The Impostor Phenomenon (IP) describes the tendency of successful individuals to attribute successes externally and failures internally, leading to a discrepancy of objective success and a subjective sense of incompetence and self-doubt (Clance, 1985; Clance & Imes, 1978). The discrepancy between self- and reflected appraisal results in a feeling of fraudulence and fear of being exposed as an impostor (Kolligian & Sternberg, 1991). This dysfunctional pattern of cognitions is to be distinguished from actual imposture, in which a person fraudulently seeks to give the impression of being better than he or she actually is (Dunning & Kruger, 1999). To protect their poor self-esteem from negative judgment, impostors tend to adopt maladaptive work styles when faced with upcoming performance situations. They either overprepare excessively (procrastination), or postpone starting a task until the last possible moment and work in a frenzied manner (procrastination; Sakulku & Alexander, 2011). The consequence of both working styles is the attribution of their successes to extraordinary effort, luck, or sympathy (Clance, 1985) and, conversely, the attribution of failures to their incompetence (Brauer & Wolf, 2016).

The IP is a continuous individual difference variable that is stable over time and has a multidimensional theoretical foundation (Mak et al., 2019). This personality trait is characterized by feelings of inferiority, fear, self-deprecation, and inclinations to an external locus of control. The IP shares overlap with the DSM-III-R Cluster C (Ross & Krukowski, 2003) and shows strong associations with neuroticism (Ibrahim et al., 2020), depression (McGregor et al., 2008), and self-criticism (Kolligian & Sternberg, 1991). Impostors exhibit performance-related anxiety (Fried-Buchalter, 1992) and lower academic and global self-confidence (Thompson, 1994) but, in contrast, also high self-expectations (Clance, 1985) and maladaptive perfectionism (Pannhausen et al., 2020). The impostor’s self-esteem is dependent on outstanding performance, and the contrast between perfectionistic strivings and fear of failure results in a contrasting achievement motivation comprising approach and avoidance orientations (Ross & Krukowski, 2003; Whitman & Shanine, 2012 The motivational discrepancy explains the increased stress response; for instance, burnout components (emotional exhaustion and depersonalization) are enhanced in impostors (Villwock et al., 2016). Their hypercritical self-appraisals and dysfunctional strive for perfection are based on the need to maintain their positive public image and protect their self-esteem, as illustrated by the connection between IP and tendencies to self-handicapping (Want & Kleitman, 2006). Also, the lower expressed performance expectations in public but not in private sittings indicate the self-presentational quality of the IP (Leary et al., 2000). Impostors are therefore more performance-goal oriented. In addition, impostors show less confidence in their intelligence (Kumar & Jagacinski, 2006). Both aspects represent an intersection with the construct (fixed) mindset, characterized by an implicit theory of intelligence as fixed and self-presentational concerns in achievement situations (Elliot & Dweck, 1988).

Fixed Mindset

The dimensional construct mindset describes the implicit theory of intelligence. The poles of this continuum are the fixed mindset, the belief that intelligence and abilities are unchangeable (entity theory), and the growth mindset, the belief that intelligence and abilities are malleable (incremental theory). Research shows that individuals with a fixed mindset attribute failure internally to their abilities (Dweck, 2006). Therefore, mistakes reduce their performance to a greater extent compared with people showing a growth mindset (Dweck & Yeager, 2019). In addition, entity theorists attribute their success more externally (Licht & Dweck, 1984), similar to impostors (Brauer & Wolf, 2016). Also, the belief for effort as evidence of low intelligence and ability is present in both impostors (Clance & O’Toole, 1987) and entity theorists (Miele et al., 2013). Individuals with a fixed mindset exhibit a performance-goal orientation (Dweck & Leggett, 1988), leading to an increased expression in impression management (Burnette et al., 2013). The result is an inhibited perception of learning opportunities and the avoidance of performance situations (Dinger et al., 2013). The intention to present one’s intelligence is prioritized over developing oneself through mistakes and performance feedback. Self-presentational concerns in achievement tasks are also present in impostors, who seek to protect their self-worth (Ferrari & Thompson, 2006). Like impostors, entity theorists view negative feedback as an indicator of low ability, leading to increased affective appraisal of information relative to the self (Mangels et al., 2006). King (2017) showed that a fixed mindset is positively related to negative but not positive affect. Nevertheless, a fixed mindset inhibits the positive affect during challenging achievement tasks due to self-deprecation when extra effort is required to succeed (Dweck, 2006; Robins & Pals, 2002). The constructs mindset and IP show various intersections, such as avoiding achievement situations, a dysfunctional attributional style, and the high priority for maintaining a positive public image. Due to the theoretical overlap of the constructs and a successful IP coaching that addressed mindset in particular (Zanchetta et al., 2020), we examine the relationship between mindset and the IP in this study.

The non-self-serving attribution of impostors

Attributions intend to explain events and their origin. Causal attributions can range on the dimensions of globality (whether the cause appears in all or specific situations only), locality (the extent to which the cause is within the person), and stability (the extent to which the cause is changeable). The internal, stable, and global attribution of success and the external, unstable, and non-global attribution of failure is called the self-serving attributional style. This attribution pattern maintains self-esteem and fosters future expectations of achievement success (Seligman, 2006; Brauer & Wolf, 2016) found that, contrary to the self-serving attributional style, impostors show external-instable attributions in positive performance situations while controlling for depression. Conversely, an internal-stable attribution was only shown in negative performance situations when not controlling for depression. This maladaptive attribution pattern is a core component of the IP (Clance, 1985).

Interestingly, Morris and Tiggemann (2013) found a relationship between academic success and a stable and global failure attribution in high-achieving students, which they explained with the IP. According to the impostor cycle, high-achieving students show an increased fear of failure and achievement pressure (Kolligian & Sternberg, 1991), leading to pro- or precrastination in achievement tasks (Want & Kleitman, 2006). Furthermore, the success could lead to a perceived elevation of their public image. With the build-up of non-internalized successes, the discrepancy between the perceived public expectations and one’s competence perception increases. The result could be developing fraudulent ideations (Sakulku & Alexander, 2011), which lead to higher tendencies in impression management (Ferrari & Thompson, 2006) and the feeling of being an impostor. Therefore, the external-instable attribution of success is considered a central component of the IP.

Correlational studies indicated the internal-stable-global failure attribution and external-stable success attribution in impostors (Ibrahim et al., 2021). Nevertheless, the effect of the IP expression on the external-instable attribution of success and internal-stable attribution of failure has not been experimentally validated yet. In a sample of students, Cozzarelli and Major (1990) found that impostors showed an increased expression of defensive pessimism and more fear of exams than non-impostors, despite finding no differences in objective indicators of performance. Impostors also showed a lower self-serving attributional style in the face of failure and attributed the failure more to low ability. After subjective failure, impostors, therefore, felt worse and showed a greater reduction in self-esteem than non-impostors. However, in the case of personal success, impostors did not differ in their mood or self-confidence from non-impostors. Cozzarelli and Major (1990) stated that their findings regarding a common affective reaction after success indicate a reformulation of the construct because the IP characteristic of discounting praise (Clance, 1985) seemed not to apply in impostors.

Thompson et al. (1998) also investigated the attributional style of impostors using a vignette experiment, which simulated hypothetical situations describing success or failure. Again, impostors attributed failure more internally and generally than non-impostors. Nevertheless, they found no significant correlation between the IP and the external attributions of success.

In a between-subject design, Thompson et al. (2000) examined the IP using a Stroop color-word task and inducing either a success or failure condition by displaying high- or low-frequency mistakes. They found that the IP was related to perfectionistic concern over mistakes, anxiety, and negative affect. However, they found no interaction between the impostor expression and success (low-frequency mistakes condition) on external attribution. Thus, the external attribution of success could not yet be experimentally validated.

Due to the central importance of the external attribution of success in the theoretical formulation of the IP (Sakulku & Alexander, 2011), the lack of experimental validation so far raises an important question. Does the theoretical construct need to be reformulated, or is a different experimental design required to capture the impostors’ external attribution of success?

Prior experiments divided the test subjects into impostors and non-impostors, despite the dimensionality of the construct. In addition, the samples compared were partly uneven and small (Thompson et al., 2000). Further, the vignette experiment and the Stroop test may not have sufficient importance for the participants to find the theoretically postulated effects on attributional style and affect.

Aim of this study

Our study intends to examine the interaction of positive and negative feedback and the IP on the attributional dimensions locus of causality and stability. In addition, we want to investigate the relationship between mindset and the IP. Due to the lower internal locus of control in impostors (Brauer & Wolf, 2016), we hypothesize the following relationships:

H1

The IP expression and personal control correlate negatively.

H2

The IP expression and external control correlate positively.

The theoretical conception of the IP proposes a non-self-serving attribution style of success and failure, which was psychometrically (Ibrahim et al., 2021), but not experimentally supported. In an experimental between-subjects design, we want to examine the moderating role of the IP in a positive or negative feedback condition. Therefore, we hypothesize:

H3

The IP expression moderates the effect of feedback (positive or negative) on the locus of causality so that a high IP expression relates to external attributions of success and internal attributions of failure.

Ibrahim et al. (2021) reported a positive correlation between the IP and stable attribution in negative situations and a negative correlation between the IP and stable attribution in positive situations. Based on these findings, we hypothesize:

H4

The IP expression moderates the effect of feedback (positive or negative) on the stability attribution so that a high IP expression leads to instable success attribution and a stable failure attribution.

Due to theoretical intersections between the constructs fixed mindset and the IP like avoidance-orientation, anxiety-driven achievement motivation, and an increased error sensitivity (Elliott & Dweck, 1988; Ross & Krukowski, 2003), the authors hypothesize:

H5

The IP expression and the fixed mindset expression correlate positively.

Method

Participants

The German sample was generated using an online survey from May to June 2021. The total sample size of N = 170 contained n = 83 men (48.8%), n = 86 women (50.6%) and one non-binary person (0.6%). The German sample consisted mostly of students (n = 101 students; 59.4%) and employees (n = 44, 25.9%). The age ranged from 19 to 66 years (M = 29.54; Md = 24.00, SD = 11.54; see online Supplement materials for full details on sample characteristics). The dataset for this study can be found in the open science framework: https://osf.io/2kdup/?view_only=5f617462feaf4648af66dc616d2b4b94.

Instruments

Impostor Profile 30 (IPP30)

The German-language IPP30 (Ibrahim et al., 2021) comprises 30 items and assesses the impostor expression across six subscales (Competence Doubt, Working-Styles, Alienation, Other-Self Divergence, Ambition, and Need for Sympathy) and a total score. The instrument uses a visual analogous scale ranging from 1 (does not apply in any aspect) to 100 (applies completely). The reliability of the six subscales ranges from ω = 0.50 to 0.91, with the total score showing very good internal consistency (ω = 0.95; Ibrahim et al., 2021).

Growth Mindset Scale

The Growth Mindset Scale (Dweck, 2006) comprises three items and uses a six-point Likert scale from 1 (strongly disagree) to 6 (strongly agree). The scale captures the implicit theory of intelligence from a fixed mindset (entity theory) to a growth mindset (incremental theory). The scale’s internal consistency is excellent (α = 0.94 – 0.98; Dweck et al., 1995). This article used a German translation of the scale, back- and forth-translated twice for validation. The German version of the scale shows very good internal consistency (α = 0.89).

Revised causal dimension scale (CDSII)

The Revised Causal Dimension Scale (McAuley et al., 1992) is based on Weiner’s (2000) achievement motivation and attribution theory. This study used a professionally pre- and back-translated German version comprising 12 items measuring the four subscales locus of causality, personal control, stability, external control with a nine-point Likert scale. The scale ranges, e.g., for the locus of causality, from 1 (reflects an aspect of the situation) to 9 (reflects an aspect of myself). The internal consistencies of the scales are very good α = 0.87–89.

Procedure

The experiment was conducted online with the survey platform Questback (version EFS Summer 2021) from May to June 2021. To test the credibility and a sufficient level of difficulty of the bogus performance test, we conducted a pre-test phase (n = 10). As a result, all subjects rated the bogus construct as credible. In addition, pre-test participants rated the task difficulty of the performance tests as moderate (n = 8) to very difficult (n = 2). At the beginning of the experiment, the subjects were informed about the bogus construct called the inductive-holistic problem-solving competence, which was described in the instruction as a stable personality trait and as a prognostically valid indicator of future academic and career success. The construct was described as a stable personality trait and valid indicator of future success to increase candidates’ motivation and effort and, therefore, to prevent the external attribution of negative feedback on lacking effort. In addition, we intended to increase the meaning of the feedback. The IPP30 and the Growth Mindset Scale were assessed in the next step. Afterwards, the first part of the bogus experiment was conducted, which contained 24 items on different personality traits. The second part of the test contained parts of the Wilde Intelligence Test-2 (WIT-2; Kersting et al., 2008) with spatial, numerical, and verbal reasoning tasks. The performance tasks were carried out under time pressure. Subsequently, after completing the performance task, the participants were shown a short waiting screen for the illusion of calculating the results. After that, the subjects received a randomized positive or negative bogus performance feedback written as follows (translated from German):

Positive performance feedback.

“You scored above average to far above average on the performance test. In the area of inductive-holistic problem-solving skills, your score is better than 82%. In the area of adaptive regulation skills, your score is better than 89% of participants in previous studies. Inductive-holistic problem-solving competence: PR = 82, 95% CI [79.31, 89.10]. Adaptive regulatory ability: PR = 89, 95% CI [85.08, 93.70].”

Negative performance feedback.

“You scored below average to far below average on the achievement test. In the area of inductive-holistic problem-solving skills, your score is better than 16%. In the area of adaptive regulation skills, your score is better than 23% of participants in previous studies. Inductive-holistic problem-solving competence: PR = 16, 95% CI [13.51, 19.40]. Adaptive regulation ability: PR = 23, 95% CI [21.73, 27.85].”

Next, a manipulation check was carried out in which subjects had to indicate whether their inductive-holistic problem-solving competence was above average (1 = true; 2 = not true). Afterwards, the CDSII was assessed. In the last step, the subjects were informed about the deception and the true objective of the research. Finally, the subjects were told that the feedback was randomly generated and that they could withdraw their participation and have their data deleted.

Analytic technique

We used the software R (R Core Team, 2016) to test the moderation and correlation models. We computed the interaction terms using mean centering and examined the IP as the moderator at a high (+ 1SD, n = 37, M = 67.38), mean ( < + 1SD and >-1SD, n = 108, M = 43.93), and low (-1SD, n = 25, M = 37.40) level. The interaction effects of the feedback condition and impostor expression were examined for the subscales locus of causality and stability.

A power analysis was conducted for multiple linear regression models with two (for moderation analysis) predictors and revealed a power of 0.80 for small effects ( < 0.15) with a sample size of n = 159 participants. For a oneway bivariate correlation analysis with a significance level of α = 0.05, a minimum sample size of n = 153 was determined to find a small effect of r = .20 with a power of 0.80 (Cohen, 1988; Faul et al., 2007).

Results

The descriptive statistics and reliabilities for the assessed scales are reported in Table 1. The correlations of the study variables are briefed in Table 2. As expected, the IPP total score correlates negatively with the CDSII subscale personal control (r = − .34, p < .001; H1) and positively with external control (r = .30, p < .001; H2). The locus of causality scale (internality attribution) is not related to the IPP total score (r = − .93, p = .230). However, the separate examination of the subgroups regarding the feedback condition shows that the impostor score is negatively related to internal attribution in the case of positive feedback (r = − .45, p < .001) and positively related to internal attribution in the case of negative feedback (r = .29, p = .008). Also the stability scale is not related to the IP (r = − .07, p = .387). The separate analysis for positive and negative feedback shows no correlation between the IP and the stability attribution within the positive feedback condition (r = − .14, p = .200). In contrast, there is a correlation between the IP and the stability attribution within the negative feedback condition (r = .27, p = .012). The fixed mindset expression also correlates positively with the IPP total score (r = .28, p < .001; H5).

Table 1 Descriptive statistics of the Assessed Instruments
Table 2 Correlations of the IPP30 with the Study Variables

Consistent with H3, the IP significantly moderates the effect of feedback on the locus of causality (B = -0.308, SE = 0.061, t = -5.071, p < .001, 95% CI [-0.43, -0.19]). The IP expression affected the locus of causality at high (B = -4.799, SE = 1.425, t = -3.367, p < .001, 95% CI = [-7.61, -1.99]) and low (B = 5.445, SE = 1.421, t = 3.832, p <. 001, 95% CI = [2.64, 8.25]) levels only, but not at a moderate level (B = 0.323, SE = 1.003, t = 0.322, p = .748, 95% CI = [-1.66, 2.30]). The interaction term significantly explained additional variance to the overall model (ΔR2 = 0.133, p < .001; Table 3). As shown in Fig. 1, a high IP expression is positively associated with the internal attribution of failure and negatively associated with the internal attribution of success. A low IP level is negatively associated with the internal attribution of success and positively associated with the internal attribution of failure.

Table 3 The Moderational Effect of the IP on the Association of Feedback and Locus of Causality
Fig. 1
figure 1

Graphical depiction of the interaction between the IP and locus of causality; high locus of causality as internal attribution

Consistent with H4, the impostor expression significantly moderates the effect of feedback on the stability attribution (B = -0.171, SE = 0.061, t = -2.804, p = .006, 95% CI = [-0.29, -0.05]). Examination of the conditional effects indicated that the IP is only associated with the stability attribution at a low (B = 4.444, SE = 1.425, t = 3.118, p = .002, 95% CI = [1.63, 7.26]), but not a moderate (B = 1. 604, SE = 1.006, t = 1.595, p = .113, 95% CI = [-0.38, 3.59]) and a high (B = -1.237, SE = 1.429, t = -0.865, p = .388, 95% CI = [-4.06, 1.58]) level. The interaction term significantly explained additional variance to the overall model (ΔR2 = 0.044, p = .006; Table 4). As shown in Fig. 2, a low IP expression is negatively related to the stable attributions of failure and positively related to the stable attribution for success. A moderate IP expression shows no significant moderating effect. Also high IP shows no significant moderating effect, but a tendency towards a more stable attribution with negative feedback and less stable attribution with positive feedback (Fig. 2).

Fig. 2
figure 2

Graphical depiction of the interaction between the IP and stability; high stability as stable attribution

Table 4 The Moderational Effect of the IP on the Association of Feedback and Stability

Discussion

This article examined the IP’s role as a moderator between performance feedback and attribution and the IP’s relationship to the mindset. The moderation analyses show that the IP moderates the effect of performance feedback on the attribution dimensions causality and stability in the hypothesized way. A significant interaction effect of the IP and the feedback condition on locus of causality could be determined. According to this, impostors tend to internalize failure and externalize success. Therefore, it could be argued that the lower internalization of success is a main reason for the lower self-efficacy and increased external locus of control among Impostors despite success (Chae et al., 1995; Vergauwe et al., 2016). The interaction effect between the feedback and the IP expression concerning the stability dimension was also significant but less explicit than between the IP and locus of causality. Moderately and strong IP tendencies do not lead to an increased stability attribution of success. On the contrary, people with low IP show a self-esteem distortion in that they attribute success as more stable than failure. However, a trend can be observed that impostors tend to attribute failure as more stable and success less stable. This pessimistic stability perception regarding performance could partially drive the fear of failure and self-doubt despite success in impostors (Sakulku & Alexander, 2011).

These experimental results correspond with the psychometrical results regarding impostors’ attributional style (Ibrahim et al., 2021). Therefore, Impostors are characterized by an external-instable attribution of success and an internal-stable attribution of failure. This non-self-serving bias could be the main reason for impostors showing less self-esteem and more anxiety (Chrisman et al., 1995).

Due to the positive relationship between the fixed mindset and the IP, impostors seem to believe to some degree that intelligence is a fixed attribute (Kumar & Jagacinski, 2006). The connection with a fixed mindset also sheds new light on the impostor’s performance orientation. It suggests that impostors tend to have performance goal orientations, which means they prioritize displaying skills as more important than developing them through challenges (Yeager & Dweck, 2012). It further suggests that impostors, similar to people with a fixed mindset, exhibit an avoidance-based achievement motivation, which manifests in higher tendencies for impression-management as reported previously (Leary et al., 2000). Therefore, the belief that intelligence and abilities are unchangeable could be a major cause of impostors’ increased fear of failure. From the fixed mindset perspective, failure means that one is not intelligent enough and that this lack of intelligence is not changeable. Failure, therefore, always offers a risk of a negative and definite label. Therefore, this implicit belief would also explain why impostors avoid competitive situations (Ross & Krukowski, 2003), which carry the risk of revealing this lack of intelligence. The implicit belief in the immutability of intelligence and the existing self-doubt thus lead to helplessness (Morris & Tiggemann, 2013), as impostors fear that they are not intelligent and that others will find that out. The doubt about one’s abilities and the conviction that one cannot change these abilities explains the non-self-serving attributional bias. In this regard, successes cannot result from one’s abilities and cannot be a sign of one’s increasing ability because these are seen as fixed. The implicit theory of intelligence could be an essential mechanism for developing impostor feelings. It could also be an essential lever for reducing impostor feelings since the mindset can be changed through learning experiences (Plaks & Stecher, 2007), as indicated in the intervention study by Zanchetta et al. (2020). However, the relationship between mindset and IP needs to be further investigated in future research, as the relationship was only moderate in this study.

Limitations

First, the cross-sectional study design does not allow conclusions about causality. A longitudinal study would allow further insights into the direction of the predictor and the criterion variable, especially regarding the assumed influences of mindset on the IP in this study. Additionally, the survey was conducted online, which allows for less control of environmental factors. Furthermore, it should be noted that the initial presentation of the bogus construct as a stable personality trait may have increased the fixed mindset expression in candidates (Sarrasin et al., 2018). In addition, the internal attribution of the participants might be increased for positive and negative feedback, as the bogus construct has been described as a stable personality trait.

Second, the study used quantitative data. In future research, qualitative data could be used to investigate attributions and implicit theories of intelligence in more depth. In addition, the sample consisted exclusively of German participants and mainly students, so the generalisability is limited. In addition, the results on the non-self-serving attributional bias must be considered in the context of previous research results, as this study is the first to find this effect. The previous studies (e.g., Thompson et al., 2000) did not find the external-instable attributional bias in success, so the findings must be replicated in future studies.

Lastly, future research could include actual performance in achievement tasks as a control variable to account for intentionally reduced effort as a performance-avoidance strategy (Burnette et al., 2013) and gain insights into the influence of the IP and mindset on performance.

Conclusions

Despite the limitations, this experimental study demonstrates the non-self-serving bias in impostors for the first time (as far as the authors know). This bias is characterized by an internal locus of causality for failure, an external locus of causality for success, a reduced stability attribution for success, and an increased stability attribution for failure. Furthermore, the IP is positively related to the fixed mindset as the implicit entity theory of intelligence. Considering mindset as an influencing factor enables a practical interventional possibility to reduce fear of failure, the non-self-serving attributional bias, and the impostor feelings in general.