Beach handball, a relatively new sport developed in the 1990s, is based on team indoor handball, and the sport’s European and World championships are consistently gaining attention. Beach handball is even being considered as an Olympic event (Gruić, Bazzeo, & Ohnjec, 2011; Handball-World.com, 2019; Iannaccone et al., 2022). The game can clearly be distinguished from its indoor version by the playing surface (sand), pitch size (27 m × 12 m), number of players on the field (4 × 4) and game duration (2 × 10 min). Another difference is in the throwing technique and the counting system, as certain throws account for two points. The spin shot (a double-leg jump with 360° rotation along the longitudinal axis) is predominantly used to score, and teams can score double with in-flights (Kempa) and specialist goals (goal keeper).

The body of literature in beach handball is constantly growing and evolving indicating further professionalization (Bon & Pori, 2020). The focus ranges, for example, from notational analysis (Iannaccone et al., 2022; Navarro, Morillo, Reigal, & Hernandez-Mendo, 2018), player profiling (Lemos et al., 2020), skill and position specific analyses (Lemos et al., 2021; Navarro et al., 2018; Zapardiel & Asín-Izquierdo, 2020), load measures (Iannaccone et al., 2022; Muller, Willberg, Reichert, & Zentgraf, 2022), physiological and kinematic requirements of beach handball (Pueo, Jimenez-Olmedo, Penichet-Tomas, Ortega Becerra, & Agullo, 2017), and energy cost considerations on sand surfaces (Balasas et al., 2013). In addition, a recent meta-analysis indicated that training sessions on sand can improve sprinting and jumping performance on firm ground as much as training on firm ground (Pereira et al., 2021).

In Germany, elite beach handball is currently almost exclusively played by indoor team handball athletes. One consequence for athletes who play both indoor and beach handball is that the indoor and beach seasons will overlap: In spring, as athletes will need to start preparing for beach handball, they will still be in the competition phase of the indoor team handball season. Also, late summer beach handball tournaments might overlap with athletes’ preparation for the upcoming indoor season. Thus, the transition phases, when athletes are switching between different surfaces, are crucial concerning performance levels and injury prevention. From this perspective, a transition needs to be started early, i.e., before the end of the respective season, so that athletes can smoothly adapt to the demands of new surfaces.

Yet, coaches and athletes may be reluctant to add training on sand surfaces for fear that it will negatively affect performance on rigid surfaces and is time consuming, even though performance-enhancing training on sand surfaces can improve results on both surfaces (Ahmadi et al.; Hammami et al., 2020; Pereira et al., 2021). From that viewpoint, an optimal training intervention is one that can increase jump, sprint, and agility performance on rigid and sand surfaces and whereby the proportion of sand exercises in training sessions should increase over time. In recent articles, systematic reviews and meta-analyses, plyometric trainingFootnote 1, further referred to as reactive strength training, has been recommended for improving jump, sprint and agility performance in team sports (Asadi, Arazi, Young, & Saez de Villarreal, 2016; Granacher, Goebel, Behm, & Büsch, 2018; Markovic, 2007; Ramirez-Campillo et al., 2020; Saez de Villarreal, Requena, & Cronin, 2012; Slimani, Chamari, Miarka, Del Vecchio, & Cheour, 2016). In addition, the efficacy of such training bouts seems to be valid for rigid as well as soft (especially sand) surfaces (Ahmadi et al., 2021; Arazi, Mohammadi, & Asadi, 2014; Büsch, Pabst, Mühlbauer, Ehrhardt, & Granacher, 2015; Hammami et al., 2020; Impellizzeri et al., 2008). Consequently, reactive strength training bouts might be recommended as the basis for a transition training, which is the focus of the present investigation.

Therefore, the aim of the present study was to evaluate a transition phase from the indoor to the beach season to improve motor performance on sand surfaces in elite beach handball athletes. The intervention program consisted of specific reactive strength exercises, and the amount of sand-specific exercises gradually increased on a weekly basis. Standardized measurements for vertical jumps on a sand surface were conducted by transferring the complex sand surface into a controlled laboratory situation. Our hypothesis was that the intervention program would significantly increase motor performance (jump, sprint, agility) on the sand surface in the intervention group (elite beach handball players) in contrast to a control group (elite handball players who did not participate in the intervention program) without losing performance on the rigid surface.

Methods

Participants

In all, 29 elite athletes (11 men, 18 women) participated in the study (Table 1). All players played beach or indoor handball at a high-performance level. The intervention group consisted of 15 beach handball players from the German national beach handball team (7 men, 8 women). As most of the national players also actively played indoor handball in the 2nd and 3rd German divisions, the control group was also chosen from the 2nd and 3rd German indoor handball divisions. The control group consisted of 14 players (4 men, 10 women). Subjects were free of injuries before and during the course of the study. Prior to participation, all athletes signed an informed consent form. The study was approved by the local human ethics committee of the department of Psychology and Sport Sciences of the University of Münster (ID: 2018-03-EE), and all procedures were performed in accordance with the principles of the Declaration of Helsinki.

Table 1 Anthropometric data of the intervention and control groups

Training intervention

The intervention was adapted from a program presented by Bansa et al. (Bansa, Novakovic, Pfänder, Zentgraf, & Büsch, 2018). It was performed twice a week (with a 48‑h break between sessions) and consisted of six different exercises that were performed directly before the normal training routines. The complete training bout lasted 20 min after a short warm-up period, and each exercise was performed for 45 s followed by a 30‑s break before subjects continued with the next exercise. The sand proportion of the exercises progressively increased from week to week, i.e., one of the six exercises was performed on sand within the first week, and all six exercises were performed on sand in the last week of the intervention program. Table 2 describes the reactive strength-like exercises in more detail. Proper implementation of the training was controlled by the coaches. The aim of the intervention was to increase athletes’ performance on sand surfaces without impairing their indoor performance on rigid surfaces. The intervention began four weeks before the end of the indoor season and ended two weeks after the start of the beach season.

Table 2 Transition training program from indoor to sand surface

Measurements

A custom-built sandbox (size 1.25 m × 1.25 m × 0.3 m) was used to transfer the sand surface into the laboratory to analyze the athletes’ countermovement jumps (CMJ) and drop jumps (DJ) from a height of 40 cm (Fig. 1). The sand fulfilled the specifications of the German Volleyball Federation, section beach for indoor sand (grain size: 0.1–1.0 mm; grain shape: from round edges to rounded; grain distribution: even; CaCO3 ≤ 2–3%; SiO2 ≥ 95–98%; Borrmann et al., 2009).

Fig. 1
figure 1

Laboratory situation when measuring drop and countermovement jumps on sand and rigid surfaces. The figure shows a subject prior to performing a drop jump into the sandbox. For countermovement jumps, the drop-box was removed and athletes performed directly within the sandbox from sand level. The jumps on the rigid surface were performed on the floor directly behind the sandbox

To measure jumping height, a reflective ball marker was placed on the athletes’ lower back at the height of the navel. Coordinates of the marker were detected using a three-dimensional motion capture system (Qualisys, Gothenburg, Sweden). For each athlete, at least three successful trials were measured and analyzed for each of the four conditions (CMJ and DJ on both rigid and sand surfaces).

Sprinting performance (5–10–20 m) as well as a handball-specific agility test (HAST; Vieira, Veiga, Carita, & Petroski, 2013) were conducted on both rigid and sand surfaces. Sprinting performance was measured using magnetic timing gates (Smartrack Diagnostics, Humotion GmbH, Münster, Germany). HAST time was measured by means of a light-gate system (wk Elektronische Zeitmessanlagen, Ditzingen, Germany). In addition, to measure reactive long jump performance on sand, athletes performed drop long jumps (DLJ), which were measured using a tape measure.

For each athlete, three successful trials were measured in each test, and the best trial was used for further analysis.

Test procedure

Before testing, athletes underwent a standardized and instructed warm-up and dynamic stretching session for 20 min. Teams were then divided into smaller subgroups. One subgroup performed the sprint, agility, and DLJ tests, while the other subgroup performed the jumping tests in the lab. The reflective marker was attached to the subjects’ lower backs, and they started the jumping analysis. Due to organizational reasons, the order of the jumps and the surfaces were pseudo-randomized, where athletes started with one surface and performed both jumps, and then moved on to the next surface. Each subject had two trials to familiarize them with the jumping type and the surface. Instructions for both jumps were standardized and repeated for each bout of jumps. Wrong executions were corrected immediately. Resting time between trials and tests were provided to avoid effects of fatigue.

For sprinting assessment, the start was 1 m behind the first magnetic gate. Subjects initiated the sprint in a self-directed way. The time began recording when the athlete passed the magnetic gates, and sprinting split times were taken for 5 m, 10 m, and the total 20 m distance. For the HAST, subjects also started 1 m behind the starting line. The drop long jump was performed by dropping down from a 20-cm-high platform to the sand and immediately performing a long jump. Resting times between trials and tests were assured, and instructions for each test were standardized.

After finishing either the jumping or sprinting assessment, agility and DLJ assessments, the groups switched tests. Each performance measurement at the start and the end of the whole intervention was conducted for each team group, i.e., female and male beach handball national team players and the two control teams (one for the male team, one for the female team).

Data analysis

Data processing and data analysis for all jumps were performed using the Qualisys Track Manager and MATLAB (The MathWorks Inc., Natick, MA, USA). Jumping height was detected using the trajectory of the back marker (difference between maximum and standing height). Sprint and HAST times were directly exported to an Excel® spreadsheet (Microsoft Corporation, Redmond, WA, USA).

An a priori required sample size calculation was performed using G*Power (Version 3.1.9.6) and resulted in a sample size of n = 28 (medium effect size f = 0.25, power = 0.8, correlations among repeated measurements = 0.6, number of groups = 2, number of measurements = 2, and ∝ = 0.05; Faul, Erdfelder, Lang, & Buchner, 2007). Statistical analysis (Jamovi Version 1.2, the Jamovi project: https://www.jamovi.org) was performed by means of a mixed model analysis of variance (ANOVA) using within factors time (pre, post) and surface (rigid, sand), and a between-subject factor group (intervention, control). Holm correction was used for post hoc analysis. The alpha level was set to 5% and effect sizes were reported as generalized eta squared (η2G). The magnitude of effect sizes was interpreted on the following criteria: η2G < 0.02 (small), η2G = 0.02–0.13 (medium), η2G = 0.13–0.26 (large; Bakeman, 2005).

Results

Results for jumping (vertical & drop long jump), sprinting, and agility are presented in Table 3.

Table 3 Results and statistics for jump, sprint, and agility tests on rigid and sand surfaces

Jumping performance

The results of the ANOVA for the CMJ showed a significant interaction between time and group (F(1.27) = 14.16, p < 0.001, η2G = 0.01), a significant main effect surface (F(1.27) = 11.67, p = 0.002, η2G < 0.01), and a significant main effect group (F(1.27) = 4.86, p = 0.036, η2G = 0.15). Post hoc analysis of time* surface*group revealed that the intervention group significantly improved performance in CMJ on the rigid surface (p = 0.002) compared to the control group due to the training intervention. For the DJ, the ANOVA showed a non-significant interaction between time and group (F(1.27) = 3.63, p = 0.07, η2G = 0.01), a significant main effect for surface (F(1.27) = 9.47, p = 0.005, η2G = 0.03), and a significant main effect group (F(1.27) = 4.28, p = 0.048, η2G = 0.11). Results are presented on an individual basis in Fig. 2. These indicate that several but not all athletes in the intervention group improved between pre- and posttest.

Fig. 2
figure 2

Individual changes between pre- and posttests for all jumps. Please note that mean values and standard deviation are also presented as big black asterisk with whiskers to the left and right of the block of individual values. Subfigures represent CMJ on rigid surface (a), CMJ on sand surface (b), DJ on rigid surface (c), and DJ on sand surface (d)

Sprinting performance

The results of the ANOVA for the 5‑m sprint showed a significant interaction between time and group (F(1.27) = 5.73, p = 0.024, η2G = 0.003), a significant interaction between surface and group (F(1.27) = 7.54, p = 0.011, η2G = 0.01), a significant main effect surface (F(1.27) = 558.22, p < 0.001, η2G = 0.44), and a significant main effect group (F(1.27) = 6.14, p = 0.020, η2G = 0.17). Analysis of the type of interaction between surface and group revealed an ordinal interaction; thus, both main effects surface and group are fully interpretable. Post hoc analysis of time*group*surface revealed that both groups did not improve performance due to the training intervention on each surface (rigid, sand) separately. Post hoc analysis of surface*group and main effects surface and group revealed that the performance on the sand surface was significantly lower (p < 0.001) for both groups compared to the rigid surface, but this difference in performance for the intervention group was smaller than that of the control group and more denoted on sand than on a rigid surface.

The results of the ANOVA for the 10‑m sprint showed a significant interaction between time and group (F(1.27) = 11.05, p = 0.003, η2G = 0.006), a significant interaction between surface and group (F(1.27) = 11.69, p = 0.002, η2G = 0.010), a significant main effect surface (F(1.27) = 959.64, p < 0.001, η2G = 0.46), a significant interaction between time and surface (F(1.27) = 5.58, p = 0.026, η2G = 0.003), and a significant main effect group (F(1.27) = 5.82, p = 0.023, η2G = 0.17). Analysis of the type of interaction between surface and group also revealed an ordinal interaction, such that both main effects surface and group are fully interpretable. Post hoc analysis of time*group*surface revealed that the performance in the control group significantly decreased on a sand surface after the intervention period (p = 0.043), whereas the performance of the intervention group remained at the same level. Additional analysis of time*surface and the main effects surface and group revealed that performance on the sand surface was significantly lower (p < 0.001) for both groups compared to the rigid surface, but for the sand surface the performance of the intervention group remained at the same level, whereas that of the control group decreased between pre- and posttest.

The results of the ANOVA for the 20‑m sprint showed a significant interaction between time and group (F(1.27) = 10.50, p = 0.003, η2G < 0.01), a significant interaction between surface and group (F(1.27) = 10.68, p = 0.003, η2G = 0.01), a significant main effect surface (F(1.27) = 723.82, p < 0.001, η2G = 0.49) and a significant main effect group (F(1.27) = 4.94, p = 0.035, η2G = 0.15). Analysis of the type of interaction between surface and group also revealed an ordinal interaction, so both main effects surface and group are fully interpretable. Post hoc analysis of time*group*surface revealed that no performance changes occurred in both groups between pre- and posttests on each surface separately, but considering the performances on both surfaces combined, the performance of the control group significantly decreased at the posttest compared to the pretest (p = 0.007). Post hoc analysis of surface*group and main effects surface and group revealed that performance on the sand surface was significantly lower (p < 0.001) for both groups compared to on the rigid surface, but the performance difference in the intervention group was smaller than that of the control group and more denoted on a sand than on a rigid surface.

Results on an individual basis are presented in Fig. 3. Most athletes in the intervention group improved their performance on rigid surface, but this tendency was less clear for the sand surface.

Fig. 3
figure 3

Individual changes between pre- and posttests for all sprints. Please note that mean values and standard deviations are also presented as big black asterisk with whiskers to the left and right of the block of individual values. Subfigures represent 5 m sprint on rigid surface (a), 5 m sprint on sand surface (b), 10 m sprint on rigid surface (c), 10 m sprint on sand surface (d), 20 m sprint on rigid surface (e), 20 m sprint on sand surface (f)

Agility performance

The results of the ANOVA showed no significant interaction between time and group (F(1.27) < 0.01, p = 0.949, η2G < 0.001), a significant main effect time (F(1.27) = 22.82, p < 0.001, η2G = 0.04), a significant interaction between surface and group (F(1.27) = 6.58, p = 0.016, η2G = 0.010), a significant main effect surface (F(1.27) = 188.57, p < 0.001, η2G = 0.22), and a significant main effect group (F(1.27) = 4.70, p = 0.039, η2G = 0.13). Post hoc analysis of time*group*surface revealed that performance in the control group significantly increased on a rigid surface after intervention period (p = 0.003), whereas the performance improvements in the intervention group were not significant. Post hoc analysis of main effects surface and group revealed that performance on the sand surface was significantly lower (p < 0.001) for both groups compared to the rigid surface, and agility performance was higher in the intervention than in the control group.

Results on an individual basis are presented in Fig. 4a, b. In the control group, most athletes showed improved performance in the posttest on rigid but not on sand surfaces, whereas individual performance in the intervention group was variable between pre- and posttest on both rigid and sand surfaces.

Fig. 4
figure 4

Individual changes between pre- and posttests for agility and drop long jumps. Please note that mean values and standard deviations are also presented as big black asterisk with whiskers to the left and right of the block of individual values. Subfigures represent HAST on rigid surface (a), HAST on sand surface (b), Drop long jump on sand surface (c)

Drop long jump performance

Analysis of drop long jump (DLJ) performance on the sand surface showed a significant interaction between time and group (F(1.27) = 4.62, p = 0.041, η2G = 0.01), a significant main effect time (F(1.27) = 4.75, p = 0.038, η2G = 0.010), and a significant main effect group (F(1.27) = 12.45, p = 0.002, η2G = 0.30), whereas only the main effect group is fully interpretable. Results indicate that the intervention group significantly increased their performance due to the training compared to the control group and that the intervention group was at a higher performance level than the control group. Results on an individual basis are presented in Fig. 4c and show that all athletes in the intervention group increased their performance between pre- and posttest, whereas results in the control group were variable.

Discussion

The aim of the present investigation was to evaluate a transition phase from indoor to beach season to optimize motor performance on sand surfaces in elite beach handball athletes without declining their motor performance during the ongoing indoor season. Within this study, performance on rigid and sand surfaces was evaluated, thus enabling a direct comparison between performance levels on both surfaces. Results show that athletes in the intervention group significantly improved their performance in the CMJ on the rigid surface and the DLJ on the sand surface. The control group showed no significant improvements for jumping, sprinting, and DLJ, but they showed improved agility on a rigid surface. Therefore, the intervention program can be considered an effective tool for improving performance during the transition from the indoor to the beach season for selected skills. Furthermore, performance on the rigid surface was not only maintained but also significantly increased in CMJ. Importantly, when focusing on individual changes between pre- and posttests, only few athletes substantially decreased their performance, indicating that coaches have no need to be concerned about performance impairments on rigid surfaces when athletes again begin to train on sand surfaces.

Intervention effects

Performance improvements in our investigation were slightly smaller compared to results from other publications (Asadi et al., 2016; Markovic, 2007; Saez de Villarreal et al., 2012; Slimani et al., 2016) but do fall in the range of practically relevant changes on a rigid surface (Markovic, 2007), and for the sand surface the improvements fall in a range of 2–5%. The reason for these relatively small improvements may be because we instituted a shorter intervention period of 6 weeks rather than the recommended 8–10 weeks (Villarreal, Kellis, Kraemer, & Izquierdo, 2009) or because we applied a lower intensity or execution time (Slimani et al., 2016). Further, although the training program was supervised by coaches, individuals were partaking in the program, not whole teams, because the elite athletes trained with their standard indoor team at the beginning of the intervention phase and not with the beach teams. Thus, some athletes may not have taken full advantage of the training, diminishing the overall group effect. This may also explain why some athletes did not show improvements in the posttests (individual results in Figs. 23 and 4). However, relevant performance changes, as reflected by their effect sizes, are obvious for CMJ and DLJ, and individual results also show that most athletes responded to the intervention.

In beach handball, both CMJ and DLJ are relevant for performance because they build the basis for an effective spin shot (the most important attack jump) in beach handball (Navarro et al., 2018; Saavedra, Pic, Jimenez, Lozano, & Kristjánsdóttir, 2019). For explaining distinct effects of the training intervention concerning the different jumps (CMJ, DLJ, DJ), biomechanical considerations have to be taken into account. Previously, it has been shown that a substantial difference exists in the mechanical output and jumping performance between the slow (CMJ) and fast (DJ) stretch-shortening cycle (SSC) in jumps (Bobbert, 1990). The sand may provide a specific surface to address the slow SSC and, therefore, to predominantly influence CMJ or slow DLJ performance. However, it was also shown that reactive strength sand training influences DJ performance on a rigid surface, but the underlying processes remain unclear (Hammami et al., 2020).

In sprinting, the control group had lower performances for almost all distances and both surfaces (significantly on a sand surface at 10 m). An explanation for the significantly lower performance on the sand surface in the control group remains unclear and speculative, since constraints in the pre- and posttest were the same. However, sand conditions in the pre- and posttest, when performed outside, may differ and, thus, influence performance. Except for the jump analysis, where sand conditions were controlled between pre- and posttest in the lab, an accurate control of the sand conditions was not possible due to wind and weather. Although weather conditions were comparable between pre- and posttests, this might have influenced sprint performance in the control group but also in the intervention group (who may have performed even better). A further standardization concerning sand moisture but also sand quality (grain size and shape) is crucial to enable valid performance measurements on a sand surface in a pre-/posttest design. This will be evaluated in further investigations.

Methodological aspects

When focusing on the results of the different tests in our investigation, similar results concerning athletic performance in sprinting and agility in handball have been reported from other investigations (Asadi et al., 2016; Chaabene et al., 2019; Cherif et al., 2012; Hammami et al., 2020; Hammami, Gaamouri, Aloui, Shephard, & Chelly, 2019; Iacono, Eliakim, & Meckel, 2015; Prieske et al., 2019; Sabido, Hernandez-Davo, Botella, Navarro, & Tous-Fajardo, 2017). When focusing on jumps (CMJ and DJ), it is important to consider that results reported in the literature are related to the method used to detect jumping height (contact times, impulse-momentum method, marker-based coordinates). When using flight times to calculate jumping height, results will be lower than when derived from a marker-based approach or the impulse–momentum method (Moir, 2008) because the initial marker height is higher when plantar flexing the ankle and lifting up the body before leaving the ground. Considering this, our results for jumping are comparable to other results reported in the literature (Chaabene et al., 2019; Iacono, Martone, Milic, & Padulo, 2016; Prieske et al., 2019).

In the literature, performance diagnostics have mainly taken place on a rigid but not a sand surface, even when evaluating effects of sand-related training interventions (Hammami et al., 2020; Impellizzeri et al., 2008). In beach sports, like beach handball or volleyball, it is of special interest that performance is evaluated on sand and not on a rigid surface because this more accurately matches sport-specific performance characteristics. Performance measurements on sand surfaces are challenging and, in the present investigation, performance on both surfaces spanning different jumps as well as sprints and agility were measured. Our jump measurements in a standardized laboratory environment are particularly important for relating the performances obtained from both surfaces. Therefore, our results for all measurements on rigid and sand surface may be used as a reference for beach sport motor performances.

When evaluating the efficacy of training interventions, it is critically important that measurements during the pre- and posttest are conducted under the same or close conditions. In the present investigation, this was the case for all jump measurements under laboratory conditions. However, sometimes when evaluating performance, we had to adapt to varying external constraints (e.g., different testing locations), as the intervention occurred during athletes’ competition phase (e.g., players were competing in the world championships). As such, the intervention group had to perform the posttest for sprinting and agility on a slightly different rigid and sand surface. This may have decreased the performance in the intervention group in the posttest and might explain the absence of significant performance increases in sprint and agility.

Performance evaluations in the intervention group took place in combination with training courses over several days, when athletes arrived from all over Germany. Due to organizational reasons, performance evaluations within these courses did not take place at the same times of day. Prior of each test, we investigated both the DOMS (delayed onset of muscle soreness) and TQR (total quality recovery) scale, and the intervention group showed significantly altered values for both scales in the pre- compared to the posttest. Therefore, increased muscle soreness and reduced recovery may also have influenced the results, although a general relationship between DOMS and reduced performance in athletes has not been demonstrated (Altarriba-Bartes, Pena, Vicens-Bordas, Mila-Villaroel, & Calleja-Gonzalez, 2020).

Athletes in the intervention group showed better performance on all tests compared to athletes in the control group, even though they play indoor handball in the same league. This is rather unexpected, since not all top athletes in the leagues play beach handball, and statistical analysis for the pretest condition did not show significant differences between groups. Statistical analysis (significant interaction time*group) underlines the better performance of the intervention group compared to the control group as a result of the intervention program for almost all tests. However, this effect was not apparent when breaking down the post hoc analysis for different surfaces. The fact that the interaction between surface and group was also significant for several parameters indicates that beach athletes perform better on a sand surface compared to the control group. In fact, beach athletes are often not only better at performing on sand but also on a rigid surface when focusing on mean and individual results. Assuming that this is either the result of the training intervention or of a long-term adaptation during athletes’ beach experience, underground sand should be considered a valuable training surface that may be able to increase performance on sand but also on rigid surfaces, although the direct effect mechanism still remains unclear (Hammami et al., 2020). Influencing factors may include an increase in force, improved intra- and intermuscular coordination or changes in muscle size and architecture (Markovic & Mikulic, 2010; Prieske et al., 2019; Ramirez-Campillo, Andrade, & Izquierdo, 2013). Training on sand is often only used as an alternative to strenuous indoor training, as it is linked to reduced muscle damage (Impellizzeri et al., 2008; Miyama & Nosaka, 2004), instead of being considered a valuable surface in its own right to improve performance on rigid surfaces. We look forward to future prospective randomized controlled trials, as they may highlight the abilities and the full potential that a sand surface offers in terms of performance enhancement.

Conclusion

This is the first study to evaluate the transition from the indoor to the beach season in elite beach handball players. The results of the present study have several important implications for this transitional period. First, our findings establish a fundamental basis for performance measurements concerning jumping, sprinting, and agility performance for both rigid and sand surfaces. Second, in beach sports, performance measurements need to take place on a sand surface because the transfer from a rigid surface to beach performance still remains unclear. Third, the specific transition-intervention program will be recommended as an effective means to facilitate the transfer from an indoor to a sand surface without impairing performance on a rigid surface. This is of special interest for coaches and athletes who are concerned that training on a sand surface might impair indoor performance. Fourth, performance measurements on a sand surface are demanding, and sand characteristics are influenced by weather conditions. Moisture content and characteristics of the sand should be kept in mind when carrying out repeated measurements on sand surfaces.