Skip to main content
Log in

Relationship between circadian rhythm and brain cognitive functions

  • Mini Review
  • Published:
Frontiers of Optoelectronics Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Circadian rhythms are considered a masterstroke of natural selection, which gradually increase the adaptability of species to the Earth’s rotation. Importantly, the nervous system plays a key role in allowing organisms to maintain circadian rhythmicity. Circadian rhythms affect multiple aspects of cognitive functions (mainly via arousal), particularly those needed for effort-intensive cognitive tasks, which require considerable top-down executive control. These include inhibitory control, working memory, task switching, and psychomotor vigilance. This mini review highlights the recent advances in cognitive functioning in the optical and multimodal neuroimaging fields; it discusses the processing of brain cognitive functions during the circadian rhythm phase and the effects of the circadian rhythm on the cognitive component of the brain and the brain circuit supporting cognition.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  1. Refinetti R. Homeostasis and circadian rhythmicity in the control of body temperature. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1997, 813(1): 63–70

    Article  Google Scholar 

  2. Huang R C. The discoveries of molecular mechanisms for the circadian rhythm: The 2017 Nobel Prize in Physiology or Medicine. Biomedical Journal, 2018, 41(1): 5–8

    Article  Google Scholar 

  3. DeMairan J. Histoire de l’Academie Royale des Sciences. Paris 1729

  4. Reddy S, Reddy V, Sharma S. Physiology, Circadian Rhythm. In: StatPearls [Internet]. Treasure Island (FL): StatPearls Publishing, 2020

    Google Scholar 

  5. Burke T M, Scheer F A J L, Ronda J M, Czeisler C A, Wright K P Jr. Sleep inertia, sleep homeostatic and circadian influences on higher-order cognitive functions. Journal of Sleep Research, 2015, 24(4): 364–371

    Article  Google Scholar 

  6. Valdez P. Circadian rhythms in attention. Yale Journal of Biology and Medicine, 2019, 92(1): 81–92

    Google Scholar 

  7. Walker W H 2nd, Walton J C, DeVries A C, Nelson R J. Circadian rhythm disruption and mental health. Translational Psychiatry, 2020, 10(1): 28

    Article  Google Scholar 

  8. Bennett C L, Petros T V, Johnson M, Ferraro F R. Individual differences in the influence of time of day on executive functions. American Journal of Psychology, 2008, 121(3): 349–361

    Article  Google Scholar 

  9. Qasrawi S O, Pandi-Perumal S R, BaHammam A S. The effect of intermittent fasting during Ramadan on sleep, sleepiness, cognitive function, and circadian rhythm. Sleep and Breathing, 2017, 21(3): 577–586

    Article  Google Scholar 

  10. Owens J A. Sleep in children: cross-cultural perspectives. Sleep and Biological Rhythms, 2004, 2(3): 165–173

    Article  Google Scholar 

  11. Preckel F, Lipnevich A, Schneider S, Roberts R D. Chronotype, cognitive abilities, and academic achievement: a meta-analytic investigation. Learning and Individual Differences, 2011, 21(5): 483–492

    Article  Google Scholar 

  12. Ni Y, Wu L, Jiang J, Yang T, Wang Z, Ma L, Zheng L, Yang X, Wu Z, Fu Z. Late-night eating-induced physiological dysregulation and circadian misalignment are accompanied by microbial dysbiosis. Molecular Nutrition & Food Research, 2019, 63(24): e1900867

    Article  Google Scholar 

  13. Wehrens S M T, Christou S, Isherwood C, Middleton B, Gibbs M A, Archer S N, Skene D J, Johnston J D. Meal timing regulates the human circadian system. Current Biology, 2017, 27(12): 1768–1775.e3

    Article  Google Scholar 

  14. Shi L, Liu Y, Jiang T, Yan P, Cao F, Chen Y, Wei H, Liu J. Relationship between mental health, the CLOCK gene, and sleep quality in surgical nurses: a cross-sectional study. BioMed Research International, 2020, 4795763

  15. Kim H Y, Seo K, Jeon H J, Lee U, Lee H. Application of functional near-infrared spectroscopy to the study of brain function in humans and animal models. Molecules and Cells, 2017, 40(8): 523–532

    Article  Google Scholar 

  16. Huettel S A, Song A W, McCarthy G. Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging. Sunderland: Sinauer Associates, 2004

    Google Scholar 

  17. Boas D A, Brooks D H, Miller E L, DiMarzio C A, Kilmer M, Gaudette R J, Zhang Q. Imaging the body with diffuse optical tomography. IEEE Signal Processing Magazine, 2001, 18(6): 57–75

    Article  Google Scholar 

  18. Niedermeyer E, da Silva F H L. Electroencephalography: Basic Principles, Clinical Applications, and Related Fields. Philadelphia: Lippincott Williams & Wilkins, 2005

    Google Scholar 

  19. Anderson J R. Cognitive Psychology and Its Implications. New York: Worth Publishers, 2005

    Google Scholar 

  20. Cohen R A, Sparling-Cohen Y A, O’Donnell B F. The Neuropsychology of Attention. New York: Springer, 1993

    Book  Google Scholar 

  21. Graw P, Kräuchi K, Knoblauch V, Wirz-Justice A, Cajochen C. Circadian and wake-dependent modulation of fastest and slowest reaction times during the psychomotor vigilance task. Physiology & Behavior, 2004, 80(5): 695–701

    Article  Google Scholar 

  22. Lim J, Dinges D F. Sleep deprivation and vigilant attention. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 2008, 1129(1): 305–322

    Article  Google Scholar 

  23. Folkard S. Diurnal variation in logical reasoning. British Journal of Psychology, 1975, 66(1): 1–8

    Article  Google Scholar 

  24. Cohen R A. Neural mechanisms of attention. In: Cohen R A, Sparling-Cohen Y A, O’Donnell B F, eds. The Neuropsychology of Attention. New York: Springer, 2014, 211–264

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  25. Sturm W, Willmes K. On the functional neuroanatomy of intrinsic and phasic alertness. NeuroImage, 2001, 14(1 Pt 2): S76–S84

    Article  Google Scholar 

  26. Gazzaley A, Rissman J, Cooney J, Rutman A, Seibert T, Clapp W, D’Esposito M. Functional interactions between prefrontal and visual association cortex contribute to top-down modulation of visual processing. Cerebral Cortex, 2007, 17(Suppl 1): i125–i135

    Article  Google Scholar 

  27. Morecraft R J, Geula C, Mesulam M M. Architecture of connectivity within a cingulo-fronto-parietal neurocognitive network for directed attention. Archives of Neurology, 1993, 50(3): 279–284

    Article  Google Scholar 

  28. Soshi T, Kuriyama K, Aritake S, Enomoto M, Hida A, Tamura M, Kim Y, Mishima K. Sleep deprivation influences diurnal variation of human time perception with prefrontal activity change: a functional near-infrared spectroscopy study. PLoS One, 2010, 5 (1): e8395

    Article  Google Scholar 

  29. Valdez P, Ramírez C, García A, Talamantes J, Armijo P, Borrani J. Circadian rhythms in components of attention. Biological Rhythm Research, 2005, 36(1–2): 57–65

    Article  Google Scholar 

  30. Riley M. Musical Listening in the German Enlightenment: Attention, Wonder and Astonishment. London: Taylor & Francis, 2017

    Book  Google Scholar 

  31. Matchock R L, Mordkoff J T. Chronotype and time-of-day influences on the alerting, orienting, and executive components of attention. Experimental Brain Research, 2009, 192(2): 189–198

    Article  Google Scholar 

  32. Nicholls C, Bruno R, Matthews A. Chronic cannabis use and ERP correlates of visual selective attention during the performance of a flanker go/nogo task. Biological Psychology, 2015, 110: 115–125

    Article  Google Scholar 

  33. Valdez P, Ramírez C, García A, Talamantes J, Cortez J. Circadian and homeostatic variation in sustained attention. Chronobiology International, 2010, 27(2): 393–416

    Article  Google Scholar 

  34. Johnson S W, North R A. Opioids excite dopamine neurons by hyperpolarization of local interneurons. Journal of Neuroscience, 1992, 12(2): 483–488

    Article  Google Scholar 

  35. Carrier J, Monk T H. Circadian rhythms of performance: new trends. Chronobiology International, 2000, 17(6): 719–732

    Article  Google Scholar 

  36. Wright K P Jr, Hull J T, Czeisler C A. Relationship between alertness, performance, and body temperature in humans. American Journal of Physiology. Regulatory, Integrative and Comparative Physiology, 2002, 283(6): R1370–R1377

    Article  Google Scholar 

  37. Bonnefond A, Rohmer O, Hoeft A, Muzet A, Tassi P. Interaction of age with time of day and mental load in different cognitive tasks. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 2003, 96(3 Pt 2): 1223–1236

    Article  Google Scholar 

  38. Babkoff H, Caspy T, Mikulincer M, Sing H C. Monotonic and rhythmic influences: a challenge for sleep deprivation research. Psychological Bulletin, 1991, 109(3): 411–428

    Article  Google Scholar 

  39. Sagaspe P, Sanchez-Ortuno M, Charles A, Taillard J, Valtat C, Bioulac B, Philip P. Effects of sleep deprivation on Color-Word, Emotional, and Specific Stroop interference and on self-reported anxiety. Brain and Cognition, 2006, 60(1): 76–87

    Article  Google Scholar 

  40. Krishnan H C, Lyons L C. Synchrony and desynchrony in circadian clocks: impacts on learning and memory. Learning & Memory (Cold Spring Harbor, N.Y.), 2015, 22(9): 426–437

    Article  Google Scholar 

  41. Dinges D F, Pack F, Williams K, Gillen K A, Powell J W, Ott G E, Aptowicz C, Pack A I. Cumulative sleepiness, mood disturbance, and psychomotor vigilance performance decrements during a week of sleep restricted to 4–5 hours per night. Sleep, 1997, 20(4): 267–277

    Google Scholar 

  42. Gillberg M, Åkerstedt T. Sleep loss and performance: no “safe” duration of a monotonous task. Physiology & Behavior, 1998, 64 (5): 599–604

    Article  Google Scholar 

  43. McCarthy M E, Waters W F. Decreased attentional responsivity during sleep deprivation: orienting response latency, amplitude, and habituation. Sleep, 1997, 20(2): 115–123

    Article  Google Scholar 

  44. De Gennaro L, Ferrara M, Curcio G, Bertini M. Visual search performance across 40 h of continuous wakefulness: Measures of speed and accuracy and relation with oculomotor performance. Physiology & Behavior, 2001, 74(1–2): 197–204

    Article  Google Scholar 

  45. Chee Y L, Crawford J C, Watson H G, Greaves M. Guidelines on the assessment of bleeding risk prior to surgery or invasive procedures. British Journal of Haematology, 2008, 140(5): 496–504

    Article  Google Scholar 

  46. Drummond SPA, Brown G G. The effects of total sleep deprivation on cerebral responses to cognitive performance. Neuropsychopharmacology, 2001, 25(5 Suppl 1): S68–S73

    Article  Google Scholar 

  47. Tomasi D, Wang R L, Telang F, Boronikolas V, Jayne M C, Wang G J, Fowler J S, Volkow N D. Impairment of attentional networks after 1 night of sleep deprivation. Cerebral Cortex, 2009, 19(1): 233–240

    Article  Google Scholar 

  48. Chee M W L, Goh C S F, Namburi P, Parimal S, Seidl K N, Kastner S. Effects of sleep deprivation on cortical activation during directed attention in the absence and presence of visual stimuli. NeuroImage, 2011, 58(2): 595–604

    Article  Google Scholar 

  49. De Havas J A, Parimal S, Soon C S, Chee M W. Sleep deprivation reduces default mode network connectivity and anti-correlation during rest and task performance. NeuroImage, 2012, 59(2): 1745–1751

    Article  Google Scholar 

  50. Muto V, Jaspar M, Meyer C, Kussé C, Chellappa S L, Degueldre C, Balteau E, Shaffii-Le Bourdiec A, Luxen A, Middleton B, Archer S N, Phillips C, Collette F, Vandewalle G, Dijk D J, Maquet P. Local modulation of human brain responses by circadian rhythmicity and sleep debt. Science, 2016, 353(6300): 687–690

    Article  Google Scholar 

  51. Miyake A, Shah P. Models of Working Memory: Mechanisms of Active Maintenance and Executive Control. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 1999, 506

    Book  Google Scholar 

  52. Baddeley A. The fractionation of working memory. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 1996, 93(24): 13468–13472

    Article  Google Scholar 

  53. Awh E, Jonides J, Smith E E, Buxton R B, Frank L R, Love T, Wong E C, Gmeindl L. Rehearsal in spatial working memory: evidence from neuroimaging. Psychological Science, 1999, 10(5): 433–437

    Article  Google Scholar 

  54. Stern C E, Sherman S J, Kirchhoff B A, Hasselmo M E. Medial temporal and prefrontal contributions to working memory tasks with novel and familiar stimuli. Hippocampus, 2001, 11(4): 337–346

    Article  Google Scholar 

  55. McDowell S, Whyte J, D’Esposito M. Working memory impairments in traumatic brain injury: evidence from a dual-task paradigm. Neuropsychologia, 1997, 35(10): 1341–1353

    Article  Google Scholar 

  56. Gerstner J R, Yin J C P. Circadian rhythms and memory formation. Nature Reviews. Neuroscience, 2010, 11(8): 577–588

    Google Scholar 

  57. Domagalik A, Oginska H, Beldzik E, Fafrowicz M, Pokrywka M, Chaniecki P, Marek T. Long-term reduction of short wavelength light affects sustained attention and visuospatial working memory. bioRxiv, 2019, 581314

  58. Laird D A. Relative performance of college students as conditioned by time of day and day of week. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1925, 8(1): 50–63

    Article  Google Scholar 

  59. Vedhara K, Hyde J, Gilchrist I D, Tytherleigh M, Plummer S. Acute stress, memory, attention and cortisol. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 2000, 25(6): 535–549

    Article  Google Scholar 

  60. Potter P, Wolf L, Boxerman S, Grayson D, Sledge J, Dunagan C, Evanoff B. Understanding the cognitive work of nursing in the acute care environment. Journal of Nursing Administration, 2005, 35(7–8): 327–335

    Google Scholar 

  61. Blake M JF. Time of day effects on performance in a range of tasks. Psychonomic Science, 1967, 9(6): 349–350

    Article  Google Scholar 

  62. Rowe G, Hasher L, Turcotte J. Age and synchrony effects in visuospatial working memory. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 2009, 62(10): 1873–1880

    Article  Google Scholar 

  63. Folkard S. Time of day and level of processing. Memory & Cognition, 1979, 7(4): 247–252

    Article  Google Scholar 

  64. Lewandowska K, Wachowicz B, Marek T, Oginska H, Fafrowicz M. Would you say “yes” in the evening? Time-of-day effect on response bias in four types of working memory recognition tasks. Chronobiology International, 2018, 35(1): 80–89

    Article  Google Scholar 

  65. Chee M W L, Choo W C. Functional imaging of working memory after 24 hr of total sleep deprivation. Journal of Neuroscience, 2004, 24(19): 4560–4567

    Article  Google Scholar 

  66. Choo W C, Lee W W, Venkatraman V, Sheu F S, Chee M W. Dissociation of cortical regions modulated by both working memory load and sleep deprivation and by sleep deprivation alone. NeuroImage, 2005, 25(2): 579–587

    Article  Google Scholar 

  67. Mu Q, Mishory A, Johnson K A, Nahas Z, Kozel F A, Yamanaka K, Bohning D E, George M S. Decreased brain activation during a working memory task at rested baseline is associated with vulnerability to sleep deprivation. Sleep, 2005, 28(4): 433–448

    Article  Google Scholar 

  68. Chee M W L, Chuah L Y M, Venkatraman V, Chan W Y, Philip P, Dinges D F. Functional imaging of working memory following normal sleep and after 24 and 35 h of sleep deprivation: Correlations of fronto-parietal activation with performance. NeuroImage, 2006, 31(1): 419–428

    Article  Google Scholar 

  69. Lim J, Choo W C, Chee M W L. Reproducibility of changes in behaviour and fMRI activation associated with sleep deprivation in a working memory task. Sleep (Basel), 2007, 30(1): 61–70

    Article  Google Scholar 

  70. Honma M, Soshi T, Kim Y, Kuriyama K. Right prefrontal activity reflects the ability to overcome sleepiness during working memory tasks: a functional near-infrared spectroscopy study. PLoS One, 2010, 5(9): e12923

    Article  Google Scholar 

  71. McKenna B S, Eyler L T. Overlapping prefrontal systems involved in cognitive and emotional processing in euthymic bipolar disorder and following sleep deprivation: a review of functional neuroimaging studies. Clinical Psychology Review, 2012, 32(7): 650–663

    Article  Google Scholar 

  72. Thomas R J, Rosen B R, Stern C E, Weiss J W, Kwong K K. Functional imaging of working memory in obstructive sleep-disordered breathing. Journal of Applied Physiology, 2005, 98(6): 2226–2234

    Article  Google Scholar 

  73. McKenna B S, Sutherland A N, Legenkaya A P, Eyler L T. Abnormalities of brain response during encoding into verbal working memory among euthymic patients with bipolar disorder. Bipolar Disorders, 2014, 16(3): 289–299

    Article  Google Scholar 

  74. Drummond S P A, Walker M, Almklov E, Campos M, Anderson D E, Straus L D. Neural correlates of working memory performance in primary insomnia. Sleep (Basel), 2013, 36(9): 1307–1316

    Article  Google Scholar 

  75. Stroop J R. Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. Journal of Experimental Psychology. General, 1992, 121(1): 15–23

    Article  Google Scholar 

  76. Hartley L R, Shirley E. Color-name interference at different times of day. Journal of Applied Psychology, 1976, 61(1): 119–122

    Article  Google Scholar 

  77. Manly T, Lewis G H, Robertson IH, Watson P C, Datta A. Coffee in the cornflakes: time-of-day as a modulator of executive response control. Neuropsychologia, 2002, 40(1): 1–6

    Article  Google Scholar 

  78. Harrison Y, Jones K, Waterhouse J. The influence of time awake and circadian rhythm upon performance on a frontal lobe task. Neuropsychologia, 2007, 45(8): 1966–1972

    Article  Google Scholar 

  79. Bratzke D, Steinborn M B, Rolke B, Ulrich R. Effects of sleep loss and circadian rhythm on executive inhibitory control in the Stroop and Simon tasks. Chronobiology International, 2012, 29(1): 55–61

    Article  Google Scholar 

  80. Schmidt C, Peigneux P, Leclercq Y, Sterpenich V, Vandewalle G, Phillips C, Berthomier P, Berthomier C, Tinguely G, Gais S, Schabus M, Desseilles M, Dang-Vu T, Salmon E, Degueldre C, Balteau E, Luxen A, Cajochen C, Maquet P, Collette F. Circadian preference modulates the neural substrate of conflict processing across the day. PLoS One, 2012, 7(1): e29658

    Article  Google Scholar 

  81. Miller E K, Cohen J D. An integrative theory of prefrontal cortex function. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 2001, 24(1): 167–202

    Article  Google Scholar 

  82. Dove A, Pollmann S, Schubert T, Wiggins C J, von Cramon D Y. Prefrontal cortex activation in task switching: an event-related fMRI study. Brain Research. Cognitive Brain Research, 2000, 9(1): 103–109

    Article  Google Scholar 

  83. Heuer H, Kleinsorge T, Klein W, Kohlisch O. Total sleep deprivation increases the costs of shifting between simple cognitive tasks. Acta Psychologica, 2004, 117(1): 29–64

    Article  Google Scholar 

  84. Bratzke D, Rolke B, Steinborn M B, Ulrich R. The effect of 40 h constant wakefulness on task-switching efficiency. Journal of Sleep Research, 2009, 18(2): 167–172

    Article  Google Scholar 

  85. Shinkai S, Watanabe S, Kurokawa Y, Torii J. Salivary cortisol for monitoring circadian rhythm variation in adrenal activity during shiftwork. International Archives of Occupational and Environmental Health, 1993, 64(7): 499–502

    Article  Google Scholar 

  86. Ramírez C, García A, Valdez P. Identification of circadian rhythms in cognitive inhibition and flexibility using a Stroop task. Sleep and Biological Rhythms, 2012, 10(2): 136–144

    Article  Google Scholar 

  87. Mednick S A. The associative basis of the creative process. Psychological Review, 1962, 69(3): 220–232

    Article  Google Scholar 

  88. Sherman S M, Mumford J A, Schnyer D M. Hippocampal activity mediates the relationship between circadian activity rhythms and memory in older adults. Neuropsychologia, 2015, 75: 617–625

    Article  Google Scholar 

  89. May C P. Synchrony effects in cognition: the costs and a benefit. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 1999, 6(1): 142–147

    Article  Google Scholar 

  90. Wieth M B, Zacks R T. Time of day effects on problem solving: When the non-optimal is optimal. Thinking & Reasoning, 2011, 17 (4): 387–401

    Article  Google Scholar 

  91. West R, Murphy K J, Armilio M L, Craik F I, Stuss D T. Effects of time of day on age differences in working memory. Journals of Gerontology. Series B, Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 2002, 57(1): 3–10

    Article  Google Scholar 

  92. Lu F M, Yuan Z. PET/SPECT molecular imaging in clinical neuroscience: recent advances in the investigation of CNS diseases. Quantitative Imaging in Medicine and Surgery, 2015, 5(3): 433–447

    Google Scholar 

  93. Chen B, Moreland J, Zhang J. Human brain functional MRI and DTI visualization with virtual reality. Quantitative Imaging in Medicine and Surgery, 2011, 1(1): 11–16

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

This study was supported by MYRG2019-00082-FHS and MYRG2018-00081-FHS Grants from the University of Macau, as wellasFDCT025/2015/A1 and FDCT0011/2018/A1 Grants from the Macau Government.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Zhen Yuan.

Additional information

Shiyang Xu is a Ph.D. student at Faculty of Health Sciences, Centre for Cognitive and Brain Sciences, University of Macau, China. He received the Master’s degree in Developmental and Educational Psychology from Shaanxi Normal University, China. He concentrates on the neuroimaging (EEG/fNIRS) with disease and children development.

Miriam Akioma is a full-time Ph.D. research scholar at the Centre for Cognitive and Brain Sciences in Faculty of Health Sciences, University of Macau, China. She holds Master’s degree in Applied Linguistics, and Specialization in Developmental Psychology. Her current research focuses on neuroimaging techniques for mapping the neural mechanisms and correlates related to language production, and comprehension in the bilingual brain. For that, she makes use of recent high-technologies such as fNIRS, EEG, fMRI, and eye-tracking.

Zhen Yuan is an associate professor with Faculty of Health Sciences (FHS) at University of Macau (UM), China. He also has the second appointment as the Interim Director of Centre for Cognitive and Brain Sciences at UM. His academic investigation is focused on cutting-edge research and development in laser, ultrasound and EEG/fMRI-related biomedical technologies as well as their clinical/pre-clinical applications in cognitive neuroscience and brain disorders. He has achieved national and international recognition through over 200 SCI publications in high ranked journals in his field. He is the editorial board member of Quantitative Imaging in Medicine and Surgery, editor of Journal of Innovative Optical Health Sciences, senior associate editor of BMC Medical Imaging, and associate editor of Frontiers in Human Neuroscience. He is a senior member of OSA and SPIE, and committee member of Chinese Biomedical Optical Society.

Supporting Information

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Xu, S., Akioma, M. & Yuan, Z. Relationship between circadian rhythm and brain cognitive functions. Front. Optoelectron. 14, 278–287 (2021). https://doi.org/10.1007/s12200-021-1090-y

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s12200-021-1090-y

Keywords

Navigation