Schools are increasingly embracing social and emotional programs as an essential part of education (Domitrovich et al. 2017; Greenberg et al. 2017). At the same time, the development of mindfulness-based programming directed at improving child well-being has been increasing in popularity as an educational initiative (Semple et al. 2017). Although these approaches are distinct, they share commonalities and can be seen as highly complementary (Feuerborn and Gueldner 2019). There are a number of school-based programs that incorporate mindfulness within a social and emotional learning (SEL) framework, and a growing interest in these approaches to student well-being that may not only be complementary, but have added value (Moreno-Gómez and Cejudo 2019; Schonert-Reichl et al. 2015). Below, we provide a brief overview of SEL programming and mindfulness-based approaches used within schools and then discuss these two complementary approaches as background for this research.

Over the past nearly two decades, there has been a growing recognition that to succeed in the twenty-first century, children need to be taught not only academic skills, but also social and emotional skills. Enhancing social and emotional competencies in children through universal programming within classrooms has been a growing area of school-based program development, and in turn, a significant body of evaluation studies (including meta-analyses) has emerged (Durlak et al. 2011; Sklad et al. 2012). The popularity of these approaches likely stems from several factors. First, many can be integrated into the daily teaching practice of educators (Greenberg et al. 2003; Oberle et al. 2016). Therefore, rather than being an add-on to classroom instruction, they can be integrated into the curriculum. Second, well-implemented programs have been shown to lead to a range of benefits versus single problem behavior targeted programs. Meta-analysis findings revealed that SEL universal programming has a significant positive impact on social and emotional skills, attitudes toward self and others, internalizing and externalizing behaviors, and academic achievement (Durlak et al. 2011). Third, although overall, immediate effects have been found to be stronger than delayed effects, there is evidence that the benefits conferred by these programs can be long-lasting (Taylor et al. 2017).

Mindfulness-informed programs are a more recent addition to school-based programming for children. Despite an explosion in popularity, there are relatively few evaluations, particularly with younger students (Maynard et al. 2017). A systematic review and meta-analysis of school-based programs concluded that such approaches hold promise and may provide benefits in the areas of cognition performance, stress, and resilience (Zenner et al. 2014). The same meta-analysis did not find an overall effect on emotional problems. The authors concluded that the field was still in its infancy and that the variability across studies (i.e., inconsistencies in findings) and small sample sizes precluded clear conclusions. The overall message from Zenner et al. (2014) meta-analysis was a call for larger and more robust intervention studies in this area. In addition, most of the included studies were for children in grades 3 and older, with only one study including students in first grade (Napoli et al. 2005). The study that included younger children found that a 24-week mindfulness intervention program led to benefits in attention among first- to third-grade students (Napoli et al. 2005). Since Zenner et al. (2014) meta-analysis, there has been a growing body of published research, although many of these studies continue to utilize small sample sizes and focus on older children. A subsequent systematic review of mindfulness-based intervention in schools identified only one additional study that extended down to kindergarten-aged students (Felver et al. 2016).

Given the evidence that SEL programming is effective for improving student outcomes, and the growing promise of mindfulness-based interventions in schools to enhance well-being, there is interest in the integration of the two approaches from a theoretical standpoint. First, in terms of potential added benefits to further enhance cognitive, educational, social-emotional, and mental well-being of students, and second, from a practical standpoint; educators do not have to choose between two programs when they can be taught and learned together (Maloney et al. 2016). The similar goals across SEL and mindfulness approaches suggest potential compatibility. Within the neuroscience research on mindfulness meditation, enhanced self-regulation has been proposed as the key outcome in mindfulness meditation and thought to constitute three interacting processes: attention control, emotional regulation, and self-awareness (Tang et al. 2015). Feuerborn and Gueldner (2019) elucidated the conceptual fit between mindfulness-based and SEL frameworks by looking at core SEL components of mindfulness-based programs. Self-management, an SEL competency which encompasses self-regulation and executive functioning, was represented in all of the 40 studies that the researchers reviewed. Further, promoting emotional regulation and cultivating compassion and empathy to build healthy relationships are key goals of both mindfulness-based and SEL interventions (de Carvalho et al. 2017).

There are a number of existing school-based programs that incorporate mindfulness and SEL skills, including MindUP, the program being evaluated in this study. Moreno-Gómez and Cejudo (2019) evaluated the effectiveness of a mindfulness-based SEL program (MindKinder) with kindergarten children and observed a significant reduction in teacher-reported externalizing behavior problems immediately after program implementation and at 6-month follow-up. Short-term neuropsychological effects were found for non-verbal development, visual perception, attention, and overall development. Conversely, enhancement of social skills was not observed until follow-up. Another comparable program (Settle Your Glitter) showed moderate gains in executive functioning in the intervention group (Thierry et al. 2018). Similar to results found in Moreno-Gómez and Cejudo (2019), there were no observed improvements in prosocial behavior. More recently, a small evaluation of the OpenMind-Korea program with preschool children (n = 83) found that children participating in the program had better emotion regulation, resilience, and prosocial behavior, compared to children receiving the usual curriculum (Kim et al. 2020).

Schonert-Reichl and Lawlor (2010) investigated the MindUP program and observed improvements in social and emotional competence and behavioral regulation. Schonert-Reichl et al. (2015) further explored the effects of MindUP with students in grades 4 and 5 by incorporating executive function tasks. Students who received the intervention had lower response times with equal accuracy relative to students who were not exposed to the intervention (i.e., comparison group). The superior performance of MindUP participants suggested an increase in ability to maintain focus, use working memory, and inhibit environmental distractions while performing tasks. Thierry and colleagues also evaluated the influence of MindUP on preschoolers’ executive functioning by utilizing a teacher-report questionnaire rather than performance-based tasks (Thierry et al. 2016). The researchers found that students who received the intervention showed greater executive functioning compared to students in the comparison group. Another study on MindUP showed that children in third and fourth grades who participated in the program scored higher on emotional regulation, experienced more positive affect, and displayed more self-compassion when compared with the comparison group (de Carvalho et al. 2017).

Thus, while the evidence base for MindUP has been growing, there has been little research focusing on students in kindergarten, despite the importance of this stage in the maturation of cognitive functions, including self-regulation and executive functioning (Montroy et al. 2016). The preschool and early school-age years are considered a critical stage of growth in self-regulation, with children typically moving from being more reactive and coregulated to acquiring more advanced self-regulation. However, differences in the development of self-regulation across early childhood can vary widely. The predictive implications for children that struggle with the development of such skills have been consistently related to poor short- and long-term outcomes such as school readiness, self-worth, academic achievement, ability to cope with stress, substance abuse, and law-breaking (McClelland et al. 2013; Montroy et al. 2016). Kindergarten is an important transition year for young students. The demands on students in first grade shift dramatically, and students require the self-regulatory capacity to undertake more teacher-directed work and spend more time in their seats (La Paro et al. 2006). Therefore, the evaluation of kindergarten programs that target the development of self-regulation through social-emotional learning and mindful awareness is critical for their potential impact on the lives of children.

The purpose of this study was to evaluate MindUP among kindergarten children with respect to impacts on behavioral symptoms, adaptive skills, and executive functioning. This study extends current research by focusing on this important transitional period in development. It was hypothesized that students in MindUP classrooms would show improvements in behavior, executive functioning, and adaptive functioning relative to their comparison group peers. We did not have specific hypotheses about gender effects, as these analyses were more exploratory in nature.

Method

Participants

All schools were located in a Catholic school district in Southwestern Ontario. The school board with whom we partnered selected schools based on a number of factors including high Social Risk Index (SRI) scores, equity of services, commitment to other ongoing research studies, and school administration interest in supporting mindfulness-based curriculum. The school board measures Social Risk Index based on the following indicators: parental education, employment, income, government-subsidized incomes, family structure, mobility, language, and immigration. High SRI scores represent higher risk due to a greater presence of disadvantageous characteristics (e.g., unemployment, low income, and lone-parent).

During the 2016–2017 school year, 15 kindergarten classrooms in eight schools were invited to participate in a pilot of the MindUP program and research methods. Kindergarten classrooms in this jurisdiction consist of both junior and senior kindergarten students (i.e., students start kindergarten the year they turn four, and have 2 years of kindergarten programming). Based on the success of the pilot, additional classrooms were added the following year at the same time that comparison classrooms were added to create a quasi-experimental research design. During the 2017–2018 school year (from which data are drawn for this study), the district recruited two additional schools to participate in the intervention condition and seven schools to serve as comparison schools. In the seven comparison schools, 19 kindergarten classrooms participated in the study. In the 10 intervention schools, 23 kindergarten classrooms participated in the study. Although the children from the pilot continued to be part of our data collection procedures, they were excluded from these analyses because some of them were older than the comparison group (i.e., we had intervention children in grade 1 but no grade 1 comparison classrooms) and because we did not want to include children who were participating in the intervention for the second time. This recruitment resulted in intervention and comparison groups of 584 students (intervention n = 261; comparison n = 323). Figure 1 provides an overview of the study design and sample size.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Study design including the number of schools and students in each condition

Procedure

This study was approved by the Western University’s Non-Medical Research Ethics Board. Educators in the invited schools sent home letters of information with all students in their classrooms and obtained written parental consent for children to participate. All children in the classes were eligible to participate. Educators were compensated to complete ratings based on the observed behaviors of the student participants at two time points: T1 (September–December 2017; pre-implementation) and T2 (May–June 2018; post-implementation). Educators completed the measures through an online, secure surveying software.

Intervention

The intervention was a mindfulness-based SEL curriculum (MindUP) implemented by trained teachers and early childhood educators over the course of a school year. The Hawn Foundation trained the educators in MindUP with a full day training at the beginning of the school year and a full day extension training partway through the school year. The program start date varied, as the educators were required to complete the pre-implementation measures prior to starting the program. The dosage (i.e., amount of time) and duration (i.e., number of weeks across which the educator implemented activities) of the intervention depended on the individual educators, consistent with a realist evaluation framework (Bonell et al. 2011). To assess implementation fidelity, educators completed lesson tracking sheets, in which they indicated the completion date, the total time spent, and completed curricular components (e.g., journal writing and literature links) for each lesson.

The MindUP program is one of the most widely studied school-based programs to date and is the focus of this research. MindUP is a manualized program informed by cognitive developmental neuroscience, contemplative science and mindful awareness, positive psychology, and SEL. The MindUP curriculum includes activities that promote mindful awareness and the five SEL competencies: self-awareness, self-management, social awareness, relationship skills, and responsible decision-making. The curriculum consists of 15 sequential lessons taught approximately once per week in 10–15 min chunks. The four domains and session topics are shown in Table 1. There are several extension activities that connect with curriculum expectations, including science, math, language arts, and SEL. There are four units: Getting Focused (introduction to brain structure and function, the core practice, and how focused awareness affects the brain and feelings), Sharpening Your Senses (focus on internal experiences such as mindful smelling and tasting), It’s All About Attitude (focus on cognitive experiences such as positive mindset, perspective taking, and practising gratitude), and Taking Action Mindfully (performing acts of kindness to others and building positive relationships). An essential component of the program is the core practice, a focused breathing exercise, and mindfulness practice that is recommended to be implemented three times per day for one or more minutes at a time. The core practice and components of the program are intended to promote cognitive control, engagement with the prefrontal cortex, regulation of behavior, regulation of stress, well-being, and prosociality (Maloney et al. 2016).

Table 1 MindUP curriculum: units, themes, lessons, and objectives

The educators received the MindUP curriculum manual designed for grades kindergarten to grade 2, a chime for mindful awareness practice, and other materials to promote the successful implementation of MindUP (i.e., storybooks, Hoberman sphere, and card deck of mindfulness games). The training and resources were provided to the educators upon their commitment to implement MindUP. Educators in both the MindUP and comparison conditions were engaged as paid research assistants. The research assistant role involved distributing and collecting informed parental consent forms and completing two measures for participating children at two data collection time points per school year: before and after implementing the MindUP curriculum.

Regular Curriculum

Comparison school educators did not participate in the MindUP training, or receive MindUP curriculum and resources. Educators implemented their usual teaching practices to meet the expectations of the early childhood curriculum. The regular kindergarten program includes religious education and areas of learning such as Belonging and Contributing and Self-Regulation and Well-being, which focus on gratitude, compassion, self-understanding, social awareness, and emotion-regulation.

Measures

As research assistants, teachers rated the students in their classes using the Behavior Assessment System for Children, Third Edition, Teacher Rating Scales (BASC-3 TRS; Reynolds and Kamphaus 2015). The BASC-3 TRS provides clinical ratings of adaptive and maladaptive behavior for children aged 2–5 (Preschool Version) and aged 6–11 (Child Version). The BASC-3 first asks for some basic child demographics (e.g., grade, age, gender, and birth date) and for the teachers to indicate how long they have known the child they are rating. Then, the teachers complete the behavior items (Preschool—105 items; Child—165 items) using a four-point scale (0 = never, 1 = sometimes, 2 = often, 3 = almost always) by the frequency they observed the child exhibit the behavior over the past several months. We utilized the BASC-3 Behavioral Symptoms Index (BSI; Cronbach’s α for Preschool and Child versions in the current study was 0.97 for both versions), the Internalizing Problems (Cronbach’s α for Preschool and Child versions was 0.91 and 0.96, respectively) and Externalizing Problems (Cronbach’s α for Preschool and Child versions was 0.95 and 0.97, respectively) composite scales, and the Adaptive Skills composite scale (Cronbach’s α for Preschool and Child versions was 0.94 and 0.96, respectively). The Internalizing and Externalizing Problems composite scales were chosen because research suggests boys are more likely to show externalizing behavior problems, and girls are more likely to display internalizing behavior problems (Maguire et al. 2016). The BSI measures the overall level of behavioral problems and consists of Hyperactivity, Aggression, Depression, Attention, Atypicality, and Withdrawal scales. The Internalizing Problems composite scale includes Anxiety, Depression, and Somatization scales. The Externalizing Problem composite scale includes Hyperactivity, Aggression, and Conduct Problems scales (on the TRS-Child Version only). Higher scores represent higher levels of maladaptive behaviors for the BSI and Internalizing and Externalizing Problems composite scales. Higher scores on the Adaptive Skills composite represent higher levels of prosocial and desirable behavior. The scales on the Adaptive Skills composite include Adaptability, Functional Communication, Social Skills, and Study Skills (on the TRS-Child Version only).

Teachers also completed the Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function-Preschool Version (BRIEF-P; Gioia et al. 1996) or Child Version (BRIEF-2; Gioia et al. 2000) for participating children. The BRIEF-P is a standardized 63-item questionnaire that measures various aspects of executive functioning in children aged 2–5 years and 11 months. The BRIEF-2 is a standardized 63-item questionnaire that measures various aspects of executive functioning in children aged 5–18. Both the BRIEF-P and BRIEF-2 required teachers to rate children’s behaviors on a three-point scale (0 = never, 1 = sometimes, 2 = often). The Global Executive Composite (GEC; Cronbach’s α for Preschool and Child versions was 0.98 and 0.97, respectively) was used from both versions of the BRIEF for this study. The GEC is intended to provide a measure of a child’s overall executive function, which takes into account all of the clinical scales (i.e., Initiate, Working Memory, Plan/Organize, Monitor, Shift, and Emotional Control). Higher GEC score indicates deficits in one or more areas of executive function. Both the BASC-3 and the BRIEF measures are used widely in clinical and research applications and have strong psychometric properties.

Data Analyses

Means and standard deviations (SD) for continuous variables, and absolute and relative frequencies for categorical variables were reported. Chi-Square test for categorical covariates and independent t test for continuous covariates were used to compare the two study groups at baseline (Table 2). In addition, the five study outcomes (i.e., BSI, internalizing composite, externalizing composite, adaptive skills, and GEC) were compared between the two study groups using independent t test, and their change scores over one-time follow-up were examined using paired t test (Table 3). The study outcomes were fitted using multilevel linear regression models to account for clustering with three levels: (a) time-specific observations (i.e., two time points), (b) students, and (c) classes. The likelihood ratio (LR) test supported an improvement in the variances favoring a three-level model (e.g., LR Chi-square = 607.0; P value < 0.001 for BSI scale). In the third level, intra-class correlations (ICC) were reported to show the proportion of the variation in the study outcomes attributable to the clustering of the study classes. For each study outcome, two models were constructed to explore the impact of the intervention (Tables 4, 5, and 6). In model 1, we examined the impact of the intervention considering the clustering levels, with time and its interaction term included in the model to demonstrate the change over time among students who were exposed to the intervention versus students in the comparison group (i.e., unadjusted model). To account for the imbalanced distribution of the student- and class-level covariates at baseline, in model 2, we examined the impact of the MindUP intervention adjusting for age, class SRI, and class size (i.e., adjusted model). The interaction of the study group and time provides the difference-in-difference estimations, referring to the difference in the average change over time in the study outcomes for the intervention group and the average change over time for the comparison group. The negative coefficients indicate the improvement of the outcome over time in the intervention group vs. comparison group, and positive coefficients indicate the improvement of the outcome over time in the intervention group vs. comparison group. The interaction between intervention and gender was also assessed for each outcome; however, no gender-specific analysis was reported as no significant interactions were identified between gender and the intervention (data are not shown but available upon request). Unadjusted and adjusted beta (β) coefficients, along with standard errors (SE) were reported for the fixed part of the models, and variances explained at each level was reported for the random part. Data were analyzed in SPSS v.24 (IBM) and Stata v.15 (StataCorp, College Station, Texas).

Table 2 Descriptive characteristics of the study participants (N = 584)
Table 3 T-score descriptive statistics of the study outcomes across the groups
Table 4 Multilevel models of the BASC-3 Behavioral Symptom Index (BSI) and BASC-3 Adaptive Composite Scale examining the effect of the intervention of interest, adjusting for student- and class-level covariates
Table 5 Multilevel models of the BASC-3 Internalizing Composite and Externalizing Composite examining the effect of the intervention of interest, adjusting for student- and class-level covariates
Table 6 Multilevel models of the BRIEF-2 Global Executive Composite (GEC) examining the effect of the intervention of interest, adjusting for student- and class-level covariates

Results

Intervention Implementation

Of the 14 educators who submitted the lesson tracking sheets upon program completion, 13 (93%) reported that they implemented all 15 lessons. The educators spent, on average, 43 min per lesson (SD 33.12), indicating high levels of implementation. The educators also demonstrated high program adherence by completing multiple curricular components for each lesson.

Pre-implementation Characteristics

The pre-intervention characteristics, both student- and class-level characteristics of the entire sample and the intervention and comparison groups, are provided in Table 2. Slightly more than half of the overall sample were girls (51.54%), with 51.08% and 52.11% in the comparison and intervention groups, respectively (P value = 0.806). Mean (SD) age of the entire sample was 4.37 (0.58) years old, with 4.45 (0.55) and 4.27 (0.61) in the comparison and intervention groups, respectively (P value ≤ 0.001). More than two-thirds of the entire sample reported being white (70.09%), with 72.58% and 67.06% in the two study groups, respectively. Class-level characteristics included SRI, with around two-thirds (61.8%) categorized as somewhat high or high, with 36.84% in the comparison groups versus 92.72% in the intervention group (P value < 0.001); class size, with an average (SD) of 23.03 (3.40) students in each class, with 23.51 (3.14) in the comparison group and 22.42 (3.60) in the intervention group (P value < 0.001); and teachers’ experience, with an average (SD) of 17.44 (10.33) years reported teaching experience in the entire sample, with 17.77 (9.61) and 17.02 (11.15) in the comparison and intervention groups, respectively (P value = 0.392).

Results comparing the study outcomes across the study groups at pre-intervention showed that comparison group had a lower mean (SD) value relative to the intervention groups with regard to (a) BSI scale (46.71 (9.50) vs. 51.19 (10.84), respectively, P value < 0.001), (b) internalizing composite (47.78 (9.50) vs. 51.27 (10.16), P value < 0.001), (c) externalizing composite (45.77 (8.23) vs. 49.71 (10.80), P value < 0.001), and (d) GEC scale (51.58 (13.07) vs. 57.7 (13.81), P value < 0.001). Conversely, the comparison group had a higher mean (SD) value for adaptive skills relative to the intervention group (52.84 (9.85) vs. 48.78 (8.87), P value < 0.001). Detailed between-group comparisons at both pre- and post-intervention time periods, as well as within-group comparisons, are shown in Table 3.

Impact of the Intervention on BSI and Adaptive Skills

Unadjusted and adjusted models showed that the intervention reduced BSI T-score among students in the intervention relative to the comparison group. Controlling for student- and class-level covariates, adjusted models demonstrated that BSI T-score was significantly reduced, on average, 2.23 units among students in the intervention in contrast to the reductions in the comparison group over time (i.e., adjusted β = − 2.47 (SE = 0.5), P value < 0.001). As seen in Table 4, the adjusted model showed that adaptive skills T-score was significantly increased by, on average, 3.45 units among students in the intervention group versus those in the comparison group (i.e., adjusted β = 3.45 (SE = 0.6), P value < 0.001).

Impact of the Intervention on Internalizing and Externalizing Behaviors

The adjusted models showed that the intervention reduced the BASC-3 internalizing composite among students in the intervention group compared to those in the comparison group (i.e., adjusted β = − 2.73 (SE = 0.7), P value < 0.001). In terms of the externalizing composite, a similar pattern emerged such that those in the intervention group had a higher reduction in externalizing behaviors than those in the comparison groups (i.e., adjusted β = − 2.05 (SE = 0.5), P value < 0.001). See Table 5.

Impact of the Intervention on GEC

The adjusted model showed that GEC T-scores were significantly reduced, on average, 4.51 units among students in the intervention group in comparison to the reductions in the comparison group over time (i.e., adjusted β = − 4.51 (SE = 0.7), P value < 0.001). See table 6.

Discussion

This research contributes to the scant literature on the impacts of mindfulness-based SEL programs on young children. Our design addressed some of the concerns with previous studies by including a larger sample size and a sufficient number of clusters (i.e., classrooms) to account for the natural grouping of children. Overall, we saw significant gains for children in all areas that we measured, including reduction of behavioral symptoms, reduction of internalizing and externalizing behavior problems, increase in adaptive skills, and reduction of executive functioning deficits. Interestingly, for adaptive skills and executive functioning, both groups improved significantly (see Table 3 for within-group differences), with the intervention group improving much more rapidly. However, in the case of internalizing problems, the intervention group improved and the comparison group actually worsened over the course of the school year.

Among the areas we evaluated, MindUP had the strongest impacts on reducing executive functioning deficits. These improvements in executive functioning are consistent with previous research on MindUP with older students (Schonert-Reichl et al. 2015) and preschool-aged students (Thierry et al. 2016; Thierry et al. 2018). Other researchers have postulated that mindfulness programs help young children practice and develop their attentional flexibility, inhibitory control, and effective emotional regulation, which in turn are reflected in better improved executive functionings (Kim et al. 2020).

Limitations and Future Research

Despite the promising results on the effects of mindfulness-based SEL intervention on children, the study has several limitations. Although the current design had comparison classrooms, schools were not randomized into the two conditions. Our school board partner does not endorse randomization because they need to take other considerations into account in making programming decisions. As a result, the intervention group had much higher social risk indicators and higher levels of behavioral challenges pre-intervention versus children in comparison schools. Although we controlled for classroom level covariates that differed between the two conditions, this adjustment is not optimal compared to groups that start out more equitably. In addition, teachers were aware of the children’s treatment condition and had a dual role of being both implementers and raters. As a result, their responses may have been influenced by expectancy effect. Adding parent ratings would be another option, but because behavior is so context-dependent, it might not be realistic to expect changes in behavior at home without specific efforts to support the generalization of gains. Furthermore, although educators implementing MindUP would experience a potential bias, the comparison educators are also utilizing classroom strategies that they feel are effective and would likely experience a similar bias of wanting the students in their class to show better self-regulation as the year progresses. Adding self-report measures can provide another perspective, but it is not feasible with children as young as the ones in this study.

Another limitation was obtaining incomplete fidelity data that relied on self-report. Thus, while we can report the amount of time teachers spent on the program as a group, we did not have sufficient data to use dosage as a covariate. When the program is implemented well, it extends beyond the individual lessons to include daily practice integrated into other activities. This limitation has been highlighted in the field of school-based mindfulness interventions, and there is a need to better identify and measure core components to assess fidelity (Feagans Gould et al. 2016). Future research should evaluate the quality of implementation through classroom observation of teachers’ lesson delivery when feasible. Finally, we anticipate that there was some contamination of the comparison group with mindfulness-based activities and yoga due to the general rise in popularity of these approaches. We did not document any comparable activities in either condition.

In summary, our findings contribute to the existing scientific literature by demonstrating the benefits of a mindfulness-based SEL intervention on positive and negative indicators of well-being in young children. Behavioral symptoms, internalizing and externalizing problems, and adaptive skills improved for kindergarten students exposed to the intervention. Consistent with previous literature, the largest improvement was found for executive functioning, regardless of gender. Examining the components of executive functioning specifically, and its role in self-regulation are important to be considered in future research. Furthermore, adding performance-based measures of executive functioning, or ratings from other informants could strengthen the findings reported in this paper. Focusing on the development of executive functioning is a high priority, because fostering these skills in early years is critical to mental health and well-being across the lifespan.