls and that there is actually no habitat preference for the quolls at this site. The observed preference inferred from raw detection data might simply reflect a detection bias as a result of imperfect detection, although this is uncertain. Conversely, the large and overlapping confidence intervals around our occupancy estimates could suggest the study design or sample size was inadequate to determine any habitat preference using occupancy, which is typically a data-hungry analysis (Jha et al. 2022). This could be due to the cryptic nature of quolls, whereby the probability of occupancy may be high, but the probability of detection is very low, resulting in imprecise estimates. Greatly increasing the number of sampling occasions can better inform the probability of detection (MacKenzie 2005). Additionally, the precision of occupancy estimates can be improved by increasing the number of sites (MacKenzie et al. 2006). Such studies may, however, be limited by financial or logistical constraints. Therefore, a trade-off between sufficient spatial replicates and sampling occasions is needed to achieve a practical survey effort. This highlights the potential limitation of using occupancy to study low-density and wide-ranging carnivores such as quolls, which may require a much greater survey effort than what is feasible.

Information on quoll habitat use in fragmented landscapes can inform appropriate conservation and management strategies by identifying important habitat to be retained and protected at the landscape level. The quolls in our study exhibited a strong preference for grassy woodlands and dry sclerophyll forests with varying degrees of preference at different times of the year. The quolls also demonstrated greater flexibility in habitat use than previously assumed in mainland Australia, indicating some adaptability to fragmented landscapes. However, due to the limitations of the analyses used, our interpretations should be treated cautiously. Nevertheless, the ability for quolls to persist in this fragmented landscape highlights our incomplete understanding of the importance of fragmented habitat for endangered predator species such as quolls. While habitat loss is often considered the greatest threat to biodiversity loss (Rands et al. 2010), the extent to which habitat fragmentation affects biodiversity remains contentious (Fahrig 2017; Fletcher et al. 2018). Quolls are generalist carnivores and may indeed benefit from habitat fragmentation, provided other larger carnivores such as foxes remain absent or at low densities. Future research should investigate specific factors that might influence quoll habitat preference such as predator densities, prey availability, and seasonal behavior. In addition, future quoll research should replicate our study in other fragmented landscapes and attempt to incorporate greater survey effort to ensure adequate data is obtained to infer habitat preferences. Continued research will improve our understanding of quoll ecology outside of contiguous forested areas and assist with quoll conservation in fragmented landscapes.