Psychopathy is characterised by a cluster of affective and interpersonal personality features such as superficial charm, egocentricity, callousness, dishonesty, lack of empathy as well as impulsive and deviant behaviours, including aggression, irresponsibility and in some cases antisocial behaviour and criminal offending (Hare, 1996). Callous-unemotional (CU) traits form the primary characteristics of psychopathy and signify a number of affective personality traits linked to lack of guilt and empathy, shallow emotions and insensitivity towards others (Fanti et al., 2017; Frick, 2004). Research indicates that CU traits develop in early childhood and they are considered to be the developmental precursor of psychopathic traits in adults (Kyranides et al., 2016; Waller et al., 2019). Given their negative developmental trajectory, children with these traits are at risk of developing more severe antisocial behaviour and emotional deficits (Fanti et al., 2017; Frick & White, 2008) and have been associated with considerable societal burden (Asscher et al., 2011). These traits have attracted much interest and as a result a specifier of limited prosocial emotions has been added to the diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders, fifth edition (DSM-5) for diagnosing conduct disorder (Fanti et al., 2018; Kimonis et al., 2015). Research has embraced the possible association between developmental factors such as attachment and psychopathic traits (Van Der Zouwen et al., 2018) with some studies showing a positive association between insecure attachment and the development of psychopathic traits in both adult and child populations (Alzeer et al., 2019; Kohlhoff et al., 2020). Despite this, there have been mixed findings in relation to the impact of gender differences on attachment contributing to CU traits and the influence of attachment figures. Therefore, this study examines the relationship between attachment, parental factors and CU traits in males and females as adults, to better understand the influence and their clinical importance and help inform CU traits prevention and/or management efforts.

Attachment and Psychopathic Traits

Bowlby (1973, 1980) put forward a theory about attachment by interpreting it as a biological mechanism that allows humans to develop emotional connections with significant others. Ainsworth (1989) expanded the theory; categorising warm and trustworthy bonds as secure attachment and alternatively those that lack the aforementioned, as avoidant and anxious attachments. More specifically, Brennan et al. (1998) suggested that individuals who score high on avoidance attachment dimension tend to rely more on themselves and do not view others as dependable in their relationships, especially with regard to satisfying and maintaining their needs, and are characterised by a reduced expression of affection and intimacy (Fraley & Shaver, 1997; Mikulincer & Shaver, 2007). On the other hand, individuals who score high on the anxiety attachment dimension have a negative view of self, they are characterised by insecurity concerning the responses of others, with a high fear of rejection, while overly seeking closeness in their relationships (Fraley & Shaver, 1997; Mikulincer & Shaver, 2007).

The relationship between a child and a parent has been connected with emotional, social and behavioural development (e.g., Van Der Voort et al., 2014). While secure attachment is associated with the development of trust, stability and control that enables one to form healthy relationships in life (Ainsworth & Witting, 1969; Simmons et al., 2009), insecure attachment has been associated with development of unhealthy traits and behaviours such as psychopathic traits (Alzeer et al., 2019; Van Der Zouwen et al., 2018). Several retrospective studies indicate that participants scoring high on psychopathic traits and anxiety attachment dimension, recall problematic childhood environments; involving lack of affection and warmth, parental neglect or separation and severe punishment strategies used by caregivers (Frodi et al., 2001; Waller et al., 2019). Literature also supports that psychopathic traits, especially the affective dimension, is associated with avoidance attachment in both males and females (Walsh et al., 2019). However, it is still unclear whether anxiety or avoidance attachment has the greatest association with CU traits. There has been a focus on studies using incarcerated populations, through the study of psychopathic scores (Bisby et al., 2017; Frodi et al., 2001) and an increase in studies conducted with non-clinical populations (e.g., Blanchard & Lyons, 2016; Mack et al., 2011), yet the results are far from conclusive. While there is support for both avoidance and anxiety attachment being associated with higher psychopathy scores in a non-clinical population (Mack et al., 2011), Van Der Zouwen et al. (2018) found no significant results in community samples compared to clinical samples. Given these mixed findings, this study aims to look more into community samples and examine the significance of this association.

Gender Differences, Attachment and Psychopathic Traits

Research shows that the prevalence of CU traits is significantly higher in men compared to women (Ciucci et al., 2014; Fanti et al., 2018). Attachment also presents differently in males and females with research suggesting that males exhibit higher levels of avoidant attachment while females display higher levels of anxiety attachment (Blanchard & Lyons, 2016; Schmitt & Jonason, 2015). Recently Blanchard and Lyons (2016) illustrated a link between avoidant attachment in males and controlling mothers, and between both anxiety and avoidant attachment in females and low-caring fathers. When it comes to the gender of parents, some findings convey that a rejecting father might be more influential, even when maternal warmth is present (Frodi et al., 2001) while low maternal warmth has been also associated with the development of CU traits (Bisby et al., 2017; Kimonis et al., 2013). Moreover, the literature shows that parental care in same-sex dyads (mother-daughter or father-son relationships) predicted affective empathy (Lyons et al., 2017), and higher quality in these relationships was a protective factor against the decline of mental health (Steele & Mckinney, 2019). According to a recent systematic review (Ruiz-Hernández et al., 2019), there have been several, behavioural differences observed within the same-sex dyads, which contributed to the development of externalising behaviours (e.g., aggressiveness, delinquency). More specifically it was reported that boys were rebelling more against maternal control than paternal control and girls with aggressive behaviour had less communication with their fathers and were more influenced by maternal control (Ruiz-Hernández et al., 2019). The literature does not extensively cover parental dyads, especially in relation to CU traits, as it has primarily focused on the mother-child relationship (Bisby et al., 2017; Kimonis et al., 2013) and their impact on developing psychopathic traits. Thus, comparing all general dyads (mother-son/daughter or father-son/daughter) is necessary to comprehend the extent to which gender impacts the developmental trajectory of psychopathic traits.

The Current Study

A more in-depth view is needed to further enhance the understanding of anxious and avoidant attachment dimensions and how they contribute to the development of psychopathic traits and more specifically CU traits. This is particularly important given their longitudinal association with conduct and emotional symptoms (Fanti et al., 2017; Kyranides et al., 2016), along with their recent connection to relationship violence in adulthood (Golmaryami et al., 2021). The reported findings from the literature are far from conclusive and investigations beyond clinical samples is vital. Insight into precursors of psychopathic traits could allow further understanding in the constellation of these personality traits. This in turn could help identify factors that could contribute to interventions, which may alleviate difficulties that individuals with these traits face and the potential harm they pose to those around them (Asscher et al., 2011). Despite the fact that there are several studies examining gender differences, there are to our knowledge few to none that investigate CU traits more in-depth by considering the gender of parents as well as parental factors and attachment dimensions.

The aim of this study is to explore how anxious and avoidant attachment dimensions, maternal and paternal factors predict CU traits in women and men separately. Considering the strong support from the literature it is hypothesised that: 1) CU traits will manifest differently across genders with males reporting higher levels of CU traits compared to females (Ciucci et al., 2014; Fanti et al., 2018). 2) Avoidance and anxiety attachment dimensions are expected to be significantly associated with CU traits, however due to mixed findings (Blanchard & Lyons, 2016; Mack et al., 2011; Walsh et al., 2019), we do not hypothesise the direction of these associations or their relationships with genders. 3) Among parental factors, there is strong support that mothers are more influential figures so it is expected that maternal factors will contribute more than paternal factors in relation to CU traits in both genders (Bisby et al., 2017; Rothbaum & Weisz, 1994).

Methods

Participants

The research study was prospectively reviewed and approved by the Ethics Committee of the University of Edinburgh. The sample consisted of 1170 adults ranging in age from 18 to 72 years (M = 30.96, SD = 11.66). One of the main aims of the study was to explore gender differences and the study variable CU traits, which led to the exclusion of 21 participants who did not identify with either gender. The final sample yielded 1149 participants 752 identified as females (65%) and 397 identified as males (35%). Of the final sample 16.1% of participants had completed a high school diploma, 7.3% had an associate’s degree or equivalent, 43.9% had a Bachelors, 29.5% had obtained a Master’s degree and 3.1% had completed a Doctorate. With regard to employment, 6.3% of participants were unemployed, 38.1% were students, 13% were working part-time, 40.7% were working full-time while 1.9% were retired.

Measures

Inventory of Callous-Unemotional Traits

The Inventory of Callous-Unemotional traits (ICU; Frick, 2004) is a 24-item self-report scale designed to assess callous, unemotional and carelessness traits (Frick, 2004). The ICU has been derived from the callous-unemotional scale of the Antisocial Process Screening Device (APSD) and captures the affective component of psychopathy (Ray & Frick, 2020). Each item is scored on a four-point scale ranging from 0 (not at all true) to 3 (definitely true). Scores are calculated by reverse scoring the positively worded items and then summing them together to get a total score (Frick, 2004). The inventory has been used widely (Ray & Frick, 2020), with community samples (Kyranides et al., 2016), and samples of juvenile offenders (Kimonis et al., 2013). In the current study the total ICU showed good internal consistency (α = .80) similar to scores reported in other studies (e.g., Kyranides et al., 2017; Ray & Frick, 2020).

Relationship Scales Questionnaire

The Relationship Scales Questionnaire (RSQ; Griffin & Bartholomew, 1994) is a self-report measure with 30 items assessing a) avoidance (α = .78; e.g., “I find it difficult to trust others”); and b) anxiety (α = .81; e.g., “I worry about being abandoned”) in close relationships (Kurdek, 2002). The items of the questionnaire include questions that explore close relationships with friends and family, as well as romantic relationships. The answer to each item is scored based on Likert-type scale from 1 (not at all) to 5 (very much). The RSQ has been assessed for its validity, efficiency and reliability and has been used to assess adult attachment (Alzeer et al., 2019; Blanchard & Lyons, 2016; Henschel et al., 2020).

Parent Adult-Child Relationship Questionnaire

The Parent Adult-Child Relationship Questionnaire (PACQ; Peisah et al., 1999) is a self-report measure comprising of 26 items examining relationships with the mother and the father reflecting the following dimensions: (1) Regard (filial gratitude or reciprocity and perceived closeness); (2) Responsibility (feeling responsible for the parent) and (3) Control (parental power). Participants are asked to indicate the extent to which each statement describes their relationship with their mother and father using a 4-point scale from 0 (not true at all) to 3 (very true). Half of the items access the relationship with the mother and the PACQ includes two factors: regard (α = .77 in the current study; e.g., ‘I respect my mother’s opinion’) and responsibility (α = .78; e.g. ‘I feel responsible for my mother’s happiness’). The other half of the items access the relationship with the father and include three factors: regard (α = .82; ‘I respect my father’s opinion’), responsibility (α = .63; e.g., ‘Something will happen to my father if I don’t take care of him’) and control (α = .76; e.g., ‘My father tries to dominate me’). Previous research has reported similar internal reliability for the PACQ (e.g., Alzeer et al., 2019; Peisah et al., 1999).

Procedure

The battery of questionnaires was processed via a secure online platform. The link to the survey was administered through various social media networks and sites, which provided the participants with a free and easier access to the questionnaires from their private devices (smartphones, computers, iPads). All individuals had to read the information about the study and provided informed consent. Participants provided some demographic information (age, gender), followed by the three questionnaires accessing callous unemotional traits, attachment and parental relationships, which were administered in the same order for all participants. The survey took about 15–20 min to complete and participation was voluntary. At the end of the study, participants were debriefed and thanked for their time.

Plan of Analysis

Analyses were conducted using IBM SPSS 25. Preliminary analyses included checking for missing data, outliers and normality of distribution. Demographic characteristics of participants were explored followed by independent t-tests to examine gender differences on CU traits, attachment dimensions, maternal and paternal relationships. Correlation analysis was used to explore the associations of mother-son, mother-daughter, father-son and father-daughter relationships with CU traits. Hierarchical multiple regressions were then run separately for male and female participants entering variables in the same order. Age was entered first (step 1), while attachment variables (avoidance and anxiety) were entered second (step 2) followed by parental factors (regard, responsibility and control) which were entered separately for mother (step 3) and father (step 4) with CU traits as the outcome variable. The overlap in variance between predictors and the unique variance by each predictor was assessed.

Results

With regard to gender differences, there were significant differences in CU traits t(1147) = 9.32, p < .001, with males reporting higher levels than females (Table 1). Additionally significant differences were found in the levels of anxiety related to attachment reported t(1147) = 3.58, p < .005 with females reporting higher levels than men. Both mother t(1139) = 2.07, p < .05 and father t(1123) = 2.90, p < .01 responsibility and father regard t(1123) = 1.97, p < .05 variables, were significantly higher in men compared to women in all cases. Due to the significant differences in CU traits found in men and women further analysis were conducted separately.

Table 1 Means, Standard Deviation (SD), for men and women for CU traits (ICU), Attachment types (RSQ) and parental relationships (PACQ)

Although correlations were generally similar in pattern across gender there were some differences in these associations. For both males and females (see Table 2), CU traits were significantly positively correlated with avoidance and anxiety. For males (Table 2 below the diagonal) CU traits were negatively correlated with age and both mother and father regard but were positively correlated to paternal control. For females (Table 2 above the diagonal) age and the parental variables were not significantly correlated with CU traits.

Table 2 Correlations on the main study variables

In order to examine which variable/s have the largest impact on CU traits, hierarchical regression analysis were conducted to assess the unique contribution of the predictors, separately for males and females. Tests to determine linearity, homoscedasticity and multicollinearity were performed and no violation of the assumptions (r < 0.7, tolerance >0.1; VIF < 10) was revealed. Age was entered in step 1 and accounted for a significant 2% of variance in CU traits for men, F(1, 384) = 6.43, p < .05 (Table 3) and 0.3% of variance of CU traits in women but the model for women was not significant (p > .05) (Table 4). Attachment dimensions (avoidance and anxiety) were entered is step 2 with the models accounting for 18% of variance in CU for males F(3, 384) = 28.67, p < .001 and 18% for women F(3,720) = 51.66, p < .001. From this overall variance, attachment dimensions explained 16% for men and 18% for women and this additional variance was significant for both men F Change(2, 381) = 39.15, p < .001 and women F Change(2, 717) = 76.16, p < .001. In step 3, the mother related variables were entered (regard and responsibility) and the models explained 21% of the variance in CU traits in men F(5, 384) = 19.63, p < .001 but remained at 18% in women. From this variance, the mother variables accounted for 3% of the variance of CU traits in male participants which was significant, F Change(2, 379) = 5.14, p < .01, but the same addition of mother related variables contributed to no additional variance explained in female participants (p > .05). Finally, the addition of the father variables (regard, responsibility and control) in step 4 explained 22% of the variance of CU traits in men F (8, 384) = 12.63, p < .001 and reached 19% in women, F (8, 720) = 20.01, p < .001. From this variance, the addition of the father variables accounted for 1% of the variance of CU traits for both men and women but this addition was not significant for either men or women (ps > .05). Following from the above, attachment explained the largest variance of CU traits in both men and women. The results of the hierarchical regression analysis were conducted and presented separately for men (Table 3) and women (Table 4).

Table 3 Hierarchical Regression Analysis for variables predicting Callous Unemotional Traits for male participants
Table 4 Hierarchical Regression Analysis for variables predicting Callous Unemotional Traits for female participants

In the final models, CU traits in men (Table 3) were significantly positively predicted by attachment avoidance (β = .40, p < .001) and negatively predicted by age (β = −.11, p < .01), attachment anxiety (β = −.11, p < .05) and mother regard (β = −.15, p < .01). For women (Table 4) CU traits were significantly positively predicted by attachment avoidance (β = .45, p < .001) and negatively predicted by age (β = −.08, p < .05) and attachment anxiety (β = −.12, p < .001). Interestingly in the final model the variable assessing feelings of responsibility for the father was also a positive predictor (β = .09, p < .05) for CU traits in women, but none of the mother variables arose as significant predictors (ps > .05).

Discussion

The current study aimed to examine the previously unexplored predictive associations of attachment dimensions (avoidance and anxiety) and parental factors of regard, responsibility and control with CU traits separately in males and females. Our findings indicated age and attachment anxiety as significant negative predictors of CU traits in both males and females, while attachment avoidance appeared as a significant positive predictor of CU traits in both genders. Surprisingly, among the parental factors, high regard for mothers negatively predicted CU traits only in males (Lyons et al., 2017), while the factor assessing responsibility towards the father positively predicted CU traits only in females. Overall, CU traits manifested differently across genders with males showing significantly higher levels of CU traits than females (Fanti et al., 2018), possibly due to stronger genetic influence associated with the presentation of these traits (Fontaine et al., 2010).

Theoretical and Practical Implications

This study contributes to the attachment and psychopathy literature by showing that age was an influential predictor for CU traits in both males and females indicating that these traits may decrease with age (Fanti et al., 2017). Considering that the current study focused on the affective dimension (CU traits), our findings are in alignment with previous research (Asscher et al., 2011), suggesting that it is important to target these traits in early years when attachment and CU traits are more malleable (Fraley & Roisman, 2019; Kohlhoff et al., 2020). Intervening early is important as individuals with high and stable conduct disorder symptoms and callous–unemotional traits are consistently at higher risk for individual, behavioral and contextual problems in adolescence (Eisenbarth et al., 2016; Fanti et al., 2018) as well as relationship issues in adulthood, reporting more incidences of dominance, violence and lower relationship satisfaction (Golmaryami et al., 2021).

Attachment dimensions explained the largest variance in CU traits in both genders, demonstrating that attachment is a determining factor even in adulthood (Alzeer et al., 2019; Blanchard & Lyons, 2016). More specifically, attachment avoidance appeared to have predictive power for CU traits in both genders. This finding is in-line with previous research (e.g., Blanchard & Lyons, 2016; Walsh et al., 2019) and is supported by the attachment framework (Brennan et al., 1998) considering that attachment avoidance involves fear of depending on others within interpersonal relationships, with an excessive need for self-reliance. Thus, individuals with high levels of avoidance attachment, tend to evade intimate and close relationships, use deactivating strategies that minimize the experience of rejection to protect against threats of self-image (Fraley & Shaver, 1997; Mikulincer & Shaver, 2007), which limits their access and awareness of recognizing emotions in others making it difficult for them to empathize, key characteristics of CU traits (i.e., Henschel et al., 2020; Mack et al., 2011; Mikulincer & Shaver, 2007; Simpson et al., 2011; Van Der Zouwen et al., 2018). Taking the aforementioned findings into account, in combination with an established link between empathetic and moral disturbances with the affective-interpersonal features of psychopathy in both genders (Seara-Cardoso et al., 2012), it seems that attachment avoidance should be prioritized when designing interventions for individuals with CU traits within clinical settings (Daly & Mallinckrodt, 2009) in managing CU traits but also in mainstream settings (schools, parenting programs) focusing on prevention (Kyranides et al., 2018; Rose et al., 2019).

Furthermore, we found attachment anxiety to negatively predict CU traits in both males and females, acting as a protective factor. One possible explanation for this finding could be that attachment anxiety involves a fear of rejection or abandonment within interpersonal relationships, which is accompanied with an excessive need for approval from others (Brennan et al., 1998; Fraley & Shaver, 1997). Therefore, individuals with high attachment anxiety are more likely to care/want and seek close intimate relationships and have been found to be more empathetically accurate (Simpson et al., 2011) so they are less likely to exhibit high CU traits. Furthermore studies have demonstrated that individuals with psychopathic traits are characterized as fearless (Kyranides et al., 2016; Waller et al., 2019), reporting low levels of anxiety and arousal. Together, these findings suggest that different attachment dimensions may independently impact CU characteristics in adults (Van der Zouwen et al., 2018), specifically highlighting that individuals with high attachment avoidance and low attachment anxiety are at higher risk to develop CU traits. Therefore it is necessary for practitioners to identify these high-risk individuals early, to reduce the possibility of them developing these dysfunctional attachments and personality traits. The findings of the present study have specific implications for clinical interventions targeting the development of CU traits, suggesting that the therapeutic goals should be focused on restoring attachment security through the identification of specific attachment dimensions (Daly & Mallinckrodt, 2009; Wright & Edginton, 2016) reducing avoidant patterns and increasing secure attachment behaviors.

For parental factors, our hypothesis was partially supported as only mother regard appeared to be a protective factor against CU traits in males, supporting findings that increased maternal warmth and more specifically having a positive relationship characterized by respect with the maternal figure acts as a protective factor against developing CU traits in males (Bisby et al., 2017). Maternal care has also been associated with increased empathy in men (Lyons et al., 2017). Surprisingly, the factor accessing feelings of responsibility for the father appeared to be a risk factor for CU traits in females only. This interesting finding perhaps resonates with the fact that increased responsibility towards fathers may lead to dissatisfaction within a father-daughter relationship, which leads to the development of frustration and irritation in females and the development of CU traits (Cicerelli, 1983; Peisah et al., 1999). The feelings of frustration arising from the sense of responsibility could be exacerbated if daughters avoid communicating effectively with their fathers, during undesirable life-events (Punyanunt-Carter, 2007). If this pattern of dysfunctional behaviors is repeated over the years, this would explain the development of these personality traits. Together these findings are in agreement with previous research suggesting that the importance of family is perceived differently across genders (Rothbaum & Weisz, 1994). Thus this should also be reflected in intervention efforts focused on rectifying relationships with attachment figures to prevent/manage callous unemotional traits (Wright & Edginton, 2016). The literature also states that same sex parents tend to have more influence on their children’s behavior, resulting from mimicking the actions of same sex parents (Ruiz-Hernández et al., 2019). However, our findings contradict these studies and that of Steele and Mckinney (2019), which illustrated the continuing impact of same gender interactions (i.e., mother-daughter dyad) on emerging adults’ mental health. It might be that cultural differences explain the differences in findings as Steele and Mckinney’s (2019) sample comprised of predominantly Caucasian college students. Unfortunately the current sample’s ethnic background information was not collected, but our sample was more diverse with regard to age and included older individuals. This highlights the need for additional research on parental factors and CU traits in different ethnic and age groups.

Finally, another theoretical contribution of our study is that attachment dimensions accounted for the largest variance in CU traits compared to parental factors and this was the case for both genders. These findings are not surprising and can be explained by the fact that adult attachments can be shaped by more recent interpersonal relationships (e.g., partners) than parental relationships which seem to become less influential as the person develops other relationships (Fraley & Roisman, 2019). While primary attachment experiences remain important, secondary attachment relationships such as friends and romantic partners become more influential beyond childhood (Imran et al., 2020). In line with this stream of research our findings reveal that an individual’s attachment is not static but dynamic and remains an influential predictor for psychopathic traits, which provides meaningful insights regarding the need to examine CU traits in the context of multiple relationships during adulthood.

Limitations

The present study has a number of limitations that should be addressed. First, the current study failed to include other variables (e.g., separation, loss of an important relationship, adverse childhood experiences) that would help decipher the risk and protective factors related to psychopathic traits. Future studies should also take into account culture backgrounds as this would help interpret the findings. Secondly, the sample was a community based low-risk sample and research on high-risk individuals is needed to corroborate the same pattern of findings in individuals with higher levels of psychopathic traits, as CU traits have stronger associations with parental factors in high-risk samples (Bisby et al., 2017; Waller et al., 2019). Thirdly, the measures used in the current study were self-reports and future research should try to replicate the findings using clinical interviews, multiple informants (parental figure, and/or partner) and possibly collect data regarding attachment and CU traits over time (following a longitudinal design).

Conclusions

The present study aimed to determine the impact of attachment dimensions and parental factors on callous unemotional traits across genders in adults. Our findings highlight the unique contribution of attachment dimensions to callous unemotional traits, specifically that high attachment avoidance can be viewed as a risk factor, while low attachment anxiety can be considered a protective factor for callous unemotional traits in both genders, beyond childhood. Parental factors were less influential than attachment dimensions on callous unemotional traits and they were perceived differently across genders. Having a positive respectful relationship with the mother is a protective factor for males while increased perceived responsibility for the paternal figure appears as a risk factor for callous unemotional traits in females, highlighting the unique role of maternal and paternal factors.