The COVID-19 pandemic has undoubtedly had a negative effect on individuals’ quality of life through multiple means, such as fear in the chronically ill, isolation in those with mental health concerns, challenges to maintaining a healthy lifestyle, and those directly harmed by contracting the coronaviruses (Gamberini et al., 2021; Huang et al., 2022; Koc et al., 2022; Rogers et al., 2021; Shoychet et al., 2022). Recent research demonstrates that the COVID-19 pandemic has negatively affected every domain that contributes to one’s quality of life as defined by the World Health Organization (World Health Organization, 1996) including social activities, long-term health problems, environmental factors, and diminished psychological well-being through mental health concerns (Alkire et al., 2021; Amdal et al., 2021; Brülhart et al., 2021; Fried et al., 2022; Saikia et al., 2021; Stamatis et al., 2022; Stone, 2022). Some researchers suggest that the coronaviruses that cause COVID-19 will likely not be eradicated soon (Case et al., 2021; Ioannidis, 2022), which suggests that learning about how to improve individuals’ quality of life, despite the presence of COVID-19, may be an effective means of mitigating some of the negative effects of the COVID-19 pandemic (Huang et al., 2021; Lucas et al., 2021; Rocco et al., 2021). Thus, research that clarifies how the COVID-19 pandemic negatively affects individuals' quality of life may contribute to the development of tools, interventions, and protocols that professionals can use to enhance people's quality of life.

One of the determinants that emerged as a negative correlate of quality of life is the fear of and anxiety about the COVID-19 pandemic, which is labeled as the perceived danger of COVID-19. Five studies to date have demonstrated this negative relationship between the perceived danger of COVID-19 and quality of life, all of which were cross-sectional (Demirbas et al., 2021; Gönenç et al., 2022; Hayes et al., 2021; Naghizadeh et al., 2021; Shoychet et al., 2022). Three of these studies focused on health-related populations (i.e., irritable bowel disease, pregnancy, and sexual health), and the effects were found in Canada, Turkey, Iran, and Australia, but researchers have yet to demonstrate these effects in the United States. Note also that none of these studies report the percentage of their sample who have had COVID-19 or if their effects differ between those who have had and not had COVID-19. It is reasonable to expect that one’s perception of the danger of COVID-19 and one’s quality of life may be affected by directly experiencing the disease, however, this group difference remains untested in the literature. Further, although one of these studies demonstrated that intolerance of uncertainty and health locus of control moderate this relationship (Shoychet et al., 2022), no studies to date have provided empirical evidence for potential explanations for why this relationship exists.

A potential explanation for why a negative relationship exists between the perceived danger of COVID-19 and quality of life may be due to the life changes caused by adjusting to the COVID-19 pandemic. Specifically, although it appears as if the perceived danger of COVID-19 is directly negatively related to an individual’s quality of life, dozens of studies have demonstrated that the perceived danger of COVID-19 is related to multiple life changes caused by the pandemic, and other studies have demonstrated that these life changes are directly related to an individual’s quality of life. For example, several studies have demonstrated that the fear of COVID-19 is negatively related to poor mental health (Belen, 2022; Duong, 2021; Kayis et al., 2021; Khattak et al., 2020; Khudaykulov et al., 2022; Mauer et al., 2022; Peker & Cengiz, 2021; Suhail et al., 2022; Vilca et al., 2022; Yenen & Çarkit, 2021), difficulties living with physical health concerns (Ahmed et al., 2021; Buran & Gerçek Öter, 2021a, 2021b; Duong, 2021), and vocational difficulties (De los Santos & Labrague, 2021; Khattak et al., 2020; Khudaykulov et al., 2022; Saracoglu et al., 2020). Further, numerous studies have found that life disruptions from COVID-19 affect domains that are involved in one’s quality of life (e.g., mental health or physical health) or quality of life measured directly (Bhogal et al., 2021; Buran & Gerçek Öter, 2021a, 2021b; Egede et al., 2022; Handberg et al., 2021; Koenig et al., 2021; Kurzhals et al., 2021; Saracoglu et al., 2020; Sugawara et al., 2021; Trucco et al., 2022). Thus, despite the numerous studies that support the indirect effect of the perceived danger of COVID-19 on quality of life through the intervening variable of COVID-19 life changes, no study to date has modeled this effect.

Evidence for the importance of stressful life changes explaining the relationship between emotion and mental health outcomes may come from the diathesis-stress model (Zuckerman, 1999). This model is a biosocial model supported by human and animal research that suggests that the development of mental illness comes from two potential and general factors which are 1) a biological or genetic factor (diathesis) and 2) stressful life situations. This model emphasizes that normal emotional experiences in individuals who are predisposed to mental illnesses may become dysfunctional when in the presence of stressful situations. In the case of COVID-19, normal fear and anxiety around COVID-19 may be exacerbated by the stressful life changes caused by COVID-19, leading to a reduction in quality of life. Specifically, this model suggests that the fear and anxiety of COVID-19 itself may not become problematic until the individual experiences the stressful situations caused by COVID-19. Thus, the relationship between fear and anxiety over COVID-19 may only exist because of the stressful life changes from COVID-19. The current study aims to find empirical evidence of the necessity of COVID-19 life changes and argue that without the stressful situations caused by COVID-19 life changes that the relationship between the perceived danger of COVID-19 and quality of life may not be significant. This study would be the first to apply the diathesis-stress model to the COVID-19 pandemic and quality of life.

To model these effects, I tested a full structural equation model involving three latent factors which are, 1) the perceived danger of COVID-19, 2) COVID-19 life changes, and 3) quality of life. The perceived danger of COVID-19 factor is comprised of two scales (i.e., fear of COVID-19 and anxiety from COVID-19). The COVID-19 life changes factor is comprised of eight scales designed to measure various domains of living disrupted by the COVID-19 pandemic (i.e., finances, loved ones, job, safety, school, mental health, physical health, and social connections). The quality of life factor is comprised of the four subscales of quality of life as defined by the World Health Organization (i.e., physical health, psychological health, environment, and relationships). I specified three direct relationships, which are 1) the perceived danger of COVID-19 onto COVID-19 life changes, 2) COVID-19 life changes onto quality of life, and 3) the perceived danger of COVID-19 onto quality of life (see Fig. 1). To test the explanatory power of COVID-19 life changes I specified an indirect effect of the perceived danger of COVID-19 on quality of life through the intervening variable of COVID-19 life changes. Finally, I tested if these effects persist in those who have had COVID-19 compared to those who have not had COVID-19, as these group differences remain untested in the literature.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Hypothesized Structural Model. Note. COVID-19 life changes explain the relationship between the perceived danger of COVID-19 and quality of life

The current study aims to address and progress several limitations and absent findings in the literature. First, I will replicate the positive relationship between the perceived danger of COVID-19 and quality of life in a United States sample. This relationship may even be stronger than in other countries because the United States has one of the highest prevalence rates of COVID-19 (World Health Organization, 2022). Second, this is the first study that tested the indirect effect and full explanatory power of the perceived danger of COVID-19 on quality of life through the intervening variable of COVID-19 life changes. Although the literature supports all these relationships independently using bivariate correlations, this is the first study to test them all simultaneously. Third, this is the only study to date that compares these relationships between those who have had COVID-19 and those who have not had COVID-19. Finally, this is the only study to date that employs structural equation modeling (SEM), which has several benefits over bivariate correlations including 1) simultaneous multivariate testing of relationships, 2) the ability to control for the effects of each independent relationship, and 3) the use of latent variable scores over observed scores (Kline, 2016). I examine these important relationships and tested for between-group differences to guide future research on improving individuals’ quality of life, despite living during the COVID-19 pandemic.

Method

Participants

Participants were undergraduate students (n = 224) from a Midwestern University in the United States who participated in the study for course credit. Individuals who failed either of the two attention checks were removed (e.g., please select moderately true; n = 28; 12.50%), which resulted in a final sample of 196 participants. The complete demographic information and the demographic information for those who have had and not had COVID-19 separately are presented in Table 1. Note that there were no significant differences in demographics or sample size between the group who acquired COVID-19 and those who did not. Thus, I can attribute any differences in the structural equation models to the differences in contracting COVID-19, not demographic variation. All the data, scripts, and materials for the current study are on an Open Science Framework page, which can be accessed at the following link: https://osf.io/3yqtv/?view_only=f3800dc28cf74f739b46ac8c0296ad9a

Table 1 Demographic characteristics (n = 196)

Procedure

I guided participants to complete the study via the SONA Systems website (SONA Systems, Ltd., Tallinn, Estonia) during the Spring of 2022. Participants completed an informed consent document, a demographic survey, a survey consisting of several measures of the effects of COVID-19, a measure of their quality of life, and then a debriefing form. I awarded the participants course credit for their participation. The entire study took no longer than 30 min.

Measures

For internal consistencies, descriptive statistics, and mean differences between groups see Table 2. Note that there were no significant differences in the means between the group who acquired COVID-19 and those who did not. Thus, any differences in the structural equation models can be attributed to the differences in the relationships among the variables, not mean differences across groups.

Table 2 Descriptive statistics and differences between groups among the scales used in the current study (n = 196)

Fear of COVID-19 scale

The Fear of COVID-19 scale is a 7-item unidimensional measure of individuals’ general fear about COVID-19 (Ahorsu et al., 2020). The participants rated items on a 5-point Likert-type scale from 1 = strongly disagree to 5 = strongly agree. The scale has a strong unidimensional factor structure, convergent validity, and internal consistency.

COVID anxiety scale

The COVID-19 Anxiety Scale is a unidimensional measure of anxiety about the COVID-19 pandemic (Lee, 2020). Individuals rate five items on a 5-point scale from 1 = not at all to 5 = nearly every day for the last two weeks. The scale has a robust unidimensional structure in both a principal components analysis and confirmatory factor analysis and has demonstrated measurement invariance across age and gender. Further, the scale demonstrated strong construct validity with measures of psychopathology and had a strong internal consistency.

WHOQOL-BREF

The World Health Organization Quality of Life – Bref is an abbreviated version of the World Health Organization Quality of Life-100 scale designed to measure various dimensions of an individual’s quality of life (World Health Organization, 1996). The participants rated 26 items on different 5-point Likert-type scales. The scale contains two global single-item subscales of global quality of life and global health quality of life, which I did not use in the current study. Instead, I used the four remaining subscales as indicators of a quality of life factor, which contains a 1) physical quality of life, 2) psychological quality of life, 3) environmental quality of life, and 4) relationships quality of life. Although the internal consistency in the current study is low, the scale has demonstrated strong psychometric properties and has been translated into multiple languages.

Coronavirus Impact Domains Scale (CIDS)

The Coronavirus Impact Domains Scale (CIDS) is a multidimensional measure of the effects the COVID-19 pandemic has had on individuals’ lives (Stone, 2022). The scale measures nine domains, containing 6-items each, which are 1) Finances, 2) Loved Ones, 3) Job, 4) Safety, 5) School, 6) Mental Health, 7) Physical Health, and 8) Social Activities. The Quality of Life subscale was removed to reduce redundancy between the independent and dependent variables. The participants rate how much an item applies to them on a 5-point scale from 1 = not at all to 5 = very much. The scale demonstrated excellent internal consistency, structural validity, and construct validity in the original study.

Planned analyses

I conducted the analyses for the current paper using IBM SPSS Statistics (IBM Corp. Released 2021. IBM SPSS Statistics for Mac, Version 28.0. Armonk, NY: IBM Corp) and R 3.6.0 (R Core Team 2020, R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, Austria). I used Kline’s (2016) three-step process of assessing model fit, which involves first fitting the model to the data and reporting the exact fit test. In step two, I examine correlational residuals larger than an absolute value of 0.10 to assess for local areas of misfit. Finally, in step three, I report the RMSEA, CFI, and SRMR. I used the lavaan package 0.6–5 (Rosseel, 2012) and the Diagonally Weighted Least Squares estimator for parameter estimates to account for ordinal indicators.

Results

Preliminary analyses

The data were first checked for outliers, skewness, and kurtosis of the scale and subscale scores. Several variables contained outliers as defined as two standard deviations above or below the mean, including the COVID-19 Anxiety Scale (23), physical quality of life (1), environmental quality of life (3), relationship quality of life (6), and Fear of COVID-19 Scale (1). However, all outliers were valid cases, so they were not eliminated from the analyses (Orr et al., 1991). An analysis of skewness and kurtosis revealed no non-normal distributions, which is defined as an absolute value of skewness of greater than two or an absolute value of kurtosis of greater than seven (West et al., 1995), except for the COVID-19 Anxiety Scale with a skewness of 3.73 and kurtosis of 15.19. I did not transform the COVID-19 Anxiety Scale because it was the only nonnormally distributed variable; thus, artifactual factors are not an issue. Skewness for the other variables ranged from -0.44 to 1.35, and kurtosis ranged from -1.09 to 0.94. To conserve power and stabilize the parameter estimates, I used the scale and subscale scores as indicators instead of the individual items. There were no missing data.

Measurement models

The first step to demonstrating a full indirect effect is demonstrating that a direct relationship exists without the specified indirect effect. I demonstrated this relationship by using a two-step process. The first step involved fitting a measurement model involving all the factors covaried. Then, the direct effects are specified, and assess the fit of the structural model. The measurement and structural models in the current study are equivalent, which means they fit the data equally well and share the same indicator loadings, error variances, and correlational residuals (Kline, 2016). Regarding the sample of those who have had COVID-19, the measurement model passed the exact fit test, χ2(74) = 61.58, p = 0.848. After examining the correlational residuals, there were 13 residuals that were larger than an absolute value of 0.10. These residuals were randomly distributed (see Table 3), RMSEA = 0.000, 90% CI[0.000, 0.033], p = 0.991, SRMR = 0.077; CFI = 1.00 (see Model A in Fig. 2). The measurement model in those who have not had COVID-19 followed a similar pattern, where it passed the exact fit test, χ2(74) = 37.71, p > 0.999, with 11 correlational residuals larger than an absolute value of 0.10 distributed in a random pattern, (see Table 3), RMSEA = 0.000, 90% CI[0.000, 0.000], p > 0.999, SRMR = 0.076; CFI = 1.00 (see Model C in Fig. 2). Although I could make modifications to these models by adding error covariances between the areas of misfit suggested by the correlational residuals, given the outstanding fit and lack of strong theoretical justifications, it was not necessary to make modifications due to diminishing returns in model fit. Thus, each measurement model was retained as a plausible explanation of the data.

Table 3 Correlational residuals of the measurement models (n = 196)
Fig. 2
figure 2

Measurement and Structural Models Between Those Who Have Had COVID-19 and Those Who Have Not Had COVID-19. Note. A = Measurement model in the COVID-19 positive sample. B = Structural model in the COVID-19 positive sample. C = Measurement model in the COVID-19 negative sample. D = Structural model in the COVID-19 negative sample, *p < .05

The relationship between the perceived danger of COVID-19 and quality of life was significant and negative in both samples. In those who have had COVID-19, the unstandardized estimate for the covariance was -0.13, SE = 0.02, p < 0.001, and in those who have never had COVID-19 the unstandardized estimate for the covariance was -0.10, SE = 0.3, p < 0.001. Thus, when I fit the structural models, they must meet two criteria to determine if COVID-19 life changes fully explain the relationship between the perceived danger of COVID-19 and quality of life. The first criterion is a significant indirect effect of the perceived danger of COVID-19 on quality of life through the intervening variable of COVID-19 life changes. Second, the relationship between the perceived danger of COVID-19 and quality of life should become nonsignificant (i.e., and thus fully explained by the indirect effect).

Structural models

I fit the structural model by specifying the direct and indirect relationships in those who have had COVID-19 (see Model B in Fig. 2). The analysis met the criteria for a full explanation, with the indirect effect being significant with an unstandardized estimate of -0.18, SE = 0.06, p = 0.003, and the direct effect from the perceived danger of COVID-19 on quality of life becoming nonsignificant with an unstandardized estimate of -0.07, SE = 0.06, p = 0.236 (see Fig. 3). The model explained 40.6% of the variability in quality of life. A different pattern emerged for those who have not had COVID-19, with the indirect effect being nonsignificant with an unstandardized estimate of -0.44, SE = 0.44, p = 0.317 (see Model D in Fig. 2). The only significant direct effect in the structural model for those who have never had COVID-19 was between the perceived danger of COVID-19 and COVID-19 life changes, with an unstandardized estimate of 1.08, SE = 0.51, p = 0.034. Thus, COVID-19 life changes can fully explain the relationship between the perceived danger of COVID-19 and quality of life, but only in those who have had COVID-19.

Fig. 3
figure 3

Structural Model in Those Who Have Had COVID-19. Note. COVID-19 life changes fully explain the relationship between the perceived danger of COVID-19 and quality of life, *p < .05

Post-hoc exploratory control for COVID-19 severity

Although the current study classified those who have had COVID-19 and those who have not had COVID-19 into two groups, it is possible that there are different effects within the group that had COVID-19 as a function of the severity of their COVID-19 symptoms. To control for this range of severity, participants reported how severe their COVID-19 symptoms were on a scale from 1 = asymptomatic to 5 = hospitalization. 20 people reported being asymptomatic (19.05%), 44 mild (41.90%), 30 moderate (28.57%), 8 severe (7.62%), and 2 were hospitalized (1.90%). Thus, 70.48% of the sample reported mild to moderate COVID-19 symptoms, suggesting that the group who reported having COVID-19 was largely symptomatic and homogeneous. Further, an independent samples t-test revealed that the difference in COVID-19 symptoms between those who have had COVID-19 and those who have not was significant and very large, p < 0.001, Cohen’s d = 3.07, suggesting that these groups are starkly different in their experiences of COVID-19.

I specified the severity of COVID-19 to control for quality of life in the measurement model in those who had COVID-19 to examine if the effects found in the study remain, despite the severity of COVID-19. In this measurement model, all three relationships between the factors were significant, ps < 0.001. In the structural model, the relationship between fear of COVID-19 and quality of life was non-significant, p = 0.260, and the indirect effect of fear of COVID-19 on quality of life through the intervening variable of COVID-19 life changes was significant and negative, with an unstandardized parameter estimate of -0.18, SE = 0.6, p = 0.003. Further, the direct effect from severity to quality of life was significant, p < 0.001. Thus, controlling for COVID-19 severity did not change the results. However, when controlling for COVID-19 severity, the R2 increased from 40.6% to 50.1%. This increase is a medium effect, Cohen’s f2 = 0.19. Although controlling for severity does not change the model, it does explain 9.5% more variability in the quality of life factor.

Discussion

The current study attempted to explain the negative relationship between the perceived danger of COVID-19 and quality of life through COVID-19 life changes and to test if this effect occurs differently between those who have had COVID-19 and those who have not. The results suggest that COVID-19 life changes fully explain the replicated effect in the literature of the negative relationship between COVID-19 perceived danger and quality of life, even when controlling for severity of symptoms. Further, I only found this full indirect effect in those who have had COVID-19. In the group that has never had COVID-19, the only significant relationship was the positive relationship between the perceived danger of COVID-19 on COVID-19 life changes. In the following disucssion, I discuss the findings, theoretical implications, practical implications, the limitations of the study, and future directions researchers may consider.

The results suggest that the reason that individuals who have had COVID-19 experience a lower quality of life when their perceived danger of COVID-19 is higher is strictly because of the changes in their lives from the COVID-19 pandemic. Therefore, the perceived danger of COVID-19 may not be the direct risk factor for the worsened quality of life as some of the literature suggests (e.g., Demirbas et al., 2021; Gönenç et al., 2022; Hayes et al., 2021; Naghizadeh et al., 2021; Shoychet et al., 2022). Instead, it is the life changes COVID-19 has forced on people that lower one's quality of life. For example, it appears as if someone experiences fear or anxiety over COVID-19, so they also make or experience life changes to protect themselves (e.g., individuals fear catching COVID-19, so they stop spending time with friends or family, resulting in limited socialization), and then it is these life changes that coincide with the worsened quality of life (e.g., less socialization coincides with mental illness and isolation, resulting in lower quality of life). Thus, although it appears as if the perceived danger of COVID-19 directly negatively relates to quality of life, this study suggests that life changes fully explain this relationship, but only in those who have had COVID-19.

Theoretical implications

The results from this study support the importance of the diathesis-stress model, but only in those who have directly experienced COVID-19. Specifically, the relationship between the experiences of fear and anxiety from COVID-19 may only be related to the quality of life because of the indirect effect of the stressful life situations caused by COVID-19. Thus, this study is the first study to apply the diathesis-stress model to the COVID-19 pandemic, emphasizing that stressful life changes, especially those that happen across domains as measured in the current study, may be a variable that explains why some people who experience a great deal of perceived danger of COVID-19 simultaneously reported worse quality of life compared to those who experience less perceived danger of COVID-19. Thus, this study provides empirical evidence that researchers and clinicians may consider the stressful life changes caused by COVID-19 to potentially be a precipitating factor in their case conceptualizations.

The current study further suggests that future research may consider the importance of the various ways a pandemic affects the areas of individuals' life, creating stress and thus vulnerability to psychopathology. The current study and the CIDS validation study (Stone, 2022) suggest that a pandemic can create stressful life circumstances across multiple domains, thus increasing the vulnerability of individuals predisposed to developing psychopathology. As such, some literature may generalize that it is catching the disease during a pandemic that causes the stress, and the current study provides some evidence of that generalization by only finding the indirect effect in those who have had COVID-19. Still, researchers and clinicians must not understate the indirect effects (e.g., reduced social contact or financial insecurity) of living in a pandemic on mental health outcomes. In this sense, living during a pandemic may pose a unique and robust vulnerability to those predisposed to psychopathology because of its effects on potentially all domains of one’s life.

Study outcome & practical implications

This indirect effect has two notable practical implications. First, interventions targeting the perceived danger of COVID-19 with the hope of improving individuals' quality of life may not be as potent as remedying the effects of COVID-19 on individuals' lives directly. Interventions aimed at reducing fear and anxiety around COVID-19 may be relevant to those with mental health concerns (Bendau et al., 2021), but generally may not be as effective for improving the general public’s quality of life. Second, for those who have had COVID-19 and are experiencing difficulties with their quality of life, intervention strategies remedying the life changes from COVID-19 may result in improved quality of life. However, the evidence present in the current study is just correlative, so this implication is speculative. Unfortunately, these intervention strategies to reduce and eradicate the coronaviruses that cause COVID-19, such as masking, vaccines, lockdowns, and medicines, have been met with hesitancy and contention in the United States (Finney-Rutten et al., 2021; Nadanovsky & Dos Santos, 2020). Generally, the results of this study suggest that researchers may want to shift away from the perceived danger of COVID-19 and onto the life changes of COVID-19 in those who have had the disease.

The current study also found that the indirect effect of the perceived danger of COVID-19 on quality of life through the intervening variable of COVID-19 life changes varies as a function of having COVID-19. In the sample that reported never having COVID-19, the only significant relationship was a positive relationship between the perceived danger of COVID-19 and COVID-19 life changes. Interestingly, this effect emerged despite no significant group differences in sample size, demographics, means, and standard deviations of each subscale. Thus, the only remaining explanation is that for people who have never had COVID-19, the perceived danger of COVID-19 and quality of life are not related when specifying the hypothesized direct effects. In addition to having no significant group differences in sample size, demographics, means, or standard deviations of each subscale, this study is the first in the literature to find a group difference in these effects, making it challenging to speculate as to why these group differences occurred.

Although interpreting the structural model in the group that has never contracted COVID-19 is possible, it is merely speculative because there is no literature or between-group differences to support the conclusions. However, the data suggest that in those who have never had COVID-19, their perceived danger of COVID-19 positively coincides with the life changes from COVID-19 (e.g., someone who experiences a great deal of perceived COVID-19 danger may result in less time in socializing with others). However, the difference between groups occurs when those who have never had COVID-19 report no effects on their quality of life. Thus, in those people who have not experienced COVID-19, their life changes from COVID-19 appear to have no relationship to their quality of life when controlling for their perceived danger of COVID-19. This finding is a particularly novel finding since many studies that include mixed samples of those who have had and not had COVID-19 have found that the impact of COVID-19 on individuals' lives affects their quality of life (e.g., Bhogal et al., 2021; Kurzhals et al., 2021; Saracoglu et al., 2020), however, this study suggests that these effects are primarily driven by those who have had COVID-19.

There are some important practical implications of these findings. First, for people who have not had COVID-19, interventions focusing on alleviating fear and anxiety about COVID-19 or the life changes from COVID-19 that attempt to improve individuals' quality of life may not be as helpful as previously thought (e.g., Demirbas et al., 2021; Gönenç et al., 2022; Kurzhals et al., 2021; Saracoglu et al., 2020). Second, the significant covariances between 1) perceived danger of COVID-19 and quality of life and 2) COVID-19 life change found in the measurement model may be illusory because they disappear once each relationship is controlled for the other. Thus, in other studies involving those who have not had COVID-19, these relationships may be overstated if they are conducted in isolation of each other. Finally, the question remains of if individuals who have never had COVID-19 report similar amounts of perceived danger, life changes, and quality of life to those who have had COVID-19, yet neither perceived danger nor life changes are related to their quality of life, then what explains the variability in quality in life in those who have never had COVID-19? This question remains an area for future research.

Limitations & future directions

This study has several important limitations for which researchers can build upon in future studies. First, the most significant limitation of these data is that they are cross-sectional. Cross-sectional data collection limits our ability to predict outcomes and demonstrate a mediation effect. The data do not clarify the temporal precedence of these relationships; thus, the results may only be interpreted as a ‘snapshot’ of these relationships across individuals. Future researchers may consider detecting the effects found in this study using a longitudinal method. Second, there are some generalizability constraints from this sample, which is mostly White, cis-gender undergraduates. Future researchers may want to replicate these effects in a more representative sample or in a community setting to demonstrate the generalizability of these effects. Third, although the models demonstrated outstanding fit given the exact fit test and fit indices, there were multiple large correlational residuals in both models. The conservative route was chosen for not making modifications to the models to make them fit better, but future researchers may want to examine the effects of these modifications in more detail. Fourth, the sample size for each model is relatively small for the expectations of structural equation modeling, which recommend 5 to 10 observations per parameter estimate (Bentler & Chou, 1987; Bollen, 1989; Nunnally, 1967). The models in the current study had about three observations per parameter estimate. This small sample size may make our estimates more unstable than if there was a larger sample. Future researchers may consider collecting a larger sample size to estimate the stability of the effects found in the current study. Finally, the model for those who have had COVID-19 while controlling for the severity of their symptoms explained 50.1% of the variability in their quality of life. Although this explained variability is substantial, there are other variables not measured in the current study that may explain the remaining 49.9% of the variability (i.e., risk perceptions, personality, or health coverage). Future researchers may want to consider exploring these extraneous variables.

Conclusion

The current study aimed to explain the negative relationship between the perceived danger of COVID-19 and quality of life through COVID-19 life changes and to test if this effect occurs differently between those who have had COVID-19 and those who have not. The results confirmed my hypothesis, but only in those that have had COVID-19. This study may guide future research away from the perceived danger of COVID-19 and onto remedying life changes from COVID-19. The current study and future research in this topic area may improve individuals’ quality of life and help make living with COVID-19 more manageable in the coming years.