Skip to main content
Log in

Perception in Practice

  • Published:
Review of Philosophy and Psychology Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

A study of culturally-embedded perceptual responses to aesthetic value indicates that learned perceptual capacities can secure compliance with social norms. We should therefore resist the temptation to draw a line between cognitive processes, such as perception, that can adapt to differences in physical environments, and cognitive processes, such as economic decision-making, that are shaped by social norms. Compliance with social norms is a result of perceptual learning when that same compliance modifies perceptible features of the physical environment.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. “Euro” denotes Europe and its descendant cultures.

References

  • Ball, Karlene and Robert Sekuler. 1982. A Specific and enduring improvement in visual motion discrimination. Science 218. 4573: 697–698.

  • Baxandall, Michael. 1972. Painting and experience in fifteenth-century Italy: A Primer in the social history of pictorial style. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Becker, Howard S. 2008. Art worlds. 2nd ed. Berkeley and Los Angeles: University of California Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Benovsky, Jiri. 2012. Aesthetic supervenience vs aesthetic grounding. Estetika 49 (2): 166–178.

    Google Scholar 

  • Berlin, Brent. 1992. Ethnobiological classification: Principles of categorization of plants and animals in traditional societies. Princeton: Princeton University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bicchieri, Cristina. 2006. The Grammar of society: The Nature and dynamics of social norms. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bullot, Nicolas, and Rolf Reber. 2013. The Artful mind meets art history: Toward a psycho-historical framework for the science of art appreciation. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 36 (2): 123–137.

    Google Scholar 

  • Burnston, Daniel. 2017. Cognitive penetration and the cognition–perception interface. Synthese 194 (9): 3645–3668.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carbon, Claus-Christian. 2011. Cognitive mechanisms for explaining dynamics of aesthetic appreciation. i-Perception 2: 708–719.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dougherty, Janet W. D. 1978. Salience and relativity in classification. American Ethnologist 5 (1): 66–80.

    Google Scholar 

  • Epstein, Brian. 2015. The Ant trap: Rebuilding the foundations of the social sciences. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Farkas, András. 2002. Prototypicality-effect in surrealist paintings. Empirical Studies of the Arts 20 (2): 127–136.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fechner, Gustav. 1876. Vorschule der ästhetik. Leipzig: Breitkopf und Härtel.

    Google Scholar 

  • Folstein, Jonathan R., Isabel Gauthier, and Thomas J. Palmeri. 2010. Mere exposure alters category learning of novel objects. Frontiers in Psychology 1 (40): 1–6.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fodor, Jerry A. 1983. The Modularity of mind. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gauthier, Isabel, Michael J. Tarr, and Daniel Bub, eds. 2010. Perceptual expertise: Bridging brain and behavior. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goldstone, Robert L. 1998. Perceptual learning. Annual Review of Psychology 49 (1): 585–612.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gombrich, E.H. 1960. Art and illusion: A Study in the psychology of pictorial representation. Princeton: Princeton University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Guala, Francesco. 2016. Understanding institutions: The Science and philosophy of living together. Princeton: Princeton University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Henrich, Joseph, Steven J. Heine, and Ara Norenzayan. 2010. The Weirdest people in the world? Behavioral and Brain Sciences 33.2–3: 61–83 and 111–123.

  • Holm, Bill, and Willie Seaweed. 1983. Smoky-Top: The Art and times of Willie Seaweed. Seattle: University of Washington Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jonaitis, Aldona. 1999. Northwest Coast totem poles. In Unpacking culture: Art and commodity in colonial and postcolonial worlds, ed. Ruth B. Phillips and Christopher Steiner, 104–121. Berkeley and Los Angeles: University of California Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Khemlani, Sangeet S., N.Y. Louis Lee, and Monica Bucciarellic. 2010. Determinants of cognitive variability. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 33 (2–3): 97–98.

    Google Scholar 

  • Layton, Robert. 2011. Aesthetics: The Approach from social anthropology. In The Aesthetic mind: Philosophy and psychology, ed. Elisabeth Schellekens and Peter Goldie, 208–222. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Leder, Helmut, Benno Belke, Andries Oeberst, and Dorothee Augustin. 2004. A model of aesthetic appreciation and aesthetic judgements. British Journal of Psychology 95 (4): 489–508.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lewis, David. 1969. Convention. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lopes, Dominic McIver. 2014. Beyond art. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lopes, Dominic McIver. 2018. Being for beauty: Aesthetic agency and value. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lopes, Dominic McIver. 2020. Desolation Sound: Social practices of natural beauty. Thought 9 (4): 266–273.

    Google Scholar 

  • Machery, Edouard. 2010. Explaining why experimental behavior varies across cultures: A missing step in “The Weirdest people in the world?”. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 33 (2–3): 101–102.

    Google Scholar 

  • Martindale, Colin, and Kathleen Moore. 1988. Priming, prototypicality, and preference. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 14 (4): 661–670.

    Google Scholar 

  • Matthen, Mohan. 2020. Art forms emerging: An Approach to evaluative diversity in art. Journal of Aesthetics and Art Criticism 78 (3): 303–318.

    Google Scholar 

  • Masuda, Takahiko, Richard Gonzalez, Letty Kwan, and Richard E. Nisbett. 2008. Culture and aesthetic preference: Comparing the attention to context of East Asians and Americans. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 34 (9): 1260–1275.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ransom, Madeleine. 2020a. Attentional weighting in perceptual learning. Journal of Consciousness Studies 27 (7–8): 236–248.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ransom, Madeleine. 2020b. Waltonian perceptualism. Journal of Aesthetics and Art Criticism 78 (1): 66–70.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ransom, Madeleine. forthcoming. Aesthetic perception and the puzzle of training. Synthese.

  • Richerson, Peter J., and Richard Boyd. 2005. Not by genes alone: How culture transformed human evolution. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rochat, Philippe. 2010. What is really wrong with a priori claims of universality? Sampling, validity, process level, and the irresistible drive to reduce. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 33 (2–3): 107–108.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rosch, Eleanor, Carolyn B. Mervis, Wayne D. Gray, David M. Johnson, and Penny Boyes-Braem. 1976. Basic objects in natural categories. Cognitive Psychology 8 (3): 382–439.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rozin, Paul. 2010. The Weirdest people in the world are a harbinger of the future of the world. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 33 (2–3): 108–109.

    Google Scholar 

  • Segall, Marshall H., Donald T. Campbell, and Melville J. Herskovits. 1966. The Influence of culture on visual perception. Indianapolis: Bobbs-Merrill.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sibley, Frank. 1959. Aesthetic concepts. Philosophical Review 68 (4): 421–450.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sibley, Frank. 1965. Aesthetic and nonaesthetic. Philosophical Review 74 (2): 135–159.

    Google Scholar 

  • Siegel, Susanna. 2010. The Contents of visual experience. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stokes, Dustin. 2014. Cognitive penetration and the perception of art. Dialectica 68 (1): 1–34.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tanaka, James W., and Marjorie Taylor. 1991. Object categories and expertise: Is the basic level in the eye of the beholder? Cognitive Psychology 23 (3): 457–482.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vogel, Susan Mullin. 1997. Baule: African art, western eyes. New Haven: Yale University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Walton, Kendall. 1970. Categories of art. Philosophical Review 79 (3): 334–367.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

We thank an anonymous referee and the editors of the special issue for some helpful suggestions.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Dominic McIver Lopes.

Ethics declarations

Conflict of Interest

None.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Lopes, D.M., Ransom, M. Perception in Practice. Rev.Phil.Psych. 14, 387–400 (2023). https://doi.org/10.1007/s13164-022-00634-0

Download citation

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s13164-022-00634-0

Keywords

Navigation